Journal of Neurochemistry, 1977. Vol. 29, pp. 13-26. Pergamon Press. Printed in Great Britain. SHORT REVIEW RELATION BETWEEN PHYSIOLOGICAL FUNCTION AND ENERGY METABOLISM IN THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM Louts SOKOLOFF Laboratory of Cerebral Metabolism, U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Bethesda, MD 20014, U.S.A. THE BRAIN is a complex organ composed of many structural and functional components with markedly different and independently regulated levels of func- tional and metabolic activity. In more homogeneous organs that do readily recognizable physical and chemical work, such as the heart, skeletal muscle, and kidney, a close relationship between functional ac- tivity and energy metabolism is well established. The existence of a similar relationship in the tissues of the CNS has been more difficult to prove, partly because of uncertainty about the nature of the work associated with nervous functional activity, but mainly because of the difficulty in assessing the levels , of functional and metabolic activities in the same functional component of the brain at the same time. Pathological and pharmacological conditions with gross and diffuse effects on cerebral functional ac- tivity, particularly those that alter the level of con- sciousness, have been shown to be associated with changes in overall cerebral metabolic rate (KETY, 1950, 1957; LASSEN, 1959; SOKOLOFF, 1976), but such associations could reflect separate independent conse- quences of cellular dysfunction rather than a direct relationship between cerebral functional activity and energy metabolism. Changes in the metabolic rate of the brain as a whole have generally not been found during physiological alterations of cerebral functional activity (LASSEN, 1959; SoOKOLOFF, 1969, 1976). What has clearly been needed is a method that measures the rates of energy metabolism in specific discrete regions of the brain in normal and altered states of functional activity. The recently developed [!*C]deoxyglucose technique (SoKOLOFF et al., 1977) appears to fulfill this need. It can be used to measure quantitatively the local rates of glucose utilization simultaneously in all the macroscopically visible structures of the brain. It can be applied to normal conscious animals as well as those under experimen- tally altered states of cerebral activity. Furthermore, Abbreviations used: DG, 2-deoxy-p-glucose; DG-6-P, 2-deox y-D-glucose-6-phosphate. 13 the ['*C]deoxyglucose method employs an auto- radiographic technique which provides pictorial representations of the relative rates of glucose utiliza- tion throughout the various components of the brain; even without quantification these autoradiographs provide clearly visible markers that map cerebral regions with increased or decreased rates of energy metabolsim in altered physiological, pharmacological, and pathological states. This method has provided unequivocal evidence of a close relationship between functional activity and energy metabolism in discrete structural and/or functional units of the nervous sys- tem. METHODOLOGY A full, detailed, and comprehensive description of the theoretical basis of the ['*C]deoxyglucose method has recently been published (ScKOLOFF et al., 1977). A brief summary of its essential principles is necessary, however, to clarify the salient features of its design, its rigid procedural requirements, and the limitations on its applications. The method was designed to take advantage of the extraordinary spatial resolution afforded by a quanti- tative autoradiographic technique that was originally developed for the measurement of local cerebral blood flow (LANDAU et al., 1955; REIvICH et al., 1969). The dependence on autoradiography prescribed the use of radioactive substrates for energy metabolism, the labeled products of which could be assayed in the tissues by the autoradiographic technique. Although oxygen consumption is the most direct measure of energy metabolism, the volatility of oxygen and the short physical half-life of its radioac- tive isotopes precluded measurement of oxidative metabolism by the autoradiographic technique. In most circumstances glucose is almost the sole sub- strate for cerebral oxidative metabolism, and its utili- zation is stoichiometrically related to oxygen con- sumption (Kety, 1957; SoKoLorr, 1976). Radidactive glucose is, however, not fully satisfactory because its labeled products have too short a biological half-life and are lost too rapidly from the cerebral tissues. The 14 Louis SOKOLOFF labeled analogue of glucose, 2-deoxy-b-['*C] glucose, was, therefore, selected because it has some special biochemical properties that make it particularly appropriate to trace glucose metabolism and to measure the local rates of cerebral glucose utilization by the autoradiographic technique. Theory The method is derived from a model based on the biochemical properties of 2-deoxyglucose (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). 2-Deoxyglucose is transported bi-direc- tionally between blood and brain by the same carrier that transports glucose across the blood-brain barrier (BIDDER, 1968; BACHELARD, 1971; OLDENDORF, 1971). In the cerebral tissues it is phosphorylated by hexo- kinase (EC 2.7.1.1) to 2-deox yglucose-6-phosphate (SoLs & CRANE, 1954). Deoxyglucose and glucose are, therefore, competitive substrates for both blood-brain transport and hexokinase-catalyzed phosphorylation. during the interval of time equals the steady state flux of glucose through the hexokinase-catalyzed step times the duration of the interval, and the net rate of flux of glucose through this step equals the rate of glucose utilization. These relationships can be mathematically defined and an operational equation derived if the following assumptions are made: (1) a steady state for glucose (ie. constant plasma glucose concentration and con- stant rate of glucose consumption) throughout the period of the procedure; (2) homogencous tissue com- partment within which the concentrations of [!4C]DG and glucose are uniform and exchange di- rectly with the plasma; and (3) tracer concentrations of [!4C]DG (ie. molecular concentrations of free [!4C]DG essentially equal to zero). The operational . equation which defines R;, the rate of glucose con- sumption per unit mass of tissues, i, in terms of measurable variables is as follows: T CHT) — kee 8187 | Che" de Oo R= ®V,Ki ve 7 ' (LPEEs | I crrcerae— ero (CHIC at | 0 0 Unlike glucose-6-phosphate, however, which is meta- bolized further eventually to CO, and water and to a lesser degree via the hexosemonophosphate shunt, deoxyglucose-6-phosphate cannot be converted to fruc- tose-6-phosphate and is not a substrate for glucose- 6-phosphate dehydrogenase (SOLS & CRANE, 1954). There is very little glucose-6-phosphatase activity in brain (Hers, 1957) and even less deoxyglu- cose-6-phosphatase activity (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). Deox yglucose-6-phosphate, once formed, is, therefore, essentially trapped in the cerebral tissues, at least long enough for the duration of the measurement. The half-lives of ['*C]deoxyglucose-6-phosphate in the various cerebral tissues have been experimentally esti- mated: the average half-lives are 7,7 (s.D. = +1.6) and 9.7 (.D. = +2.6) h in gray and white matter, respect- ively (SOKOLOFF ef al., 1977). The shortest half-life is 6.th in the inferior colliculus (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). If the interval of time is kept short enough, for example, less than 1h, to allow the assumption of negligible loss of ('“C]DG-6-P from the tissues, then the quantity of ['4C]DG-6-P accumulated in any cer- ebral tissue at any given time following the introduc- tion of [!*C]DG into the circulation is equal to the integral of the rate of ['*C]DG- phosphorylation by hexokinase in that tissue during that interval of time. This integral is in turn related to the amount of glu- cose that has been phosphorylated over the same in- terval, depending on the time courses of the relative concentrations of [!“C]DG and glucose in the pre- cursor pools and the Michaelis-Menten kinetic con- stants for hexokinase with respect to both ['*C]DG and glucose. With cerebral glucose consumption in a steady state, the amount of glucose phosphorylated (1) where C* (T) equals the combined concentrations of [!4C]DG and ['*C}DG-6-P in the tissue, i, at time, T, determined by quantitative autoradiography; C and Cp equal the arterial plasma concentrations of [)4C]DG and glucose, respectively; k%, k&%, and k¥ are the rate constants for the transport from the plasma to the tissue precursor pool, for the transport back from tissue to plasma, and for the phosphoryla- tion of free ['*CJDG in the tissue, respectively; A equals the ratio of the distribution volume of [!4C]DG in the tissue to that of glucose; @ equals the fraction of glucose which once phosphorylated continues down the glycolytic pathway; and K%, and V*,., and K,, and Via, are the familiar Michaelis— Menten kinetic constants of hexokinase for ['*C]DG and glucose, respectively. The rate constants are determined in a separate group of animals by a non-linear, iterative process which provides the least squares best-fit of an equa- tion which defines the time course of total tissue 4c concentration in terms of the time, the history of the plasma concentration, and the rate constants to the experimentally determined time courses of tissue and plasma concentrations of '*C (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). The rate constants have thus far been determined only in normal conscious albino rats; the values obtained for kt, k%, and k$ and their standard errors are 0.189 + 0.012, 0.245 + 0.040, and 0.052 +.0.010 per min in gray matter and 0.079 + 0.008, 0.133 + 0.046, and 0.020 + 0.020 per min in white matter, respectively (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). Prelimi- nary estimates indicate that the values are quite simi- lar in the conscious monkey (Kennedy, Sakurada, Shinohara, & Sokoloff, unpublished observations). Function and energy metabolism in CNS 15 The 4, @, and the enzyme kinetic constants are grouped together to constitute a single, lumped con- stant (see equation). It can be shown mathematically that this lumped constant is equal to the asymptotic value of the product of the ratio of the cerebral extraction ratios of ['*C]DG and glucose and the ratio of the arterial blood to plasma specific activities when the arterial plasma ['*C]DG concentrations is maintained constant. The. lumped constant is also determined in a separate group of animals from arter- ial and cerebral venous blood samples drawn during a programmed intravenous infusion which produces and maintains a constant arterial plasma ['*C]DG concentration (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). Thus far it has been determined only in the albino rat and the mon- key. Values are 0.483 (SEM. = +0.022) in the rat (SoKoLorF et al., 1977) and 0.344 (8.6.M. = +0.036) in the monkey (Kennedy, Sakurada, Shinohara & Sokoloff, unpublished observations). The lumped con- stant appears to be characteristic of the species and does not appear to change significantly in a wide range of conditions (SOKOLOFF et al., 1977). Despite its complex appearance, equation (1) is really nothing more than a general statement of the standard relationship by which rates of enzyme-cata- lyzed reactions are determined from measurements made with radioactive tracers. The. numerator of the equation represents the amount of radioactive product formed in a given interval of time; it is equal to C#, the combined concentrations of ['*C]DG and ['+C]DG-6-P in the tissue at time, 7, measured by the quantitative autoradiographic technique, less a term that represents the free unmetabolized ()*C]DG still remaining in the tissue. The denominator rep- resents the integrated specific activity of the precursor pool times a factor, the lumped constant, which is equivalent to a correction factor for an isotope effect. The term with the exponential factor in the denomi- nator takes into account the lag in the equilibration of the tissue precursor pool with the plasma. Procedure Because local rates of cerebral glucose utilization are calculated by means of equation (1), this equation dictates the variables to be measured. The specific procedure employed is designed to evaluate these variables and to minimize potential errors that might occur in the actual application of the method. If the rate constants, k%, k%, and k%, are precisely known, then equation (1) is generally applicable with any mode of administration of ['*C]DG and for a wide range of time intervals. At the present time the rate constants have been determined only in the conscious rat. These rate constants can be expected to vary with the condition of the animal, however, and for most accurate results should be re-determined for each con- dition studied. The structure of equation (1) suggests a more practicable alternative. All the terms in the equation. that contain the rate constants approach zero with increasing time if the ['*C]DG is so admin- istered that the plasma ['*C]DG concentration also approaches zero. From the values of the rate con- stants determined in normal animals and the usual time course of the clearance of ['*C]DG from the arterial plasma following a single intravenous pulse at zero time, an interval of 30-45 min is adequate for these terms to become sufficiently small that con- siderable latitude in inaccuracies of the rate constants is permissible without appreciably increased error in the estimates of local glucose consumption (SOKOLOFF et al, 1977). An additional advantage derived from the use of a single pulse of [’*C]DG followed by a relatively long interval before killing the animal for measurement of local tissue '*C concentration is that by then most of the free ['*C]DG in the tissues has been either converted to {'*C]DG-6-P or transported back to the plasma; the optical densities in the auto- radiographs then represent mainly the concentrations of ['4C]DG-6-P and, therefore, reflect directly the relative rates of glucose utilization in the various cere- bral tissues. The experimental procedure is to inject a pulse of ['*C]DG intravenously at zero time and to decapi- tate the animal at a measured time, T, 30-45 min later; in the interval timed arterial samples are taken for the measurement of plasma ['*C]DG and glucose concentrations. Tissue '*C concentrations, C¥, are measured at time, 7; by the quantitative autoradio- graphic technique. Local cerebral glucose utilization is calculated by equation (1) (SOKOLOFF ef al., 1977). RATES OF LOCAL CEREBRAL GLUCOSE UTILIZATION IN THE NORMAL CONSCIOUS RAT Thus far quantitative measurements of local cere- bral glucose utilization have been completed only in the albino rat. These values are presented in Table 1. The rates of local cerebral glucose utilization in the normal conscious rat vary widely throughout the brain. The values in white structures tend to group together and are always considerably below those of gray structures. The average value in gray matter is approx 3 times that of white matter, but the indivi- dual values vary from approx 50 to 200 pmol of glu- cose/100 g/min. The highest values are in the struc- tures involved in auditory functions with the inferior colliculus clearly the most metabolically active struc- ture in the brain. Quantitative determinations of local cerebral glu- cose utilization in the normal conscious monkey are currently being carried out in this laboratory, and the values should be available soon. The results thus far indicate similar heterogeneity in the monkey brain, but the values are considerably lower, approxi- mately one-third to one-half those in the rat, probably because of the lower cellular packing density and greater amounts of white matter (Kennedy, Sakurada, Shinohara & Sokoloff, unpublished observations). 16 Louis SOKOLOFF EFFECTS OF GENERAL ANESTHESIA In the albino rat thiopental anesthesia reduces the rates of glucose utilization in all structures of the brain (Table 1). The effects are not uniform; the per- cent effects in white matter are relatively small com- pared to those in most gray structures. Anesthesia also markedly reduces the heterogeneity normally present within gray matter, an effect clearly visible in the autoradiographs (SOKOLOFF et al.,.1977). These results are in agreement with those of previous studies in which anesthesia has been found to decrease the cerebral metabolic rate of the brain as a whole (KETy, 1950; Lassen, 1959; SOKOLOFF, 1976). Preliminary studies indicate that thiopental anes- thesia has effects in the rhesus monkey like those in the rat (SHAPIRO et al., 1975). The effects of halothane anesthesia in the monkey are similar, except that it appears to leave the basal ganglia unaffected (SHAPIRO et al., 1975). In contrast, phencyclidine, which is often used as an anesthetic agent but is probably a convul- sant, causes 10-50% increases in glucose consumption in all gray structures, except the inferior colliculus, pontine nuclei, and cerebellar cortex where significant decreases are observed (SHAPIRO et al., 1975). RELATION BETWEEN LOCAL FUNCTIONAL ACTIVITY AND ENERGY METABOLISM The results of a variety of applications of the method demonstrate a clear relationship between local cerebral functional activity and glucose con- sumption. The most striking demonstrations of the close coupling between function and energy metabo- lism are seen with experimentally induced local alter- ations in functional activity that are restricted to a few specific areas in the brain. The effects on local glucose consumption are then so pronounced that they are not only observed in the quantitative results but can be visualized directly on the autoradiographs which are really pictorial representations of the rela- tive rates of glucose utilization in the various struc- . tural components of the brain. Effects of increased functional activity Effects of sciatic nerve stimulation. Electrical stimu- lation of one sciatic nerve in the rat under barbiturate anesthesia causes pronounced increases in glucose consumption (i.e. increased optical density in the autoradiographs) in the ipsilateral dorsal horn of the lumbar spinal cord (KENNEDY ef al., 1975). Effects of olfactory stimulation. The ['*C]deoxyglu- cose method has been used to map the olfactory sys- tem of the rat (SHARP et al., 1975). Olfactory stimu- lation with amyl acetate has been found to produce increased labeling in localized regions of the olfactory bulb. Preliminary results obtained with other odors, such as camphor and cheese, suggest different spatial patterns of increased metabolic activity with different odors. Effects of experimental focal seizures. The local in- jection of penicillin into the hand-face area of the motor cortex of the rhesus monkey has been shown to induce electrical discharges in the adjacent cortex and to result in recurrent focal seizures involving the face, arm, and hand on the contralateral side (Cave- NESS, 1969). Such seizure activity causes selective in- creases in glucose consumption in areas of motor cor- tex adjacent to the penicillin locus and in small dis- crete regions of the putamen, globus pallidus, caudate nucleus, thalamus, and substantia nigra of the same side (Fig. 1) (KENNEDY et al., 1975). Similar studies in the rat have led to comparable results and pro- vided evidence on the basis of an evoked metabolic response of a ‘mirror’ focus in the motor cortex con- tralateral to the penicillin-induced epileptogenic focus (COLLINS et al., 1976). Effects of decreased functional activity Decrements in functional activity result in reduced rates of glucose utilization. These effects are particu- larly striking in the auditory and visual systems of the rat and the visual system of the monkey. Effects of auditory deprivation. In the albino rat some of the highest rates of local cerebral glucose utilization are found in components of the auditory system, i.e. auditory cortex, medial geniculate gang- lion, inferior colliculus, lateral lemniscus, superior olive, and cochlear nucleus (Table 1). The high meta- bolic activities of some of these structures are clearly visible in the autoradiographs (Fig. 2). Bilateral audi- tory deprivation by occlusion of both external audi- tory canals with wax markedly depresses the meta- bolic activity in all of these areas (Fig. 2) (Des Rosiers, Kennedy & Sokoloff, unpublished observations). The reductions are symmetrical bilaterally and range from 35 to 60% Unilateral auditory deprivation also depresses the glucose consumption of these structures but to a lesser degree, and some of the structures are asymmetrically affected. For example, the meta- bolic activity of the ipsilateral cochlear nucleus equals 75% of the activity of the contralateral nucleus. The lateral lemniscus, superior olive, and medial genicu- late ganglion are slightly lower on the contralateral side while the contralateral inferior colliculus is mark- edly lower in metabolic activity than the ipsilateral structure (Fig. 2). These results demonstrate that there is somé degree of lateralization and crossing of audi- tory pathways in the rat. Visual deprivation in the rat. In the rat, the visual system is 80-85% crossed at the optic chiasma (LasHiey, 1934; MONTERO & GUILLERY, 1968), and unilateral enucleation removes most of the visual in- put to the central visual structures of the contralateral side. In the conscious rat studied 24h after unilateral enucleation, there are marked decrements in glucose utilization in the contralateral superior colliculus, lateral geniculate ganglion, and visual cortex as com- pared to the ipsilateral side (Fig. 3) (KENNEDY et al., 17 Fic. 1. Effects of focal seizures produced by local application of penicillin to motor cortex on local cerebral glucose utilization in the rhesus monkey. The penicillin was applied to the hand and face area of the left motor cortex. The left side of the brain is on the left in each of the autoradiographs in the figure. The numbers are the rates of local cerebral glucose utilization in pmol/100 g tissue/min. Note the following: Upper left, motor cortex in region of penicillin application and corresponding region of contralateral motor cortex; Lower left, ipsilateral and contralateral motor cortical regions remote from area of penicillin applications; Upper right, ipsilateral and contralateral putamen and globus pallidus; Lower right, ipsilateral and contraiateral thalamic nuclei and substantia nigra. From KENNEDY et al. (1975). Fic. 2. Effects of auditory deprivation on cerebral glucose utilization of some components of the auditory system of the albino rat. Upper. autoradiograph of section of brain from normal conscious rat with intact bilateral hearing in ambient noise of laboratory. The autoradiograph shows the inferior colliculi, the lateral lemnisci, and the superior olives, all of which exhibit bilateral’ symmetry of optical densities. Middle, autoradiograph of comparable section of brain from rat with bilateral occlusion of external auditory canals with wax and kept in sound-proof room. Note the virtual disappearance of the inferior colliculi, lateral lemnisci, and superior olives. Lower, autoradiograph of comparable section of brain from rat with one external auditory canal blocked. Note the asymmetry of the inferior colliculi, and the almost symmetrical intermediate reductions of densities in the lateral lemnisci and superior olives. The ear that was blocked was contralateral to the inferior colliculus that was markedly depressed. From Des Rosiers, Kennedy & Sokoloff (unpublished observations). Fic. 3. Effects of unilateral enucleation on ['*C]deoxyglucose uptake in components of the visual system in the rat. In the normal rat with both eyes intact the uptakes in the lateral geniculate bodies (LG), superior colliculi (SC) and striate cortex (STR C) are approximately equal on both sides (A and C). In the unilaterally enucleated rat there are marked decreases in optical densities in the areas corresponding to these structures on the side contralateral to the enucleation (B and D). From KENNEDY et al. (1975). 20 e—— 5.0mm Fic. 4. Autoradiographs of coronal brain sections from rhesus monkeys at the level of the lateral geniculate bodies. Large arrows point to the lateral geniculate bodies; small arrows point to oculomotor nuclear complex. (A) Animal with intact binocular vision. Note the bilateral symmetry and relative homogeneity of the lateral geniculate bodies and oculomotor nuclei. (B) Animal with bilateral visual occlusion. Note the reduced relative densities, the relative homogeneity, and the bilateral symmetry of the lateral geniculate bodies and oculomotor nuclei. (C) Animal with right eye occluded. The left side of the brain is on the left side of the photograph. Note the laminae and the inverse order of the dark and light bands in the two lateral. geniculate bodies. Note also the lesser density of the oculomotor nuclear complex on the side contralateral to the occluded eye. From KENNEDY et al. (1976). , 21 Cc 5.0mm Fic. 5. Autoradiographs of coronal brain sections from rhesus monkeys at the level of the striate cortex. (A) Animal with normal binocular vision. Note the laminar distribution of the density; the dark band corresponds to Layer IV. (B) Animal with bilateral visual deprivation. Note the almost uniform and reduced relative density, especially the virtual disappearance of the dark band correspond- ing to Layer IV. (C) Animal with right eye occluded. The half-brain on the left side of the photograph represents the left hemisphere contralateral to the occluded ‘eye. Note the alternate dark and light striations, each approx 0.3-0.4 mm in width, representing the ocular dominance columns. These columns are most apparent in the dark lamina corresponding to Layer IV but extend through the entire thickness of the cortex. The arrows point to regions of bilateral asymmetry where the ocular dominance columns are absent. These are presumably areas with normally only monocular input. The one on left, contra- lateral to occluded eye, has a continuous dark lamina corresponding to Layer IV which is completely absent on the side ipsilateral to the occluded eye. These regions are believed to be the loci of the cortical representations of the blind spots. From KENNEDY ef al. (1976). Function and energy metabolism in CNS 23 TABLE 1. NORMAL VALUES FOR LOCAL CEREBRAL GLUCOSE UTILIZATION IN THE CON- SCIOUS AND THIOPENTAL-ANESTHETIZED ALBINO RAT*§ Local cerebral glucose utilization (umol/100 g/min) Structure Control (6)t Anesthetized (8)t % Effect Gray matter Visual cortex Wi+5 644+ 3 -—42 Auditory cortex 157 + 5 81+3 —48 Parietal cortex 107 + 3 65+2 —39 Sensory-motor cortex 118 + 3 67+2 —43 Lateral geniculate body 924+2 53 +3 —42 Medial geniculate body 126+ 6 63 +3 —50 Thalamus: lateral nucleus 108 + 3 58 +2 —46 Thalamus: ventral nucleus 98 +3 55+1 —44 Hypothalamus 63 + 3 4342 —32 Caudate-putamen 1i+t4 72+3 —35 Hippocampus: Ammon’s horn 7941 564+ 1 —29 Amygdala 56+4 4142 —27 Cochlear nucleus 12447 79+5 —36 Lateral lemniscus {1447 75+4 —34 Inferior colliculus 198 +7 13148 —34 Superior olivary nucleus 141 +5 104+7 —26 Superior colliculus 99 + 3 59 +3 —-40 Vestibular nucleus 133 +4 81+4 —39 Pontine gray matter 69 +3 46+ 3 —33 Cerebellar cortex 66 +2 4442 —33 Cerebellar nucleus 106 + 4 715+4 —29 White matter Corpus callosum 4242 30 +2 —29 Genu of corpus callosum 3545 30 +2 —14 Internal capsule 35+2 294+2 -17 Cerebellar white matter 384+2 29 +2 —24 * Determined at 30 min following pulse of ['*C]deoxyglucose. +The values are the means + standard errors obtained in the number of animals indicated in parentheses. All the differences are statistically significant at the P < 0.05 level. § From SoKoLorF et al. (1977). 1975). In the rat with both eyes intact, no asymmetry in the autoradiographs is observed (Fig. 3). Visual deprivation in the monkey. In animals with binocular visual systems, such as the rhesus monkey, there is only approx 50°% crossing of the visual path- ways, and the structures of the visual system on each side of the brain receive equal inputs from both retinae. Although each retina projects more or less equally to both hemispheres, their projections remain segregated and terminate in six well-defined laminae in the lateral geniculate ganglia, three each for the ipsilateral and contralateral eyes (HUBEL & WIESEL, 1968, 1972; WlesEL et al., 1974; Raxic, 1976). This segregation is preserved in the optic radiations which project the monocular representations of the two eyes for any segment of the visual field to adjacent regions of Layer IV of the striate cortex (HUBEL & WIESEL, 1968, 1972). The cells responding to the input of each monocular terminal zone are distributed transversely through the thickness of the striate cortex resulting in a mosaic of columns, 0.3-0.5 mm in width, alter- nately representing the monocular inputs of the two eyes. The nature and distribution of these occular dominance columns have previously been character- ized by electrophysiological techniques (HUBEL & WIESEL, 1968), Nauta degeneration methods (HUBEL & Wiese, 1972), and by autoradiographic -visualiza- tion of axonal and transneuronal transport of [3H]proline- and (*H]fucose-labeled protein and/or glycoprotein (WieseL et al., 1974; RAKIC, 1976). Bila- teral or unilateral visual deprivation. either by enuc- leation or by the insertion of opaque plastic discs, produce consistent changes in the pattern of distribu- tion of the rates of glucose consumption, all clearly visible in the autoradiographs, that coincide closely with the changes in functional activity expected from known physiological and anatomic properties of the binocular visual system (KENNEDY et al., 1976). In animals with intact binocular vision no bilateral asymmetry is seen in the autoradiographs of the structures of the visual system (Figs. 4A, 5A). The lateral geniculate ganglia and oculomotor nuclei appear to be of fairly uniform density and essentially the same on both sides (Fig. 4A). The visual cortex is also the same on both sides (Fig. 5A), but through- out all of Area 17 there is heterogeneous density dis- tributed in a characteristic laminar pattern. These observations indicate that in animals with binocular visual input the rates of glucose consumption in the visual pathways are essentially equal on both sides 24 Louis SOKOLOFF of the brain and relatively uniform in the oculomotor nuclei and lateral geniculate ganglia, but, markedly different in the various layers of ‘te “hate “Cottey: Autoradiographs from animals with both eyes occluded exhibit generally decreased labeling of all components of the visual system, but the bilateral symmetry is fully retained (Figs. 4B, 5B), and the den- sity within each lateral geniculate body is for the most part fairly uniform (Fig. 4B). In the striate cortex, however, the marked differences in the densities of the various layers seen in the animals with intact bila- teral vision (Fig. 5A) are virtually absent so that, except for a faint delineation of a band within Layer IV, the concentration of the label is essentially homo- geneous throughout the striate cortex (Fig. 5B). Autoradiographs from monkeys with only monocu- lar input because of unilateral visual occlusion exhibit markedly different patterns from those described above. Both lateral geniculate bodies exhibit exactly inverse patterns of alternating dark and light bands corresponding to the known laminae representing the regions receiving the different inputs from the retinae of the intact and occluded eyes (Fig. 4C). Bilateral asymmetry is also seen in the oculomotor nuclear complex; a lower density is apparent in the nuclear complex contralateral to the occluded eye (Fig. 4C). In the striate cortex the pattern of distribution of the ['*C]DG-6-P appears to be a composite of the pat- terns seen in the animals with intact and bilaterally occluded visual input. The pattern found in the former regularly alternates with that of the latter in columns oriented perpendicularly to the cortical sur- face (Fig. 5C). The dimensions, arrangement, and dis- tribution of these columns are identical to those of the ocular dominance columns described by Hubel and Wiesel (HUBEL & WIESEL, 1968, 1972; WIESEL et al., 1974). These columns reflect the interdigitation of the representations of the two retinae in the visual cortex. Each element in the visual fields is represented by a pair of contiguous bands in the visual cortex, one for each of the two retinae or their portions that correspond to the given point in the visual fields. With symmetrical visual input bilaterally, the columns representing the two eyes are equally active and, therefore, not visualized in the autoradiographs (Fig. SA). When one eye is blocked, however, only those columns representing the blocked eye become metabolically less active, and the autoradiographs then display the alternate bands of normal and depressed activities corresponding to the regions of visual cortical representation of the two eyes (Fig. SC). There can be seen in the autoradiographs from the animals with unilateral visual deprivation a pair of regions in the folded calcarine cortex that exhibit bila- teral asymmetry (Fig. SC). The ocular dominance columns are absent on both sides, but on the side contralateral to the occluded eye this region has the appearance of visual cortex from an animal with nor- mal bilateral vision, and on the ipsilateral side this Yas region looks like cortex from an animal with both eyes occluded (Fig. 5). These regions are the loci of the ddHichl répréséntdtion of the blind spots of the visual fields and normally have only monocular input (KENNEDY et al, 1975, 1976). The area of the optic dise in the nasal half of each retina cannot transmit to this region of the contralateral striate cortex which, therefore, receives its sole input from an area in the temporal half of the ipsilateral retina. Occlusion of one eye deprives this region of the ipsilateral striate cortex of all input while the corresponding region of the contralateral striate cortex retains uninterrupted input from the intact eye. The metabolic reflection of this ipsilateral monocular input is seen in the autoradiograph in Fig. 5C. The results of these studies with the ['*C]deoxyglu- cose method in the binocular visual system of the monkey represent the most dramatic demonstration of the close relationship between physiological changes in functional activity and the rate of energy metabolism in specific components of the CNS. PHARMACOLOGICAL STUDIES The ability of the ['*C]deoxyglucose method to map the entire brain for localized regions of altered functional activity on the basis of changes in energy metabolism offers a potent tool to identify the neural sites of action of agents with neuropharmacological and psychopharmacological actions. The method is still too new to have been extensively used for this purpose, but a number of such studies have been in- itiated, and preliminary reports have appeared. The inhalation of 5—10° CO,. which increases cer- ebral blood flow and produces desynchronization and a shift to higher frequency activity in the electroence- phalogram, causes in the conscious rat moderate but diffuse reductions in local cerebral glucose utilization (Des Rosters et al., 1976). y-Hydroxybutyrate and -butyrolactone, which is hydrolyzed to y-hydroxybutyrate in plasma, produce trance-like behavioral states associated with marked suppression of electroencephalographic activity (GIARMAN & Rotu, 1964). These effects are reversible, and these drugs have been used clinically as anesthe- tic adjuvants. There is evidence that these agents lower neuronal activity in the nigrostriatal pathway and may act by inhibition of dopaminergic synapses (RotH, 1976). Studies in rats with the ('*C]deoxyglu- ‘cose technique have demonstrated that y-butyrolac- tone produces profound dose-dependent reductions of glucose utilization throughout the brain (WOLFSON et al., 1976). At the highest doses studied, 600 mg/kg of body weight, glucose utilization was reduced by approx 75% in gray matter and 33% in white matter, but there was no obvious further specificity with re- spect to the local cerebral structures affected. The reversibility of the effects and the magnitude and dif- fuseness of the depression of cerebral metabolic rate suggests that this drug might be considered as a chemical substitute for hypothermia in conditions in Function and energy metabolism in CNS 25 which profound reversible reduction of cerebral meta- bolism is desired. Ascending dopaminergic pathways appear to have a potent influence on glucose utilization in the fore- brain of rats. Electrolytic lesions placed unilaterally in the lateral hypothalamus or pars compacta of the substantia nigra caused marked ipsilateral reductions of glucose metabolism in numerous forebrain struc- tures rostral to the lesion, particularly the frontal cer- ebral cortex, caudate-putamen, and parts of the tha- lamus (SCHWARTZ et al., 1976). Similar lesions in the locus coeruleus had no such effects. Enhancement of dopaminergic synaptic activity by administration of the agonist of dopamine, apomor- phine (WOLFSON & Brown, 1976). or of amphetamine (Wechsler, Savaki & Sokoloff, unpublished observa- tions), which stimulates release of dopamine at the synapse, produces marked increases in glucose con- sumption in some of the components of the extrapyr- amidal system known or suspected to contain dopa- mine-receptive cells. With both drugs, the greatest in- creases noted were in the zona reticulata of the sub- stantia nigra and the subthalamic nucleus. Surpris- ingly, the caudate nucleus was unaffected, and reduc- tions were noted in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus and the habenula (Wechsler, Savaki & Sokoloff, unpublished observations). The effects of the potent psychotomimetic agent p-lysergic acid diethylamide, have been examined in the rat (SHINOHARA et al., 1976). In doses of 12.5-125 ug/kg, it caused dose-dependent reductions in glucose utilization in a number of cerebral structures. With increasing dosage more structures were affected and to a greater degree. There was no pattern in the distri- bution of the effects, at least none discernible at the present level of resolution, that might contribute to the understanding of the drug’s psychotomimetic actions. Acute morphine administration depresses glucose utilization in many areas of the brain, but the specific effects of morphine could not be distinguished from those of the hypercapnia produced by the associated respiratory depression (SAKURADA et al., 1976). In contrast, morphine addiction, produced within 24h by a single subcutaneous injection of 150 mg/kg of morphine base in an oil emulsion, reduces glucose utilization in a large number of gray structures in the absence of changes in arterial pCO,. White matter appears to be unaffected. Naloxone (1 mg/kg subcu- taneously) reduces glucose utilization in a number of structures when administered to normal rats, but when given to the morphine-addicted animals pro- duces an acute withdrawal-syndrome and reverses the reductions of glucose utilization in several of the structures, most strikingly in the habenula (SAKURADA et al.. 1976). SUMMARY The results of studies with the ['*C]deoxyglucose technique unequivocally estabiish that functional ac- tivity in specific components of the CNS is, as in other tissues, closely. coupled to the local rate of energy metabolism. Stimulation of functional activity increases the local rate of glucose utilization; reduced functional activity depresses it. These changes are so profound that they can be visualized directly in auto- radiographic representations of local tissue concen- trations of [!4C]deoxyglucose-6-phosphate. Indeed, the existence of such evoked metabolic responses to experimentally induced alterations in local functional activity, together with the ability to visualize them by the [!4C]deoxyglucose method, has become the basis of a potent technique for the mapping of func- tional pathways in the CNS (KENNEDY et al., 1975, 1976; PLum et al. 1976). The potential usefulness of this technique would be greatly advanced by techno- logical improvements in the autoradiographic pro- cedure that improve the spatial resolution to the microscopic or even the single-cell level. Acknowledgements—The author wishes to express his appreciation to SUZANNE M. Cook for her valuable assist- ance in the preparation of this manuscript. REFERENCES BACHELARD H. S. (1971) J. Neurochem. 18, 213-222. Bipper T. G. (1968) J. Neurochem. 15, 867-874. Caveness W. F. (1969) in Basic Mechanisms of the Epilep- ’ sies (JASPER H. H., Warp A. A. & Pope A., eds.) pp. 517-534. Little, Brown, Boston. Coitins R. C., KENNEDY C., SoKoLorF L. & PLuM F. (1976) Arch Neurol. 33, 536-542. Des Rosters M. H., KENNEDY C., SHINOHARA M. & SOKO- LOFF L. (1976) Neurology 26, 346. GIARMAN N. J. & Rotu R. H. (1964) Science 145, 583-584. Hers H. G. (1957) Le Métabolisme du Fructose. p. 102. Editions Arscia, Bruxelles. Huse, D. H. & Wiese, T. N. (1968) J. Physiol. 195, 215-243. Huser D. H. & Wiese T. N. (1972) J. comp. Neurol. 146, 421-450. Kennepy C., Des Rosiers M. H., ReivicH M., SHARP F., JEHLE J. W. & SoKOLOFF L. (1975) Science 187, 850-853. KeENneDY C., Des Rosters M. H., SAKURADA O., SHINO- HARA M., REIVICH M., JEHLE J. W. & SOKOLOFF L. (1976) Proc. natn. Acad. Sci., U.S.A. 73, 4230-4234. Kery S. S. (1950) Am. J. Med. 8, 205-217. Kety S. S. (1957) in The Metabolism of the Nervous System (RICHTER D., ed.) pp. 221-237. Pergamon Press, London. LANDAU W. H., FREYGANG W. H., ROWLAND L. P., SOKO- Lorr L. & Kety S. S. (1955) Trans. Am. Neurol. Ass. 80, 125-129. LasHLey K. S. (1934) J. comp. Neurol. 59, 341-373. LASSEN N. A. (1959) Physiol. Rev. 39, 183-238. Montero V. M. & GuILLery R. W. (1968) J. comp. Neurol. 134, 211-242. OLDENDORF W. H. (1971) Am. J. Physiol, 221, 1629-1638. PLuM F., GseDvE A. & SAMSON F. E. (1976) Neurosci. Res. Prog. Bull. 14, 457-518. Rakic P. (1976) Nature 261, 467-471. Reivicu M., JeHLe J. W., SOKOLOFF L. & KETyY S. S. (1969) J. Appl. Physiol. 27, 296-300. 6 Louis SOKOLOFF Rotu R. H. (1976) Pharmac. Ther. 2. 71-88. SAKURADA O., SHINOHARA M., KLEE W. A. KENNEDY C. & SOKOLOFF L. (1976) Neurosci. Abstr. 2 (Part 4). 613. ScHwartz W. J., SHARP F. R., GUNN R. H. & EvartTs E. V. (1976) Nature 261, 155-157. Sapiro H. M., GREENBERG J. H., REIVICH M., SHiPKo E., VAN Horn K. & SoxovorF L. (1975) in Blood F low and Metabolism in the Brain (HARPER A. M., JENNETT W. B., MILLER J. D. & Rowan J. O., eds.) pp. 9.42-9.43. Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh. Suarp F. R., KAUER J. S. & SHEPHERD G. M. (1975) Brain Res. 98. 596-600. SHINOHARA M., SAKURADA O., JEHLE J. & SOKOLOFF L. (1976) Neurosci. Abstr. 2 (Part 1), 615. Reprinted by the SOKOLOFF L. (1969) in Psychochemical Research in Man (MANDELL A. J. & MANDELL M. P., eds.) pp. 237-252. Academic Press, New York. SoKOLOFF L. (1976) in Basic Neurochemistry (SiEGEL G. J., Avpers R. W., Katzman R. & AGRANOFF B. W., eds.) 2nd ed, pp. 388-413. Little, Brown, Boston. SoKOLorF L., REIVICH M., KENNEDY C., Des ROSIERS M. H., PatLak C. S., PeTTiGRew K. D., SAKURADA oO. & SHINOHARA M. (1977) J. Neurochem. 28, 897-916. Sots A. & CRANE R. K. (1954) J. biol. Chem. 210, 581-595. Wiese. T. N., Huser D. H. & Lam D. M. K. (1974) Brain Res. 79, 273-279. WOLFSON L. I. & BROWN L. (1976) Neurosci. Abstr. 2 (Part 1), 510. WOLFSON L. I., SAKURADA O. & SOKOLOFF L. (1976) Trans. Am. Soc. Neurochem. 7, 165. U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION, AND WELFARE © National Institutes of Health