CO Nutrition and Health g of this amount is storage iron. The total iron requirement during the entire course of pregnancy averages | g and, therefore, exceeds the amount of storage iron in most women. However, an increase in efficiency of dietary iron absorption compensates in part for the limited iron stores. Most of the additional iron is needed for growth of the fetus and placenta during the last half of pregnancy (Bothwell et al. 1979). Even with the normal adaptive increase in iron absorption that takes place during pregnancy, iron stores may become depleted. For this reason, clinicians commonly recommend an iron supplement that provides the equivalent of about 30 mg of elemental iron or more per day during the last half of pregnancy. After delivery, the iron requirement of the mother is temporarily reduced below normal as the expanded mass of red cells decreases to baseline levels. Mild iron deficiency in the pregnant woman generally has no detectable effect on hemoglobin concentration in the newborn. However, the severe _iron deficiency anemia seen in pregnant women in some developing coun- tries can cause decreased placental weight and birth weight as well as anemia in the newborn (Singla et al. 1978). Men and Postmenopausal Women. Normally, iron stores increase through- out adult life in men and after menopause in women (Cook, Finch, and Smith 1976), so overt nutritional iron deficiency rarely develops in these groups. Among older persons, anemia is more commonly associated with chronic diseases, inflammation, and infections than with iron deficiency. However, factors that can predispose these groups to iron deficiency ane- mia include frequent blood donations (three or more times a year) and blood losses due to chronic aspirin ingestion, bleeding ulcers, and colorec- tal cancers. Frequent aspirin consumption impairs blood coagulation and leads to blood loss from the intestine, usually in minute amounts that can be detected only by sensitive tests. For example, one study found that 300 mg of aspirin taken three times a day for a week increased intestinal blood loss to 5 ml/day (which averages several times normal menstrual losses) from a normal average of 0.5 ml/day (Pierson et al. 1961). When there is no known basis for iron deficiency anemia, lesions such as a peptic ulcer or carcinoma may cause chronic and inapparent blood loss from the intestine. Parasitic infestation, particularly with hookworm,-com- monly causes inapparent intestinal blood loss in some developing coun- tries, but is relatively rare in the United States. / 476 Anemia @| Consequences of Iron Deficiency The manifestations of iron deficiency can be subtle and depend on the relative severity of functional impairment caused by reduced levels of the “essential” iron compounds (Dallman 1982). These manifestations are related to the effects of iron deficiency on other tissues, to the resultant anemia, or to a combination of the two (Dallman 1986). One difficulty in interpreting studies in this area is that the effects of impaired iron status may differ among iron-deficient individuals who are—or are not—anemic. Work Performance. Studies in humans as well as in rats show that iron deficiency causes a substantial reduction in work capacity, particularly when the concentration of hemoglobin falls below 10 g/dl, which is 2 to 4 g/ di below the lower limit of normal for adults. Some studies in humans indicate that even mild anemia can decrease performance in brief, hard exercise (Viteri and Torun 1974). To determine the practical significance of these findings for productivity, manual laborers in developing countries who were suffering from iron deficiency anemia were studied. For exam- ple, among men on a rubber plantation in Indonesia (Basta et al. 1979) and women on a tea plantation in Sri Lanka (Edgerton et al. 1979), the produc- tivity of iron-deficient individuals was significantly less than that of work- ers with a normal hemoglobin concentration. After iron supplementation, the performance of the iron-deficient subjects improved, with the greatest improvement occurring in those who had the lowest initial hemoglobin concentrations. In these studies, it has not been possible to distinguish the extent to which this impaired work performance is due to anemia per se or to the tissue abnormalities accompanying iron deficiency. Experiments with rats have shown that dietary iron deficiency also results in a marked impairment in the oxidative production of cellular energy in skeletal muscle (Finch et al. 1976; McLane et al. 1981; Davies et al. 1984). The major consequence of this muscle impairment is a lessened capacity for prolonged exercise or physical endurance, whereas anemia primarily restricts the performance of brief, strenuous exercise (McLane et al. 1981; Davies et al. 1984). Body Temperature Regulation. An impaired capacity to maintain body temperature in a cold environment is another characteristic of iron defi- ciency anemia. This abnormality is related to decreased secretion of thy- roid-stimulating hormone and thyroid hormone (Beard et al. 1984) and to the anemia itself; studies of the iron-deficient rat show that a transfusion of red blood cells corrects the abnormality. Furthermore, normally fed rats 477 C) Nutrition and Health develop impaired heat production when they are made anemic by an exchange transfusion that replaces red blood cells with plasma that lacks hemoglobin. Behavior and Intellectual Performance. Increasing evidence suggests that changes in behavior and impaired development and intellectual perfor- mance may result from iron deficiency (Lozoff and Brittenham 1986; Pollitt 1985). Most studies have evaluated infants between 6 months and 2 years of age using the Bayley Scale of Infant Development, a test to evaluate sensory development, fine and gross motor skills, and language develop- ment in this group. By this standard, infants who are even mildly iron deficient have a statistically significant decrease in responsiveness, activi- ty, and attentiveness and have increased body tension, fearfulness, and tendency to fatigue (Lozoff et al. 1982b). The long-term significance of these observations has not been determined. Of particular interest is the observation that these deficits are most profound in the oldest infants (19 to 24 months), in whom iron deficiency may have been present for the longest period (Lozoff et al. 1982a). Another important finding is that even infants with very mild iron deficiency anemia, or simply with early evidence of impaired hemoglobin production, do not score as well as infants with no laboratory evidence of iron deficiency or evidence merely of depleted iron stores (Lozoff et al. 1982b; Oski et al. 1983; Walter, Kovalskys, and Stekel 1983). Although the results of these studies are not as conclusive, the behavioral abnormalities are significant because the brain’s rapid rate of growth and differentiation during infancy might make it particularly vulner- able to nutrient deficiencies. Because the same adverse environmental factors that lead to iron deficiency could also be responsible for behavioral deficits, these studies are difficult to interpret. This complexity may help to explain why some children display a rapid improvement in the Bayley score after iron treatment (Oski et al. 1983), whereas others do not (Lozoff et al. 1982a, 1982b). Resistance to Infections. Decreased resistance to infection is characteristic of iron deficiency in both humans and experimental animals (Beisel 1982; Vyas and Chandra 1984; Dallman 1986). Iron-deficient children have abnor- malities in lymphocytes and neutrophils, two types of white blood cells that help defend against infections (see chapter on infections and immunity). Although infections are associated with decreases in measures of iron status, these decreases may not be related to deficient iron intake. Despite numerous studies that show an impaired resistance to infection under laboratory conditions, no conclusive evidence demonstrates that iron deficiency itself causes an increased rate of infections in free-living 478 Anemia O individuals. Iron deficiency anemia and infections are both common among poor populations, but a cause-and-effect relationship, although plausible, has not been established (Strauss 1978). These issues are discussed further in the chapter on infections and immunity. Lead Poisoning. Iron deficiency substantially increases the risk of lead poisoning, particularly in young children. Iron-deficient individuals absorb increased amounts of lead (Watson et al. 1986), and elevated blood lead concentrations have been observed among some children with laboratory evidence of iron deficiency (Yip and Dallman 1984). Prevention of Iron Deficiency Iron deficiency can be prevented by increasing dietary iron intake, improv- ing the bioavailability of iron in the diet, or decreasing body losses of iron. Dietary iron intakes can be improved by increasing the consumption of iron-rich foods, administering iron supplements to specific target popula- tions, and fortifying certain food products with iron. Parental awareness of appropriate diet is especially important for infants, whose diet is relatively simple. Typically, an infant’s diet is discussed during routine health mainte- nance visits. Fortification of cereal and grain products is a relatively inexpensive and effective means of increasing iron intake (International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group 1982; Clydesdale and Wiemer 1985). These foods usually reach the population as a whole in adequate amounts to make a difference without the need for individual counseling. Nevertheless, sev- eral aspects of fortification are controversial. Because some iron com- pounds are poorly absorbed (Cook and Reusser 1983; Yetley and Glins- mann 1983; Hurrell 1984), it is difficult to document that eating fortified foods helps prevent iron deficiency, especially because baseline data are unavailable. Readily absorbed forms of iron react with foods and some- times decrease shelf life. One compound that may strike a reasonable compromise between shelf life and bioavailability is finely powdered metal- ic iron, although some studies find that this form, too, is poorly absorbed (Elwood et al. 1968; Hallberg, Brune, and Rossander 1986). Continuation of breastfeeding for 6 months or more confers substantial protection against development of iron deficiency in full-term infants (AAP Committee on Nutrition 1985). Iron-fortified infant formula, which was introduced about two decades ago in the United States, has gradually supplanted most unfortified formulas and fresh cow milk as infant food during the first year of life (Martinez and Nalezienski 1979). In a large 479 CO Nutrition and Health survey conducted in 1984, 77 percent of 5- to 6-month-old infants in the United States who were receiving cow milk formula were consuming the iron-fortified form (Martinez 1985). Today, iron deficiency in otherwise healthy infants is almost entirely restricted to those on a diet of fresh cow milk (Sadowitz and Oski 1983) or unfortified cow milk formula (Saarinen 1978). Vitamin C (Ascorbic Acid) Fortification. The absorption of iron from fortified cereals can be increased twofold to threefold if the cereals are also fortified with about 5 mg of vitamin C per mg of iron (International Nutri- tional Anemia Consultative Group 1982; Cook and Bothwell 1984). Some of the effectiveness of iron-fortified infant formulas in preventing iron defi- ciency has been attributed to their fortification with this vitamin (Stekel 1984). Iron Supplements. Supplementation has the disadvantage of requiring extra effort and expense compared with fortified foods. People do not always remember to take medications consistently and regularly. Supple- ments are less available to the poor and other groups most likely to be iron deficient. Supplementation is, however, a reasonable approach to preven- tion of iron deficiency in breastfed premature infants, in pregnant women, and in targeted high-risk population groups. In these situations, a large amount of dietary iron is provided over a short period of time. Its useful- ness for a given individual requires evaluation by a qualified health profes- ~ sional. Iron supplement use and recommendations to increase dietary iron intake are usually not necessary for the general population. Among Ameri- cans as a whole, poor iron status is relatively rare (LSRO 1984b) and iron supplements are not associated with improved iron status (Looker et al. 1987). An additional concern is that increased iron intake can harm individ- uals who are susceptible to iron overload (LSRO 1984b). Federal Food Programs. Because iron deficiency is more common among the poor than among persons above the poverty line (DHHS/USDA 1986), iron status should improve as a result of Federal food programs that serve socioeconomically disadvantaged groups. In one study, infants and chil- dren who had not received aid from the WIC program in 1973-74 were far more frequently anemic than a similar population in 1977 who were en- rolled in the WIC program since birth (Miller, Swaney, and Deinard 1985). ' ‘Although differences unrelated to iron intake probably existed between the two groups, which might account for differences.in iron status, the foods provided by the WIC program included several sources of iron or enhan- 480 Anemia e) cers of iron absorption: formula fortified with 12 mg of iron per liter for infants up to 12 months of age, and iron-fortified infant cereal and ascorbic acid-fortified fruit juice for infants between 6 and 12 months of age. Potentially Adverse Consequences. Under ordinary circumstances, a mod- est excess of dietary iron (over requirements) does not have adverse consequences. On the other hand, increased iron intake might be harmful for some individuals. A much discussed example is the hereditary condi- tion hemochromatosis, in which abnormal amounts of tissue iron accumu- late over the years as a result of a genetic defect in absorption, eventually damaging the liver, heart, pancreas, and adrenal glands (Bothwell et al. 1979). A subset of homozygous individuals with hemochromatosis usually develops symptoms by middle age. Heterozygous carriers of the hemo- chromatosis gene may be at slightly increased risk for iron overload (Brown 1981), but the clinical significance of this risk is uncertain. Although there is no direct evidence that current levels of iron fortification are harmful, concerns have been raised that any increased level of iron in the food supply may harm individuals susceptible to iron overload. Individuals who require repeated blood transfusions for relatively rare conditions, such as thalassemia, may also accumulate excess iron, which is then deposited in soft tissues. Fortification iron adds an undesirable, but relatively small, load compared with that administered by transfusion. Excessive iron intake may affect the absorption of other trace elements. Although a high dose of iron medication impairs the absorption of zinc or copper administered at the same time (Solomons and Jacobs 1981), this interaction probably does not occur at the much smaller fortification levels. Of greater concern is the possibility of compromised zinc or copper absorp- tion in groups who take larger amounts of iron as supplements, such as infants and pregnant women (Breskin et al. 1983; Hambidge et al. 1987). Excessive iron administration may increase the risk of infection (Pearson and Robinson 1976; Beisel 1982). The basis for this concern is that a high degree of unsaturation in the iron-binding protein transferrin suppresses the growth of many bacteria. Conditions of iron overload are associated with very low levels of unsaturation. Although this issue may be important in clinically diagnosed iron overload, recent U.S. survey data have found a low prevalence of abnormally high transferrin saturation values in the general population (LSRO 1984b). 481 C) Nutrition and Health Role of Folate and Vitamin B,. in Anemia Although deficiencies of folate or vitamin B,, are relatively rare among the general U.S. population, certain groups are particularly at risk for develop- ing these nutrient deficiencies. Folate deficiency is of special concern. Its extent in the general population is virtually unknown. Low socioeconomic groups at vulnerable stages of the life cycle are at greatest risk—pregnant women (especially adolescents), infants (especially those who are small or premature), young children, and older persons (Shojania 1984). Surveys of the folate status of these groups have been too limited in sample size and methodology to permit adequate evaluation (LSRO 1986). Folate requirements for pregnant women are greatest in the last trimester of pregnancy; however, most women’s diets meet these requirements. Oral folate supplementation during this period is often recommended for high-risk groups. Premature infants, particularly those of very low birth weight, may need more folate to support their growth than is provided by infant formula or breast milk. Under a physician’s guidance, this folate can be supplied as an oral supplement. Infants fed primarily unfortified goat milk are also at risk for folate deficiency because of the unusually low folate content of this food. Other risk factors for folate deficiency include chronic disease and use of alcohol and drugs. Chronic and severe diarrhea, as in tropical sprue or celiac disease, for example, can cause folate deficiency. Folate deficiency also occurs among alcoholics (see chapter on alcohol). As discussed in the chapter on drug-nutrient interactions, individuals who use oral contracep- tives and other medications may be at increased risk for folate deficiency, but the extent and public health significance of these interactions is uncer- tain (LSRO 1984a). A dietary vitamin B,, deficiency can occur in strict vegetarians who avoid all foods derived from animals, including eggs and dairy products. Nursing infants of women consuming such diets or of women who are vitamin By>- deficient are also at risk because the vitamin B,, content of the breast milk decreases as the mother’s vitamin B,, stores decline (Shojania 1984). Any strict vegetarian who becomes pregnant should be advised to supplement her diet with vitamin B,,. 482 Anemia C Implications for Public Health Policy Dietary Guidance General Public Prevention of nutrition-related anemia depends on adequate dietary intake of iron, vitamin B,,, and folate as well as the full complement of other essential nutrients. Except for younger children and women of reproduc- tive age, who are at greater risk for iron deficiency, it appears that current iron consumption levels are sufficient for most of the population. Special Populations Routine health care for infants and pregnant women, the groups at highest risk for anemia, should include laboratory evaluation for anemia and nutritional advice on methods to ensure adequate iron intake. Nonpreg- nant women in their childbearing years and adolescents are also at greater risk for iron deficiency anemia; these individuals should be monitored and should receive special counsel on preventing iron deficiency. Frequent blood donors, another high-risk group, should be advised by blood bank personnel about dietary methods to enhance iron intake and absorption. Groups who may need iron supplements, such as premature infants, preg- nant women, women with excessive menstrual bleeding, frequent blood donors, strict vegetarians, and regular aspirin users, should also receive advice from health professionals on enhancing iron bioavailability from the diet. Specific education efforts directed toward these special groups, even though difficult, are needed. Folate deficiency anemia usually occurs among women late in the course of pregnancy, among small and premature infants, and among alcoholics. These groups, especially from low-income families, should receive advice about dietary and supplemental sources of this vitamin. Strict vegetarians who consume no foods of animal origin, especially women who are pregnant or nursing, should be advised to consume supple- mental sources of vitamin B,5. Nutrition Programs and Services Food Programs Because groups that benefit from food programs are those at highest risk for anemia, such programs should continue to be made available to high- 483 C Nutrition and Health risk groups and should encourage consumption of foods rich in iron and folate. Evidence suggests that current levels of iron fortification are safe and adequate, and no changes should be recommended at this time. Food Labels Evidence related to the role of iron and folate in anemia suggests that food labels should indicate the content of these nutrients. Special Populations Patients with anemia should receive counseling and assistance to develop diets that have adequate amounts of bioavailable iron, folate, or vitamin B 2 from dietary or supplemental sources. Research and Surveillance Research and surveillance issues of special priority related to the role of diet in anemia should include investigations into: © Screening for earlier stages of iron deficiency using tests that identify iron depletion (e.g., erythrocyte protoporphyrin). ® Elucidation of the health consequences of conditions of iron depletion _prior to development of anemia. @ Validation of methodologies for identification of the extent of deficien- cies of iron, folate, and vitamin B,, in the general population and in high-risk groups. @ Interactions between iron, folate, vitamin B,,, and other nutrients consumed in the diet. © Improved methods for analysis of the folate content of food. @ Determination of iron requirements at various stages of the life cycle and under various physiologic conditions. @ Identification of appropriate levels and types of iron in the food supply for individuals with hereditary conditions of excess iron absorption. @ Identification of the level of iron intake that confers maximum protec- tion against major infections. @ Determination of trends in iron fortification in the U.S. food supply. ® Development of effective methods to educate the general public and high-risk groups about consuming.diets adequate in iron and folate. 484 Anemia ie Literature Cited AAP Committee on Nutrition. See American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Nutrition. American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Nutrition. 1985. Pediatric nutrition hand- book. 2d ed. Elk Grove Village, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics. Anonymous. 1986. Declining anemia prevalence among children enrolled in public nutrition and health programs—selected states, 1975-1985. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 35:565-66. Basta, $.S.; Soekirman, M.S.; Karyadi, D.; and Scrimshaw, N.S. 1979. Iron deficiency anemia and the productivity of adult males in Indonesia. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 32:916-25. Beard, J.; Green, W.; Miller, L.; and Finch, C.A. 1984. Effect of iron-deficiency anemia on hormone levels and thermoregulation during cold exposure. American Journal of Physiology 247:R114-19. Beisel, W.R. 1982. Single nutrients and immunity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 35:417-68. Bothwell, T.H.; Charlton, R.W.; Cook, J.D.; and Finch, C.A. 1979. Iron metabolism in man. Oxford: Blackwell. Breskin, M.W.; Worthington-Roberts, B.S.; Knopp, R.H.: Brown, Z.: Plovie, B.: Mottet, N.K.; and Mills, J.L. 1983. First trimester serum zinc concentrations in human pregnancy. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 38:943-53. Brown, E.B. 1981. Recognition and treatment of iron overload. Advances in Internal Medicine 26: 159-86. Castro, O.L.; Haddy, T.B.; Rana, S.R.,; Worrell, K.D.; and Scott, R.B. 1985. Electronically determined red blood cell values in a large number of healthy black adults: subpopulations with low hemoglobin and red cell indices. American Journal of Epidemiology 121:930-36. Charlton, R.W., and Bothwell, T.H. 1983. Iron absorption. Annual Review of Medicine 34:55-68. Clydesdale, F.M., and Wiemer, K-L., eds. 1985. Iron fortification of foods. Orlando, FL: Academic. Cook, J.D., and Bothwell, T.H. 1984. Availability of iron from infant food. In Iron Nutrition in Infancy and Childhood, ed. A. Stekel, 119-43. New York: Raven Press. Cook, J.D., and Finch, C.A. 1979. Assessing iron status of a population. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 32:2115-19. Cook, J.D., and Monsen, E.R. 1976. Food iron absorption in human subjects. III. Comparison of the effect of animal proteins on non-heme iron absorption. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 29:859-67. Cook, J.D., and Reusser, M.E. 1983. Iron fortification; an update. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 38:648-S9. Cook, J.D.; Finch, C.A.; and Smith, N. 1976. Evaluation of the iron status of a population. Blood 48:449-5S. Council on Foods and Nutrition. 1972. Iron in enriched wheat flour, farina. bread, buns and rolls. Journal of the American Medical Association 220:855-59. Crosby, W.H. 1987. Whatever became of chlorosis? Journal of the American Medical Associa- tion 257:2799-2800. 485 ie Nutrition and Health Daliman, P.R. 1982. Manifestations of iron deficiency. Seminars in Hematology 19:19-30. . 1986. Biochemical basis for the manifestations of iron deficiency. Annual Review of Nutrition 6:13-40. . 1987. Has routine screening of infants for anemia become obsolete in the United States? Pediatrics 80:439-40. Dallman, P.R.; Siimes, M.A.; and Stekel, A. 1980. Iron deficiency in infancy and childhood. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 33:86-118. Daliman, P.R.; Yip, R.; and Johnson, C. 1984. Prevalence and causes of anemia in the United States, 1976-1980. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 39:437-45. Davies, K.J.A.; Donovan, C.M.; Refino, C.A.; Brooks, G.A.; Packer, L.; and Daliman, P.R. 1984. Distinguishing effects of anemia and muscle iron deficiency on exercise bioenergetics in the rat. American Journal of Physiology 246:E535-43. DHHS/USDA. See U.S. Department of Health and Human Services and U.S. Department of Agriculture. Eaton, $.B., and Konner, M. 1985. Paleolithic nutrition. A consideration of its nature and current implications. New England Journal of Medicine 312:283-89. Edgerton, V.R., Gardner, G.W.; Ohira, Y.; Gunawardena, K.A.; and Senewiratne, B. 1979. Iron-deficiency anaemia and its effect on worker productivity and activity patterns. British Medical Journal 2:1546-49. Elwood, P.C.; Newton, D.; Eakins, J.D.; and Brown, D.A. 1968. Absorption of iron from bread. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 21:1162-69. Finch, C.A., ed. 1982. Clinical aspects of iron deficiency and excess. Seminars in Hematology 19: 1-67. Finch, C.A.; Miller, L.R.; Inamdar, A.R.; Person, R.; Seiler, K.; and Mackler, B. 1976. Iron deficiency in the rat: physiological and biochemical studies of muscle dysfunction. Journal of Clinical Investigation 58:447-53. Florentino, R.F., and Guirriec, R.M. 1984. Prevalence of nutritional anemia in infancy and childhood with emphasis on developing countries. In /ron nutrition in infancy and childhood, ed. A. Stekel, pp. 61-72. New York: Raven. Food and Nutrition Board. 1974. Proposed fortification policy for cereal-grain products. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences. Fomon, S.J.; Ziegler, E.E.; Nelson, S.E.; and Edwards, B.B. 1981. Cow milk feeding in infancy: gastrointestinal blood loss and iron nutritional status. Journal of Pediatrics 98:540-45. Hallberg, L. 1981. Bioavailability of dietary iron in man. Annual Review of Nutrition 1:123-47. Haliberg, L.; Brune, M.: and Rossander, L. 1986. Low bioavailability of carbonyl] ion in man: studies on iron fortification of wheat flour. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 43:59-67. Hallberg, L.; Hogdahkm A.; Nilsson, L.; and Rybo, G. 1966. Menstrual blood loss—a population study. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica 45:320-51. Hallberg, L.; Rossander, L.; Persson, H.; and Svalin, E. 1982. Deleterious effects of pro- longed warming of meals on ascorbic acid content and iron absorption. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 36:848-50. Hallberg, L.; Bengtsson, C.; Garby, L.; Lennartsson, J.; Rossander, L.; and Tibblin, E. 1979. An analysis of factors leading to a reduction in iron deficiency in Swedish women. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 57:947-54. 486 Anemia @ Hambidge, K.M.; Krebs, N.F.; Sibley, L.: and English, J. 1987. Acute effects of iron therapy on zinc status during pregnancy. Obstetrics and Gynecology 70:593-96. Hefnawi. F.: Askalani, H.; and Zaki, K. 1974. Menstrual blood loss with copper intrauterine devices. Contraception 9:133-39. Herbert, V. 1984. An “extrinsic factor” and pernicious anemia. Journal of the American Medical Association 251:522-23. Huebers, H.A., and Finch, C.A. 1987. The physiology of transferrin and transferrin recep- tors. Physiological Reviews 67:520-81. Hurrell, R.F. 1984. Bioavailability of different iron compounds used to fortify formulas and cereals: technological problems. In /ron nutrition in infancy and childhood. ed. A. Stekel, pp. 147-76. New York: Raven. International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group. 1982. The effects of cereals and legumes on iron absorption. New York: Nutrition Foundation. Israel, R.; Shaw, S.T.; and Martin, M.A. 1974. Comparative quantitation of menstrual blood loss with the Lippes loop, Dalkon shield, and Copper T intrauterine devices. Contraception 10:63-71. Life Sciences Research Office. 1984a. Assessment of the folate nutritional status of the U.S. population based on the data collected in the second National Health and Nutrition Examina- tion Survey, 1976-1980, ed. S.M. Pilch and ER. Senti, p. 87. Bethesda, MD: Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology. _ 1984b. Assessment of the iron nutritional status of the U.S. population based on the data collected in the second National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1976-1980, ed. S.M. Pilch and F.R. Senti, p. 65. Bethesda, MD: Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology. . 1986. A report of the scientific community's views on progress in attaining the Public Health Service national nutrition goals for 1990. Bethesda, MD: Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology. Looker, A.C.; Sempos, C.T.; Johnson, C.L.; and Yetley, E.A. 1987. Comparison of dietary intakes and iron status of vitamin-mineral supplement users and nonusers, aged 1-19 years. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 46:665-72. Loudon, i.S.L. 1980. Chlorosis, anaemia, and anorexia nervosa. British Medical Journal 281:1669-75. Lozoff, B., and Brittenham, G.M. 1986. Behavioral aspects of iron deficiency. Progress in hematology, vol. 14, ed. E.B. Brown, pp. 23-53. New York: Grune & Stratton. Lozoff, B.; Brittenham, G.M.; Viteri, FE.; Wolf, A.W.; and Urrutia, J.J. 1982a. Developmen- tal deficits in iron-deficient infants: effects of age and severity of iron lack. Journal of Pediatrics 101:948-52. _ 1982b. The effects of short-term oral iron therapy on developmental deficits in iron- deficient anemic infants. Journal of Pediatrics 100:351-57. LSRO. See Life Sciences Research Office. Marsh, A.; Long, H.; and Stierwolt, E. 1959. Comparative hematologic response to iron fortification of a milk formula for infants. Pediatrics 24:404-12. Martinez, G.A. 1985. 1984 Milk-feeding patterns in the United States. Pediatrics 76:1004-8. Martinez, G.A., and Nalezienski, J.P. 1979. Recent trends in breast feeding. Pediatrics 64:686-92. 487 e Nutrition and Health McCollum, E.V. 1957. A history of nutrition: the sequence of ideas in nutrition investigations. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. McLane, J.A.; Fell, R.D.; McKay, R.H.; Winder, W.W.; Brown, E.B.; and Holloszy, J.0. 1981. Physiological and biochemical effects of iron deficiency on rat skeletal muscle function. American Journal of Physiology 241:C47-54, Miller, V.; Swaney, S.; and Deinard, A. 1985, Impact of the WIC program on the iron status of infants. Pediatrics 75:100-105. Oski, F.A.; Honig, A.S.; Helu, B.; and Howanitz, P. 1983. Effect of iron therapy on behavior performance in nonanemic, iron-deficient infants. Pediatrics 71:877-80. Pearson, H.A., and Robinson, J.E. 1976. The role of iron in host resistance. Advances in Pediatrics 23:1-33. . Pierson, R.N., Jr.; Holt, P.R.; Watson, R.M.,; and Keating, R.P. 1961. Aspirin and gastroin- testinal bleeding. Chromate 51 blood loss studies. American Journal of Medicine 31:259-65, Pollitt, E. 1985. Effects of iron deficiency with and without anemia on mental development among infants and preschool children. In Nutritional anthropology, ed. FE. Johnson. New York: Liss. Rios, E.; Hunter, R.E.; Cook, J.D.; Smith, N.J.; and Finch, C.A. 1975. The absorption of iron as supplements in infant cereal and infant formulas. Pediatrics 55:686-93. Rossander, L.; Hallberg, L.; and Bjorn-Rasmussen, E. 1979. Absorption of iron from break- fast meals. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 32:2484-89, Saarinen, U.M. 1978. Need for iron supplementation in infants on prolonged breast feeding. Journal of Pediatrics 93:177-80. Saarinen, U.M., and Siimes, M.A. 1977. Iron absorption from infant formula and the optimal level of iron supplementation. Acta Pediatrica Scandinavica 66:719-22. : Saarinen, U.M.; Siimes, M.A.; and Dallman, PR. 1977. Iron absoprtion in infants: high bioavailability of breast milk iron as indicated by the extrinsic tag method of iron absorption and by the concentration of serum ferritin. Journal of Pediatrics 91:36-39. Sadowitz, P.D., and Oski, FA. 1983. Iron status and infant feeding practices in an urban ambulatory center. Pediatrics 72:33-36. Shojania, A.M. 1984. Folic acid and vitamin B,> deficiency in pregnancy and in the neonatal period. Cliniés in Perinatology 11:433-59. Singla, P.N.; Chand, S.; Kharma, S.; and Agarwal, K.N. 1978.-Effect of maternal anemia on the placenta and the newborn infant. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica 67:645—-48. Solomons, N.W., and Jacobs, R.A. 1981. Studies of the bioavailability of zinc in man. IV. Effects of heme and non-heme iron on absorption of zinc. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 34:475-82. , Stekel, A. 1984. Prevention of iron deficiency. In fron nutrition in infancy and childhood, ed. A. Stekel, pp. 179-92. New York: Raven. Stekel, A.; Olivares, M.: Pizarro, F.; Chadud, P.: Lopez, I.;and Amar, M. 1986. Absorption of fortification iron from milk formulas in infants. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 43:917-22. . Stockman, J.A. 1987. Iron deficiency anemia: have we come far enough? Journal of the American Medical Association 258:1645—47. 488 Anemia OQ Strauss, R.G. 1978. Iron deficiency, infections, and immune function: a reassessment. Ameri- can Journal of Clinical Nutrition 31:660-66. Svanberg, B.; Arvidsson, B.; Norrby, A.; Rybo, G.; and Solvell, L. 1975. Absorption of supplemental iron during pregnancy. Acta Obstetricia et Gynaecologica Scandinavica 48(suppl.):87-108. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services and U.S. Department of Agriculture. 1986. Nutrition monitoring in the United States: a progress report from the Joint Nutrition Monitor- ing Evaluation Committee. DHHS publication no. (PHS) 86-1255. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Vazquez-Seone, P.; Windom, R.; and Pearson, H.A. 1985. Disappearance of iron deficiency anemia in a high-risk infant population given supplemental iron. New England Journal of Medicine 313(9):1239-40. Viteri, EE., and Torun, B. 1974. Anemia and physical work capacity. Clinical Hematology 3:609-26. Vyas, D., and Chandra, R.K. 1984. Functional complications of iron deificiency. In fron nutrition in infancy and childhood, ed. A Stekel, pp. 45-49. New York: Raven. Walter, T.; Kovalskys, J.; and Stekel, A. 1983. Effect of mild iron deficiency on infant mental development scores. Journal of Pediatrics 102:519-22. Watson, W.S.; Morrison, J.; Bethel, M.I.F.; Baldwin, N.M.; Lyon, D.T.B.; Dobson, H.; Moore, M.R.; and Hume, R. 1986. Food iron and lead absorption in humans. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 44:248-56. Yetley, E.A., and Glinsmann, W.H. 1983. Regulatory issues regarding iron bioavailability. Food Technology 37(10):121-23, 126. Yip, R., and Dallman, P.R. 1984. Developmental changes in erythrocyte protoporphyrin: roles of iron deficiency and lead toxicity. Journal of Pediatrics 104:710-13. Yip, R.; Schwartz S.; and Deinard, A.S. 1984. Hematocrit values in white, black, and American Indian children with comparable iron status: evidence to support uniform diag- nostic criteria for anemia among all races. American Journal of Diseases of Children 138:824-27. Yip, R.; Binkin, N.J.; Fleshood, L.; and Trowbridge, F.L. 1987. Declining prevalence of anemia among low-income children in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association 258:1619-23. Yip, R.; Walsh, K.M.; Goldforb, M.G.; and Binkin, N.J. 1987. Declining prevalence of anemia in childhood in a middle-class setting: a pediatric success story? Pediatrics 80:330-34. 489 q Chapter 13 Neurologic Disorders The man is, above all else, the mind of the man, and not only the mind as an organ of conscious thought but the mind as an organ of bodily nutrition. . . . James J. Putnam Boston Medical and Surgical Journal (1899) Introduction Historical Perspective Since biblical times, brain function and certain nutritional factors have been thought to be related, but knowledge of the interaction between nutrition and neurology is still sparse. Scientific interest in the link between nutrition and brain function began in the early 19th century when Magendie proposed that the central nervous system controlled appetite (Anand 1961, Widdowson 1982). Due to the predominance of theories that hunger was purely a peripheral sensation (Cannon and Washburn 1912), it was not until the 1930’s that control of feeding began to be attributed to specific regions of the brain. Classically, nutritional deficiencies have also been related to brain func- tion. Starvation was shown to produce a variety of overt behavioral changes as well as mental deterioration (Blanton 1919), and severe mal- nutrition in animals is now associated with impaired brain development and learning (Winick 1976). The absence of specific vitamins or minerals from the diet has been known for many years to produce behavioral or neu- rologic symptoms in animals (Funk 191 1). In the early 20th century, disor- ders of the human nervous system were observed as consequences of deficiencies of certain vitamins, copper, and magnesium in the diet (Wid- dowson 1972). In most industrialized societies, however, only a few nutri- tional deficiency diseases, such as those associated with alcoholism (see chapter on alcohol), occur with sufficient frequency to be of substantial concern to policymakers and the public. 491 ie Nutrition and Health Because the brain appears to be protected by the blood-brain barrier from fluctuations in plasma nutrient levels caused by eating, it has been difficult to demonstrate effects of specific foods or dietary components on brain function. Furthermore, the nutrient transport and cellular uptake mecha- nisms as well as the factors affecting the nutrient content of the brain are not fully understood (Pardridge 1986). In laboratory animals, brain levels of certain essential constituents have been reported to vary markedly as a function of the nature and timing of food consumption (Pardridge 1977: Wurtman, Hefti, and Melamed 1981; Blusztajn and Wurtman 1983), but whether human neurotransmitter systems are affected by the intake of dietary precursors (Wurtman, Wurtman, and Growdon 1981b; Blusztajn and Wurtman 1983) is as yet uncertain. This chapter reviews these and other nutritional aspects of neurologic phenomena, and because cerebrovascular diseases account for sucha large proportion of neurologic cases and hospital admissions, it also reviews the relationship between diet and stroke. Significance for Public Health Stroke is the most common life-threatening neurologic disease in the United States: It ranks third after heart disease and cancer as a cause of death and is also a major cause of long-term disability (NCHS 1986). Although stroke mortality rates have declined dramatically in recent years (see chapter on high blood pressure), in part as a result of improved high blood pressure management, about 500,000 new cases still occur annually. About 2 million Americans are estimated to suffer from stroke-related disabilities at an annual cost of more than $11 billion (NINCDS 1986). The mortality rate among black Americans is almost twice that of whites. Because a significant portion of the important risk factors for stroke are under behavioral control, efforts to reduce these risks seem to be especial- ly worthwhile. Other neurologic conditions are also prevalent in the U.S. population. Approximately 2 million Americans suffer from epilepsy, at an estimated health care cost of $3 billion. Chronic headaches afflict 40 million people in the United States and are responsible for 8 million annual visits to a physician and 64 million days of work lost due to pain. Alzheimer’s disease affects 2 to 3 million Americans, at an estimated annual health care cost of up to $50 billion (NINCDS 1986). Whether any appreciable fraction of these conditions can be prevented or ameliorated by dietary means is unknown but seems unlikely given current evidence. 492 Neurologic Disorders @| Scientific Background Nutritional Needs of the Nervous System The brain and nervous system probably require the full complement of essential nutrients and energy to develop and to maintain their neurons and supporting cells. Although it seems reasonable to expect that a deficiency of any one of the essential nutrients or of energy would impair the develop- ment and subsequent maintenance of these structurally and functionally complex tissues, evidence to support this hypothesis has been difficult to obtain (Shoemaker and Bloom 1977; Nowak and Munro 1977). The human brain normally metabolizes 100 to 150 g of glucose per day as fuel, but the mature nervous system is relatively insensitive to restrictions of energy and protein. During starvation, it adapts and uses ketones, derived from breakdown of body fat stores, for energy and thus spares blood glucose and conserves body protein. The documented behavioral changes that occur in starving individuals—depression, apathy, irritability, and loss of libido—do not necessarily reflect damage to the nervous system (Kerndt et al. 1982). Most direct research on the neurologic effects of nutritional deprivation has been performed on experimental animals. In part because of species vari- ability in the time course of neurologic development, the results of this research may not be applicable to humans. Additional information on the effects of dietary deficiencies on human brain development and nervous system function derives from “natural experiments” of human starvation or inadequate dietary intake. With a few notable exceptions, it has been difficult to separate the effects of nutritional deficits in these situations from those of other environmental, social, and medical problems that often accompany poor nutritional status (Pollitt and Thomson 1977). Nonetheless, severe deficiencies of vitamins, especially the B-complex group, impair nervous system function (Dreyfus 1988; Lipton, Mailman, and Nemeroff 1979). Thiamin deficiency causes beriberi neuropathy as well as a peripheral neuropathy and polyneuritis that leads to Wernicke-Kor- sakoff’s syndrome (paralysis of the eye muscles, loss of muscular coordina- tion, and memory loss) in long-term alcoholics (Dreyfus 1988; see chapter on alcohol). Inadequate niacin intake causes pellagra, with symptoms that include intellectual impairment and dementia, in individuals of all ages. Deficiency caused by vitamin B,> malabsorption in untreated pernicious 493 ie Nutrition and Health anemia or as a result of a long-term vitamin B ,, deficient vegetarian diet can result in subacute degeneration of the spinal cord, optic nerves, cerebral white matter, and peripheral nerves (Dreyfus 1988). Severely deficient intakes of other vitamins of the B-complex group also affect neurologic function. In the early stages, these symptoms are readily overcome by increased dietary intake of the appropriate vitamin, but nerve damage in later stages appears to be irreversible (Lipton, Mailman, and Nemeroff 1979). Whether subclinical deficiencies of these vitamins affect nervous system function is as yet uncertain (Goodwin, Goodwin, and Gary 1983). Deficiencies of other nutrients may relate to defects in nervous system function. For example, iodine deficiency during brain development causes mental retardation and neuromotor abnormalities. Chronic iron deficiency is associated with deficits in cognitive abilities (Lozoff et al. 1987). A few cases of vitamin E deficiency-induced spinal cord, cerebellar, and periph- eral nerve degeneration with muscle wasting have been reported among patients with severe cholestatic liver disease or malabsorption syndromes (Muller, Lloyd, and Wolff 1983; Laplante et al. 1984; Weder et al. 1984). Levels of vitamin E are greatly reduced in the peripheral nerves of such patients (Traber et al. 1987). In most cases, neurologic damage resolves with administration of vitamin E (Sokol et al. 1985), suggesting that vitamin E deficiency may be responsible. The effects of nutritional deficiencies on the developing nervous system are difficult to interpret. Studies in rats have almost always restricted total dietary intake rather than that of specific nutrients. These studies have indicated that nutritional deprivation at various stages of fetal and postnatal development produces its own constellation of defects. Early maternal malnutrition reduces fetal brain weight and brain protein and DNA con- tent. Postnatal malnutrition also induces deficits in brain weight and, in addition, reduces myelination and dendrite formation and retards develop- ment of mature enzyme patterns (Shoemaker and Bloom 1977). These changes persist even after adequate nutrition is restored (Nowak and Munro 1977; Winick 1976). In experimental studies of pregnant animals, diets deficient in zinc and folate result in high rates of malformations such as neural tube defects and spina bifida in the offspring ( Hurley and Shrader 1972; see chapter on maternal and child nutrition). Supplementation of these animals resulted in a significant reduction in the incidence of these congenital nervous system defects. Studies in animals have shown that the neuropathology resulting from intrauterine starvation or postnatal malnutrition does not produce recog- nizable focal lesions but instead produces diffuse anatomical effects that 494 Neurologic Disorders e appear to be spread throughout the brain. Different effects occur in differ- ent brain regions depending on the rate of cell division within each region. Because changes of this type provide little information about functional connections, the significance of these changes for cognitive and behavioral function is uncertain (Nowak and Munro 1977). The relevance of animal studies for human brain development and cog- nitive functioning is also uncertain. Reduced cellularity and myelination have been shown to occur in the brains of severely nutritionally deprived human infants. Malnutrition during and after pregnancy produces human infants with lower birth weights, smaller stature, and, therefore, smailer head circumferences and lower brain weights. None of these changes, however, has been demonstrated to affect higher mental processes (Dob- bing 1984). Dietary Precursors of Brain Neurotransmitters Nervous system function is mediated through the work of various neu- rotransmitters such as serotonin, the catecholamines (dopamine and nor- epinephrine), and acetylcholine. These neurotransmitters are manufac- tured by the action of enzymes in the brain on the precursor amino acids tryptophan, tyrosine, and choline, respectively. Because the brain cannot make adequate quantities of the various precursors, it must rely on uptake from the bloodstream. Studies in animals have suggested that meal com- position can affect plasma levels of the precursors and, therefore, synthesis of these neurotransmitters (Wurtman, Hefti, and Melamed 1981; Pardridge 1977). Whether the various high dose dietary contributions to precursors of brain neurotransmitters affect neurologic function in animals other than the rat is uncertain (Trulson 1985). The effects of dietary precursors of neu- rotransmitters on animal function and behavior are also uncertain (see chapter on behavior). Also unknown is whether dietary precursors of neurotransmitters can alter normal human behavior (Spring 1986; Young 1986) or correct abnormal behavior (Growdon 1979a; Van Praag and Lemus 1986). Clinical investigators have tested the utility of neurotransmitter precursors as treatments for diseases associated with neurotransmitter deficiencies such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, depression, and sleep disor- ders (Growdon 1981; Growdon and Wurtman 1982; Growdon and Gibson 1982; Young 1986; Van Praag and Lemus 1986). There is no evidence that such conditions result from nutritional deficiencies. Thus, tryptophan, tyrosine, and choline (as such or as phosphatidylcholine) were given in 495 ie Nutrition and Health these studies in purified form independent of other food constituents. In the case of choline, doses were greater than would ordinarily be consumed in the normal diet. Pharmacologic amounts of L-tryptophan, for example, have been adminis- tered to human subjects to induce sleep (Hartmann 1982), treat insomnia in older persons (Jenicke 1985), relieve anxiety and suppress food consump- tion in obese persons (Wurtman, Hefti, and Melamed 1981), suppress posthypoxic myoclonus (Growdon 1979b), and treat certain pain syn- dromes (Hosobuchi, Lamb, and Bascom 1980; Lieberman et al. 1982; King 1980). However, the effectiveness of tryptophan for these purposes has not been established, and its use remains controversial (DeFeudis 1987). Simi- larly, tyrosine, the dietary precursor of the neurotransmitters dopamine and norephinephrine, has been used to accelerate dopamine turnover in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Growdon and Melamed 1982) and to treat patients with mild Parkinson’s disease (Growdon 1981) or endogenous depression (Van Praag and Lemus 1986); however, these applications are also preliminary and require further confirmation. Large doses of choline and purified phosphatidylcholine, precursors for the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, have been reported to improve the symptoms of patients with tardive dyskinesia (Growdon and Wurtman 1982) and mania (Cohen et al. 1980). These observations remain to be confirmed. Similar treatment of patients with Huntington’s disease, Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome, familial ataxias, and epilepsy has produced negative or inconclusive results (Wood and Allison 1982). Despite a single report that suggests that the administration of phos- phatidylcholine for at least 6 months might retard the progression of Alzheimer’s disease in a subset of older patients with this disorder (Little et al. 1985), more than a dozen additional studies have found choline to be ineffective in the treatment of this condition (Bartus et al. 1982). Evidence is also lacking for the hypothesis that Alzheimer’s disease is a consequence of the breakdown of phosphatidylcholine in neuronal membranes to pro- duce choline for acetylcholine biosynthesis (Blusztajn and Wurtman 1983). Key Scientific Issues ® Role of Diet in Cerebrovascular Disease (Stroke) ® Role of Diet in Other Neurologic Disorders ®@ Role of Noncaloric Dietary Components in Neurologic Disorders 496 Neurologic Disorders O Role of Diet in Cerebrovascular Disease (Stroke) Stroke is the sudden loss of brain function caused by one of four vascular events: thrombosis, or blood clot, in a cerebral artery; embolism, or blockage, of a cerebral artery by a circulating clot; stenosis, or narrowing, of a cerebral artery by atherosclerosis; or hemorrhage from rupture of a ~ cerebral artery. These events deprive the brain of blood and oxygen and cause tissue death and irreversible damage to nervous tissue. The effects of the damage depend on the location and size of the tissue loss. Symptoms range from those too trivial for the victim to notice to major sensory deficits, blindness, paralysis, speech loss, coma, and death. Although some return of function is possible in tissues damaged but not destroyed, losses persisting beyond weeks are likely to be permanent. Persons at greatest risk for stroke are those with hypertension and diabetes _ and those who smoke cigarettes and display impaired cardiac function due to coronary heart disease, congestive heart failure, or hypertensive heart disease. These major risk factors for stroke are in part related to nutri- tional, dietary, and lifestyle factors. Current evidence suggests that certain dietary substances that promote high blood pressure. or diabetes may increase the likelihood of stroke. Thus, excessive dietary consumption of | calories (if it results in obesity), sodium, and alcohol may increase the risk for stroke. The evidence that links dietary factors to high blood pressure and diabetes is reviewed in detail in the chapters devoted to those topics. Although there is a wealth of evidence supporting the relationship between diet, blood cholesterol levels, and atherosclerotic coronary disease (see chapter on coronary heart disease), the link to cerebrovascular disease is less clear. Persons with hypertension are at greatly increased risk for stroke. This risk increases with elevations in blood pressure regardless of age and sex; it decreases when blood pressure is reduced. How hypertension predisposes to stroke is not fully understood, but it appears to injure cerebral artery walls. Diabetes and high blood cholesterol may also act, alone or in concert, to injure the inner wall of a cerebral artery. Through consequent platelet interaction, the injury becomes the site of plaque formation and subsequent atherosclerosis. Thus, hypertension seems to be the precursor of the hemorrhagic, the stenotic, and the embolic types of stroke. Moderate sodium restriction (Koolen and Van Brummelen 1984; Kawasaki etal. 1978), high potassium intake (Treasure and Ploth 1983; Langford 1983, Kaplan et al. 1985), vegetarian diets (Ophir et al. 1983), calcium (Blaustein 497 e Nutrition and Health and Hamlyn 1983; McCarron et al. 1984), weight reduction, and alcohol restriction all have been suggested as factors associated with blood pres- sure lowering. Of these, weight reduction and restriction of sodium have the most evidence to substantiate this association, but excessive consumption of alcohol is associated with increased rates of hypertension (Klatsky et al. 1977; MacMahon and Norton 1986; Potter and Beevers 1984). Epi- demiologic evidence associates excessive drinking with increased frequen- cy of subarachnoid hemorrhage and cerebral infarction (Hillbom and Kaste 1978, 1982), and heavy alcohol intake has been identified as an independent risk factor for stroke in humans (Gill et al. 1986). The effect of dietary potassium on stroke has also been investigated re- cently. In a prospective study, individuals consuming somewhat higher levels of potassium from food sources had a reduced risk for stroke- associated mortality (Khaw and Barrett-Connor 1987). The effects of spe- cific dietary factors on causation of stroke warrant further investigation. Role of Diet in Other Neurologic Disorders Headache Headache, one of the most common complaints evaluated by neurologists, is estimated to affect 40 million Americans. Clinical investigations of the role of foods in precipitating vascular or migraine headaches remain con- troversial (Dreyfus 1988). The foods most frequently implicated often contain tyramines (e.g., cheese, red wines) or more rarely phenylethyl- amine (e.g., chocolates) (Dreyfus 1988; Egger et al. 1983). The “Chinese Restaurant Syndrome” is associated with numbness around the mouth, tingling, flushing of the face, dizziness, and headache (Kenny 1980). Use of the artificial sweetener aspartame has been found not to be associated with increased susceptibility to headache (Schiffman et al. 1987). Current evidence suggests that dietary factors are unlikely to be responsi- ble for most cases of headache, although toxicity of vitamin A, caused by excess intake of supplements, has been reported to cause headaches. Until conclusive evidence has established the link between certain foods and the occurrence of headaches, especially migraine, it may be prudent for such individuals to abstain whenever possible from those foods thought to provoke an attack (Dreyfus 1988). Epilepsy Low levels of magnesium can cause seizures, and the magnesium-deficient rat is used as a model of experimental epilepsy (Buck, Mahoney, and 498 Neurologic Disorders C Hendricks 1978). Magnesium deficiency in humans most often results from kidney disease and is not a significant cause of epilepsy in people. A wide variety of neurobehavioral symptoms common in the general population, including epilepsy, have been reported to occur in people consuming the artificial sweetener aspartame (Wurtman 1985). No well-controlled clinical studies have indicated that a causal relationship exists. Ketogenic diets (low in carbohydrate) have been used for decades to treat certain forms of intractable epilepsy. The mechanism of action of this diet is not understood (Withrow 1980), and it is tolerated poorly by some patients (Trauner 1985). Furthermore, it is nutritionally inadequate, unpalatable, and inappropriate for use in young children. Other specific nutrients such as vitamin D,; have been suggested to have beneficial effects on seizure thresholds in rats (Siegel et al. 1984), but more information is needed before conclusions can be drawn about the role of diet in epilepsy. Role of Noncaloric Dietary Components in Neurologic Disorders Excessive Vitamin Intake Vitamin A intoxication can result from the chronic use of relatively low levels (14,000 IU/day in infants and 25,000 to 50,000 [U/day in adults) of the vitamin (Farris and Erdman 1982), and toxic symptoms may appear sud- denly with the onset of liver dysfunction due to other causes (Hatoff et al. 1982). Excessive intake of vitamin A causes reversible intracranial hyper- tension, which when not so identified has led to needless surgery; it can also occasionally result in headache, blurred vision, seizures, and encepha- lopathy. High doses of pyridoxine (vitamin B,) have been used to treat conditions such as carpal tunnel syndrome; however, excessive pyridoxine in gram quantities has recently been associated with peripheral nerve deterioration, and more recent studies indicate that lesser amounts may produce toxic symptoms (Ditmars and Houin 1986; Dreyfus 1988, Schaum- berg et al. 1983). Endogenous and Exogenous Toxins Naturally occurring food-borne toxins either consumed in the diet or produced by the body as a result of rare metabolic diseases also affect the mature nervous system; brain damage resulting from lung, liver, or kidney failure is a common example. Moreover, rare metabolic diseases or inborn errors of metabolism can cause naturally occurring food constituents to become toxic (e.g., copper in Wilson’s disease, phenylalanine in phen- ylketonuria) (Dreyfus 1988). The seeds of certain cycad plants contain neurotoxic substances that, when ingested by experimental animals, cause symptoms similar to those of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), parkin- 499 ie Nutrition and Health sonism, or Alzheimer’s disease (Spencer et al. 1987). However, in man, the role of cycad plants and other dietary factors in ALS is still controversial (Garruto and Yase 1986). Other neurotoxins have caused acute poisonings on those rare occasions when they have contaminated foods in large quantities. These include ingestion of lead-contaminated paint, soil, or food and water (Mahaffey 1981); industrial contamination of shellfish with methyl mercury; and paralytic poisons produced by certain shellfish (Hat- ten et al. 1983). Specific dietary constituents, such as heavy and trace metals, may have especially adverse effects on the nervous systems of older adults. For example, increased amounts of aluminum and calcium have been reported in brains of patients with Alzheimer’s disease, and concentrations are especially high in the damaged neurons of the cortex (Linton et al. 1987), particularly in association with the senile plaques and tangles. Whether aluminum causes the cellular damage or accumulates simply because the cells are dying has not been established (see chapter on aging). Food Additives Numerous compounds added to foods can affect the nervous system. Some investigators have reported that very high levels of the dipeptide sweetener aspartame raise brain levels of tyrosine in rats and, as a conse- quence, impair synthesis of catecholamine neurotransmitters (Yokogoshi et al. 1984; Pardridge 1977), but others have not been able to identify such effects (Potts, Bloss, and Nutting 1980; Torii et al. 1986). The rat, unlike humans, rapidly converts phenylalanine (a component of aspartame) to tyrosine. Until aspartame has been tested in well-controlled human clinical studies, its effects on the nervous system remain speculative. Drug-Nutrient Interactions Some of the drugs used to treat neurologic diseases can lead to vitamin deficiencies by changing the metabolism of the vitamins, causing a second- ary impairment of brain function (see chapter on drug-nutrient interac- tions). Dilantin, used to treat epilepsy, can increase folate requirements and cause vitamin K deficiency in the infants of mothers treated with this drug. In addition to the hypertensive medication effects on mineral metabolism described in the chapter on drug-nutrient interactions, hydralazine is a vitamin B, antagonist that can cause peripheral neuropathy. Tranquilizers such as chlorpromazine and other phenothiazines may cause hyperphagia and weight gain. Monoamine oxidase inhibitors can cause acute hyperten- sive crises, including excruciating headaches or fatal intracranial hemor- 500