ESSENTIALS OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY ESSENTIALS OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BY EMORY Ph.D. PROFESSOR OF SOCIOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN CALIFORNIA AUTHOR OF “INTRODUCTION TO SOCIOLOGY” UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN CALIFORNIA PRESS LOS ANGELES Copyright 1918, University of Southern California Press University of Southern California Press Los Angeles DEDICATED TO EDITH PRITCHARD BOGARDUS CONTENTS I. Introduction ..... 13 (1) The field of social psychology (2) The methods to be pursued (3) The leading authorities and books II. Psychological Bases of Social Psychology . 22 (1) Instinctive bases (a) The nature and social function of instinct (2) Habitual bases (a) The nature and social uses ol habit (3) Conscious bases (a) The affective phase of consciousness (b) The cognitive nature of consciousness (c) The volitional characteristic of conscious- ness. III. The Social Characteristics of the Individual 38 (1) The Social Instincts (a) The gregarious, sex and parental, curi- osity, combative, acquisitive, and play instincts (2) Sympathy, the emotions, the sentiments, char- acter (3) The social self, the looking-glass self 8 Contents IV. The Social Characteristics of the Individual (Continued) ... 59 (4) Language—a social product (5) Laughter—a social instrument (6) Suggestion—two forms, direct and indirect (7) Imitation—function and types V. The Social Operation of Imitation . . 74 (1) Contemporary imitation—conventionality and fashion imitation (a) Fashion imitation—five causes (2) Custom imitation—laws of operation (3) Rational and socio-rational imitation VI. The Psychology of the Group . . 92 (1) Permanent groups—types and functions (2) Temporary groups—crowds, mobs, assemblies, publics (a) The psychology of the crowd (3) Group conflicts—nature and social function (a) Discussion—a specialized form of conflict (4) Group and social consciousness (a) The rise of social co-operation (b) Group loyalty, patriotism, internationalism VII. The Psychology of Invention and of Leadership 116 (1) The psychology of invention (2) Types of leadership (3) Qualities of leadership VIII. The Psychology of Social Control and of Social Progress . . . 132 (1) The factors in social control (a) Public opinion—strength and weakness (2) The nature of social progress (a) A sociocratic theory of progress PREFACE This book is written specifically for the purpose of developing the “problem-getting” method of education. To this end, the main attention has been given to the formulation of the “problems” which appear in connec- tion with the theme or themes of the chapters. Each of the “problems” has been tried out in the classroom and found productive of constructive thought on the part of students. These exercises are intended to set the student at work, and to stimulate him to do his own thinking. The student who has an introductory acquaintance with social psychology should begin the study of each chapter with the problems. To these problems, he should first seek answers in his own experiences and observations; then he may inquire of others; he may look for further hints in the context of each chapter, and search through the findings of the specialists whose related works are cited in the lists of selected references at the close of the chapters. If the student has an inad- equate background for giving his attention first to the exercises, he may read the context, not as an end in 10 Social Psychology itself, but as a method of preparation for attacking the problems. The context of each chapter should not be “remembered,” but utilized as a means of finding an- swers, seeing new relationships, and making new dis- coveries. If the student comes into the class-room remembering, this book is intended to send him out thinking. The second aim of the author has been to write a treatise which would meet the needs of the under- graduate student in colleges, junior colleges, and nor- mal schools. The subject of social psychology is of such vital, far-reaching, and practical importance that every college student should be introduced to a scientific con- sideration of the field. Every such student is com- pelled to study the psychology of the individual; but few are required or even encouraged to study the psychology of the interactions of individuals in their multifarious group relationships. Surely the latter phenomena are of as vital importance as the former. A third need which this book aims to meet arises in connection with the method of organizing the sub- ject-matter of social psychology. To some writers, social psychology consists chiefly of a study of the social nature and the social activities of the individual; to other authors, the subject consists largely of an analysis of the psychic interactions of the members of groups. The first emphasis is essentially subjective, genetic, psy- chological; the second is chiefly objective and sociol- logical. Preface 11 But the new science of social psychology must de- velop its own methodology and speak from its own vantage ground. Its sector of the field of the social sciences is that important territory where the activi- ties of psychology and sociology overlap. Instead of allowing its advance to be directed from either psycho- logical or sociological headquarters, it must develop its own methods and programs, but remain subject, of course, to the rules of scientific and of social science procedure. It is true that according to another view, social psychology has no distinct field and must be either psychology, or sociology; but the probabilities are that time will prove this conception to have been a mis- taken one. It is the plan of this book, and of the courses in social psychology which have preceded the writing thereof, to begin the discussion with the psychological bases of social psychology, to analyze the social charac- teristics of the individual, to consider the social opera- tion of these characteristics, to study the group, the types of groups, and the nature of group conflicts, to investi- gate the psychology of leadership, as well as the psychol- ogy of social control, and to close with an analysis of world progress. The method is inductive, evolutionary, cumulative; it moves from the particular to the general, from the individual to the group, and from the group to mankind, and it culminates in the subject of social progress. 12 Social Psychology The writer is indebted to so many authors that it is impossible to make adequate acknowledgments. The interest of the writer in the subject was awakened by Professor G. H. Mead; the books and syllabi which have been the most helpful are those of Professors McDougall, Tarde, Ross, Howard, Baldwin, and Ell- wood. For the stimulus to develop the “problem-get- ting” method of teaching and for encouragement in the preparation of the manuscript, I am indebted to Dr. E. C. Moore. For many of the problems that are given at the close of each chapter, I am under obligation to various persons, but chiefly Professor Ross and my ad- vanced students. Sometimes a re-phrasing of a quota- tion or quoted exercise has been necessary, in which case it has not been feasible to use quotation marks and thus to indicate my indebtedness. The encouragement and suggestions of Professor George Elliott Howard, who has read the manuscript, are gratefully acknowledged. Emory S. Bogardus University of Southern California February 21, 1918 ESSENTIALS OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Chapter I INTRODUCTION I. Social psychology as at present considered is based upon the facts and principles of general psychology. Upon these fundamental data must the foundations of social psychology be laid. It is necessary, first of all, therefore, to consider the characteristics of the human mind in action. This field includes an understanding of the nature and types of the instinctive, of the habitual, and of the conscious reactions of the human mind. The conscious reactions of individuals, upon analysis, are seen to have their affective, cognitive, and volitional phases. An important group of instinctive reactions, namely, the social instincts, comprises so basic a factor in the study of social psychology that it calls for special atten- tion. As examples of the social instincts, there are the gregarious, the sex and parental, the curiosity instincts. Then there are other closely related social phases of the life of the individual which invite examination, 14 Social Psychology such as sympathy, the emotions, the sentiments, char- acter, the social self, the “looking-glass self,” language, laughter, suggestion, and imitation. The psychology of group life, as such, the classifica- tion of groups, the conflicts between groups, the psy- chology of the crowd and of the public are vital phases of social psychology. The outstanding features of group life and group conflicts are leadership and social control. It is the individual who initiates, who works over the ideas of his time into new and advanced forms; it is the group which appropriates and adopts the inven- tions of the few. It is the individual who stimulates the group; it is the group which chokes off the initiative of the individual. The unanswered question arises: How much and what kind of control shall the group exer- cise in order that the individual members may co-ordi- nate themselves and function as one brain, and in order that the group may progress ? Social psychology is the scientific study of the social nature and reactions of the mind, of the interactions of individuals within groups, of group conflicts, of group leadership and control, and of the nature of group and societary progress. Social psychology ap- proaches the problems of life from the psychological viewpoint; it draws conclusions and offers programs with reference to societary ends. Social psychology studies the social phases of personality, the interactions of personalities within groups, and the nature of group control and progress. Introduction 15 II. Social psychology lends itself to the “problem-get- ting” method of study. The student must not assume a memorizing attitude, but a problem-getting method of approach. He must have “problems”, or targets. He can become an efficient student only by keeping problems, or targets, constantly before his mind. He must read, not for the purpose of memorizing, but in order to find answers and solutions. If he has no problems in mind, his reading, or even his so-called studying, is practically of little value. As no one can develop skill as a marksman except by aiming at targets in his practice work, so no student can acquire thorough methods, for example, in social psychology, except by keeping problems, or targets, constantly before his mind. Who is more foolish than a would-be marksman who spends hour after hour shooting in all directions but at no particular object, or target? Target-hitting is the worth-while achievement in marksmanship, and problem-solving is the valuable goal in studying.1 For this reason, exercises have been formulated and given prominence at the end of each chapter. The plan involves a study of the problems first of all. The student is expected to search his own mind, his own experience, and the experiences of others, for solutions of the given problems. Then the subject matter of 1 Moore, E. C., What is Education, Ch. VIII. 16 Social Psychology the respective chapters may be consulted for securing further light, and finally the readings at the close of each chapter will afford additional help. III. The literature on social psychology is so extensive that it may be grouped under certain heads. Only representative and leading books in each group can be mentioned. The student will need to refer fre- quently to the representative works in psychology proper, such as the psychologies which bear the names of James, Royce, Titchener, Thorndike, Angell, Pills- bury, and others. In social psychology, there are several well known books. McDougall’s Introduction to Social Psychology affords the student a fundamental discussion of the nature of the social instincts and of their functioning in society. Ross’ Social Psychology is widely used as a text. While it does not inquire into the social nature of the individual, nor into the genesis of social attitudes of individuals, it enters extensively into a discussion of the operation of suggestion and imitation in society. Custom imitation, conventionality imita- tion, and fashion imitation, are central themes. Where Mr. McDougall concludes his analysis, Professor Ross begins; their two books have little in common except the title. Professor Ross’ Social Control is undoubtedly the best book upon the subject that is stated in the Introduction 17 title, and constitutes an excellent supplement to his Social Psychology. Human Nature and the Social Order and Social Organization by Charles H. Cooley should be studied in order, because together they form a well-balanced system of thought in social psychology. The first cen- ters about the individual and his social nature; the sec- ond about the nature of group-life and of group con- trol through the organization of the members of the group. Gabriel Tarde’s Laws of Imitation explains at con- siderable length the operation of imitation in society. In Social Laws, Tarde gives a succinct summary of his main ideas concerning the psychical processes going on in society. His La logique sociale, L’opinion et la foule, and related works are of first rank. Psychical Interpretations of Society by Michael M. Davis, Jr. re- views historically and critically the literature of a socio- psychological nature, and summarizes and criticizes Tarde’s theory of imitation. Professor J. Mark Bald- win has written Social and Ethical Interpretations, which presents a fundamental analysis of the nature and characteristics of the social self from the genetic point of view. The best syllabus (including the most comprehensive bibliography) for the study of social psychology is that prepared by Professor George Elliott Howard. Professor Charles A. Ellwood in his Sociology in its Psychological Aspects and in the recent Introduction 18 Social Psychology to Social Psychology has given a careful and synthetic statement of the nature and function of the leading psychic elements operating in social progress. From a point of view somewhat philosophic and socialistic, The Great Society by Graham Wallas, gives keen analy- ses of social processes. In The Crowd and The Psychology of the Great War, well-known books by Gustave LeBon, the reader needs to be prepared for an exaggerated form of crowd and mob psychology. Re- lated works are those by Scipio Sighele. A new field of social psychology is that which treats of the psychologi- cal explanations of social origins; the work of Professor W. I. Thomas in Sex and Society and in the Source Book for Social Origins is an outstanding example. Hobhouse’s Morals in Evolution and Sumner’s Folk- ways may also be mentioned in this connection. For further references, the reader may turn to the end of each chapter and to the selected list which is given in the closing pages of this book.2 2 After this manuscript was in the hands of the printer, Volume XII of the Publications of the American Sociological Society was published. It is entitled Social Control. The volume contains thirteen papers dealing with the history and problems of social control; it is an indispensable docu- ment for the social psychologist. It was planned by Pro- fessor George Elliott Howard and was edited by Professor Scott E. W. Bedford. Introduction 19 PROBLEMS 1. At the outset of this study, what meaning does the term “social psychology” have to you? 2. What is the relation of the psychological phases of sociology to social psychology ? 3. Which is the more important for the study of social psychology, a knowledge of psychology or of sociology ? , 4. Distinguish between individual psychology and social psychology. 5. Which is the more useful, the study of individ- ual psychology or the study of social psychology? 6. Explain: “The older psychology was individu- alistic in its interpretations.” 7. Why has the American been primarily an indi- vidualist? 8. Is the American youth today more of an indi- vidualist than his father? Why? 9. Distinguish between racial psychology and social psychology. 10. Is social psychology an old or new subject? 11. Explain the recent rapid development of social psychology. 12. What meaning do you see in the terms “indi- vidual ascendency” and “social ascendency”? 13. Would an abnormal development of either “individual ascendency” or “social ascendency” be good for a community ? Why ? 20 Social Psychology 14. When do you feel of greater importance,—on a mountain alone, or in a crowd? 15. What aim may one have in studying social psychology ? 16. What constitutes the laboratory of the student of social psychology? 17. Do you expect that the study of social psychol- ogy will make you more dependent upon others, or more independent of others? 18. Who are the founders of modern social psy- chology? READINGS Angell, J. R., Chapters from Modern Psychology, Ch. VI. Baldwin, J. M., The Story of the Mind, Ch. I. The Individual and Society, Ch. I. Blackmar and Gillin, Outlines of Sociology, Pt. Ill, Chs. IV-VI. Bogardus, E. S., Introduction to Sociology, Ch. XIII. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Ch. I. Dealey, J. Q., Sociology, Chs. IV, XV. Dewey, John, “The Need for Social Psychology,” Psycholog- ical Rev., July, 1917, 264-77. Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Ch. VI. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. I. Gault, R. H., “Psychology in Social Relations,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXII: 734-48. Giddings, F. H., Democracy and Empire, Ch. III. Hall, G. S., “Social Phases of Psychology,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVIII: 613-21. Hayes, E. C., Introduction to the Study of Sociology, Ch. XVII. Hobhouse, L. T., Mind in Evolution, Ch. I. Introduction 21 Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sect. I. Leuba, J. H., “Psychology and Sociology,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XIX: 323-42. “Methods and Principles of Social Psychology,” Psychological Bui., XIV: 367-74. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. I. Psychology, Ch. VIII. Maciver, R., “What is Social Psychology,” Sociological Rev., VI :147-60. Mead, G. H., “Social Psychology as a Counterpart to Phy- siological Psychology,” Psychological Bui., VI: 401-08. Nearing, S., Social Sanity, pp. 11-42. Orano, P., Psicologia sociale, pp. 9-114. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Ch. I. Sighele, S., La foule criminelle, pp. 1-22. Smith, W. R., An Introduction to Educational Sociology, Ch. II. Tarde, G., La logique sociale, Ch. II. Thomas, W. I., “The Province of Social Psychology”, Amer. Jour, of Sociol., X: 445-55. Tosti, G., “Social Psychology and Sociology,” Psychological Rev., V: 347-81. Wallas, G., The Great Society, Ch. II. Chapter II PSYCHOLOGICAL BASES OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY I. The study of social psychology is based on a knowl- edge of psychological principles. It is apropos that attention be given here to the more important facts of psychology which are essential to an understanding of our field. Psychology may be divided into two branches, struc- tural and functional. The former treats chiefly of the states of consciousness, while the latter describes the mind in action. It is in functional psychology that the social psychologist is directly interested. Func- tional psychology furnishes the principles for interpret- ing the social nature of individuals and for understand- ing the interactions in group life. The reactions.of the mind may be divided into three general classes, namely, (1) instinctive, (2) habitual, and (3) conscious. Instinctive tendencies are based on ready-made, inborn co-ordinations. They represent psychical acquisitions which have been biologically trans- Psychological Bases 23 mitted. They have been described by Mr. McDougall as representing pre-formed pathways in the nervous sys- tem and as having been made in response to the demands of previous life-conditions.1 They have been slowly evolved in the process of adaptation of species to en- vironment. In every case, according to Mr. McDougall, some sense-impression or combination of sense-impressions ex- cites a perfectly definite behavior or some movement or train of movements. The result is the same in all indi- viduals of the species and on all similar occasions. In general, the behavior thus caused promotes (1) the welfare of the individual, (2) perpetuates the species, or (3) advances the welfare of the given group. These innate or inherited tendencies are the essen- tial springs or motive powers of all thought and action, whether individual or collective.2 They are the founda- tions upon which character is developed. All that we learn and all of our mastery of life is constructed upon the basis of our instincts. Our later adaptations are modifications of these original, inherited reactions. The instinctive tendencies are also at the root of our social life. The social interactions between individuals, no matter how complex these interactions may become, rest upon the instincts. All social structures and insti- tutions have their beginnings in instincts. Back of the 1An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. II. 2Ibid., p. 44. 24 Social Psychology family as a social institution are the sex and parental impulses; back of fraternal organizations is the gre- garious instinct. II. The failure of an instinct to function successfully in a new situation leads to the appearance of conscious- ness and the re-construction of the instinctive way of acting. The modification may be slight, or almost en- tirely new. These modifications when repeated become habitual. Habits both new and old, likewise may be disturbed, and through the action of consciousness may be made over. Habits are modifications of instincts or of previously formed habits. The concept of “crisis”, as used by Professor W. I. Thomas, is a useful tool in in this connection; it refers to the disturbance of an established way of doing, the concentration of atten- tion, and the development of a new method as a re- sultant. The actions of the lower forms of animal life are mostly tropistic, reflex, or instinctive. Higher animals adapt, within narrow limits, their instinctive actions to peculiar circumstances. Man has modified his instincts so completely that they operate almost entirely in hid- den ways. It is our privilege as human beings deliberately to modify our instincts and old habits, and to build up new habits which will make us masters of ourselves and, in a sense, of our environment. Within limits, an Psychological Bases 25 individual normally situated can acquire habits in al- most any direction of growth that he wills. Habitual reactions are of distinct advantage to the individual in several ways.3 (1) They reduce the necessary time of action. (2) They increase accuracy. (3) They reduce the attendant fatigue. (4) They re- lease the mind from the necessity of paying attention to the details of the successive steps of an act. (5) They give a permanency to experience. In other words, the custom of deprecating habits is not entirely cor- rect. If it is true that the man who is in the grip of habit with reference to certain methods of acting is a slave, it may be true also that he is the most truly free to advance. No one can make rapid progress until he has succeeded in establishing a mass of useful habits. Or, as Professor C. E. Seashore has said, nothing is well done until it is reduced to an automatic stage.4 Habits signify social reliability. A person with strength of character has a number of well-organized habits. The reliability of an individual is due to habitual re- actions. His honesty or dishonesty in dealing with others is largely a matter of habit; he who can be trusted is the person who is honest by habit. 3 Scott, W. D., Increasing Human Efficiency in Business, Ch. III. 4 Psychology in Daily Life, p. 89. 26 Social Psychology III. In addition to instinctive and habitual tendencies, there are marginal reactions which are conscious. Con- scious reactions appear chiefly at those points where the neuro-physiological mechanism is incapable of meeting the demands of the environment, that is, where instinc- tive and habitual reactions fail. Consciousness appears where changes, or new adjustments, are necessary; it is related to the process known as adjustment to envir- onment. It may be said to have three characteristics: (1) affective, or the feeling phase; (2) cognitive, or the thinking phase; and (3) volitional, or the “willing” phase.5 The feelings are closely related to the instincts. While they are perhaps as old in their origin as the in- stinctive tendencies, the development of the feeling side of life came later, phylogenetically, than that of the instinctive phase. Agreeable feelings accompany those ideas which further our momentary interests; while dis- agreeable feelings characterize the ideas which obstruct or thwart those interests, according to Professor J. R. Angell.6 With a slight modification the statement of Professor Ellwood may be accepted, namely, that feel- ing is the agreeable or disagreeable tone of conscious- ness that accompanies an idea or an activity.7 An act 5 Miller, I. E., The Psychology of Thinking, pp. 64 ff. 8 Psychology, p. 315. ''An Introduction to Social Psychology, p. 64. Psychological Bases 27 which as a rule has been favorable in the past to the organism and to the race produces an agreeable tone of consciousness; an experience which as a type has been unfavorable in past times to the individual and to the race produces a disagreeable tone of consciousness. The agreeable or disagreeable tone appears quickly and in far less time than is required to analyze and to evaluate that act. In other words, the feeling character of con- sciousness gives a quicker-than-thought evaluation to a given activity upon the basis of the past experience, not only of the organization itself, but also of the race. A pleasurable feeling that accompanies a given idea indicates that in the history of the organism or of the race, the group of acts to which the given idea is re- lated has been helpful and constructive. The pleasur- able tone does not necessarily indicate the present value of a given act. The fact that a type of acts in the past has been helpful or harmful indicates that in all prob- ability this type will continue to be helpful or harmful. People are peculiarly alike in their feelings, an obser- vation which has been explained on the basis of the fact that people have had about the same racial experi- ence. In this long racial history, certain ways of doing have been favorable to race development, and others, unfavorable. Current actions fall into certain group- ings as regards this racial experience and the result in all individuals is the same, an agreeable or disagreeable tone of consciousness, in accordance with favorable or unfavorable racial experiences. 28 Social Psychology It has been observed that it is difficult to argue against the feelings. The reasons are many. A main explanation is that the feeling phase of consciousness is outside the range of the cognitive side. Cognition can recognize, classify, and describe the events leading up to the expression of a given feeling, but can not do much else. An idea thrown against a feeling by way of argument travels on a different plane. Feelings can be “argued against” best through the stimulation of counter feelings. Then, it may also be said that the feeling side of consciousness developed much earlier, phylogenetically, than cognition. The feelings are older, phylogeneti- cally, than defined ideas. In some ways, the feelings are closer to the inner citadel of the self than are thoughts. He who acts according to his feelings acts usually in harmony with the dictates of racial experience. He thus acts wisely in-so-far as racial history is similar to current conditions. But conditions of life, both physi- cal and social, are constantly undergoing change. Hence, racial experience is not always a safe guide; another factor is necessary. This element is found in cognition, which is the core of consciousness. Cognition developed to enable the organism to adjust itself to new factors in the environ- ment. If there are no new problems to solve, then the feelings—as representing past experience—would be sufficient. But in a social environment which is so Psychological Bases 29 characterized by change, and in which so many new situations are constantly arising, the feelings are an in- adequate guide. A new element is required; cognition meets this need. With the feeling phase of conscious- ness for the evaluation of acts on the basis of past his- tory, and with the cognitive side for the evaluation of acts on the basis of present conditions and future prob- abilities, the organism is well equipped for the solution of the problems of life. As the social environment is more changeable and gives rise to more new problems than the physical en- vironment, cognition may be said, in a sense, to be a social product. Its development has come in response to the changing elements in the social environment. Even in the case of a child brought up away from peo- ple, it is probable that his cognitive characteristics would remain undeveloped. Or, in the case of an ordinary individual, the effects of an unusually stimulating social and mental environment are clearly seen. Pro- fessor Ross has used the term, “high potential of the city/’ in referring to the relatively large number of mental stimulations which come to an individual in a day and which result in increased mental activities. The highest form of cognition is reason. Pure reason takes cognizance of factors present in neither time nor space; it considers a larger environment than that pres- ent to the senses. It is a supreme adjuster. It helps 30 Social Psychology the individual to adjust himself to the factors of a uni- versal environment.8 The third characteristic of consciousness is the voli- tional. In the simpler sense, the volition is the acting side of consciousness; in a developed form, it is the choosing phase. Consciousness can make evaluations, not only upon the basis of past experience, and with reference to present needs and future probabilities, but it can also make choices and act upon them. Every idea is dynamic; every idea tends to be carried out. This motor characteristic of ideas is at the basis of volition. But volition includes the power of choos- ing. While many choices are probably made upon bases which are quite largely determined by hereditary and environmental factors, there is left a certain mar- gin wherein the individual may make original choices. This margin of freedom in choosing is undoubtedly a result of selection. Individuals with this margin of freedom survive better and are able to adjust them- selves more satisfactorily to their social environment with its elements of change than persons without this leeway. The margin of freedom of choice would be useless in a static environment, or in a purely physical, materialistic, and mechanistic universe. Thus, it is seen that volition has its fundamental roots in the changing factors of the social environment. 8 Op. cit., pp. 67, 68. Psychological Bases 31 The margin of freedom in making choices varies. When health conditions are unfavorable, when poverty pinches, or when wealth stifles, the margin shrinks. It is probable that the margin varies from hour to hour with every individual; but for nearly all individuals most of the time, this degree of freedom in choosing is in many ways the most significant psychical charac- teristic that they possess. The marginal degree of freedom means that the in- dividual is not completely plastic. He is relatively in- dependent of his environment. His innate and organic needs unite with his acquired habitual methods and his margin of freedom in determining what stimuli in the environment he shall respond to and which he shall ignore. Unconsciously and consciously, the organism makes choices among the countless stimuli with which it is being constantly bombarded. Volition, hence, has its fundamental basis in the changing factors of the social environment. Volition provides not only for in- dividual growth, but also for the social advancement of mankind. PROBLEMS (Instinctive Reactions) 1. What is an instinct? 2. What is the origin of instincts? 3. Give the most striking example of purely in- stinctive action that has come to your attention. 32 Social Psychology 4. Why are instincts common to people of every race? 5. Why can instincts never be eradicated from the mental constitution of the individual? 6. Distinguish between “social instincts” and “in- dividual instincts.” 7. What social instincts can you name? 8. Give three illustrations of the statement: Social institutions rest upon the basis of instincts. (Habitual Reactions) 9. Criticize: He instinctively closed the door after he entered the room. 10. What is the origin of habits? 11. What is the underlying purpose of habits? 12. What is the derivation of the term, habit? 13. Explain: The process of building up new habits is the method by which the individual grows. 14. Explain: The process of building up new habits is the method by which society progresses. 15. Illustrate: One’s strength of character is due to habit. 16. Illustrate: Habit is a time-saver. 17. Illustrate: Habit increases accuracy. 18. Illustrate: Habit gives a permanency to ex- perience. 19. Explain: “Habit is the bank into which con- sciousness puts its deposits.” Psychological Bases 33 20. Explain: Speed which is habitual is never hur- ried. 21. Explain: The population of London would be starved in a week if the flywheel of habit were removed. 22. Is it true that whatever is worth doing at all, is worth doing well? Why? 23. What is the habit of greatest usefulness that one can form? 24. How would you go about it, psychologically, to break a habit? 25. What class of habits are the most difficult to break ? 26. What is meant by a “social habit”? 27. Which would represent a greater loss to the individual, the loss of his habits, or the loss of his in- stincts ? 28. Which will be used in the following cases, in- stinct or habit? (a) By an untrained puppy when his mistress ap- pears with a plate of scraps. (b) By a trained puppy under similar circum- stances. (c) By a salmon in a whirling current of a river. (d) By a fireman who sees a house on fire. (e) By a mother whose child is in imminent danger. 34 Social Psychology (Conscious Reactions) General 29. When does consciousness appear in the ex- perience of an individual? 30. Qualitatively, which is a higher art: writing or walking; thinking or writing; deciding “no” or de- ciding “yes”? Why, in each case? A ffective 31. Define the feelings. 32. What does a pleasant feeling signify? 33. Why are human beings remarkably the same in regard to their feelings? 34. Why is it difficult to argue against the feelings ? Cognitive 35. Why do you think? 36. Why are you thinking now? 37. When do you do the least amount of thinking? 38. When do you do the highest grade of thinking? 39. Illustrate: Cognition plays the decisive role in adapting the organism to its environment. 40. Does a squirrel need to be more intelligent than a fish? 41. Does an architect need to be more intelligent than a mason? 42. Does the child of the tenements need to be more intelligent than the child of wealthy parents? Psychological Bases 35 43. Explain: “No two individuals can ever think alike, whilst any number can feel alike.” 44. Which tends to be expressed the quicker, the feeling side or the cognitive side of consciousness? Why? 45. Define the imagining phase of cognition. 46. Explain: “The tap-root of selfishness is weak- ness of imagination.” 47. Is the intolerant, selfish nation the unimagina- tive nation? 48. Of what does remembering consist? 49. Is the average person of today less able to re- member than the average person of three centuries ago ? 50. Give an illustration (a) of ancient feats of remembering, and (b) of present-day achievements along the same line ? 51. Explain: “The average student habitually be- gins the study of his lessons by memorizing, with the expectation of doing whatever thinking is necessary later.” 52. Is the examination system in universities sound, psychologically ? 53. Can one think quickly and well at the same time? 54. What is reasoning? 55. What is the highest function of reasoning? 56. Why do so few people develop the reasoning phase of consciousness to its full extent, when it would be so greatly to their advantage to do so ? 36 Social Psychology 57. When do you act most rationally? 58. Are the judgments which are made by men more impartial than those made by women? 59. Can the making of sound judgments be ac- quired through training? Volitional 60. Explain: (a) Thought is motor; (b) ideas are dynamic; and (c) the motor character of an idea. 61. Define volition. 62. Is it more common for a person to base his decision upon evidence, or to seek evidence to justify his decision ? Illustrate both methods. 63. Bring to the class an advertisement which ap- peals (a) to the feelings, (b) to cognition, and (c) to volition. READINGS (Instinctive Reactions) Angell, J. R., Psychology, Ch. XVI. Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. VI. Colvin and Bagley, Human Behavior, Chs. IX, X. Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Ch. IX. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. IX. Hayes, E. C., Introduction to the Study of Sociology, Ch. XIII. Hobhouse, L. T., Mind in Evolution, Ch. IV. Holmes, A., Principles of Character Making, Ch. V. James, Wm., Psychology (briefer course), Ch. XXV. Kirkpatrick, E. A., Fundamentals of Child Study, Chs. Ill, IV. Genetic Psychology, Ch. IV. Psychological Bases 37 Parraelee, Maurice, The Science of Human Behavior, Ch. XIII. (Habitual Reactions) Baldwin, J. M., Mental Development, Ch. XVI. Holmes, A., Principles of Character Making, Ch. VI. James, Wm., Psychology (briefer course), Ch. X. Scott, W. D., The Psychology of Advertising, Ch. IX. ■ Increasing Human Efficiency in Business, Ch. XIII. Wallas, G., The Great Society, Ch. V. (Conscious Reactions) Angell, J. R., Psychology, Chs. XIII, XXII. Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. VII. Mental Development, Ch. XIII. Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Chs. X, XII. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. IX. Horne, H. H., Psychological Principles of Education, Pts. II, III, IV. James, Wm., Psychology {briefer course), Ch. XVIII. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. IX. Miller, I. E., Psychology of Thinking. Pillsbury, W. B., Essentials of Psychology, Ch. XI. The Psychology of Reasoning. Royce, J., Outlines of Psychology, Chs. VIII, XV. Wallas, G., The Great Society, Chs. X-XII. Chapter III. THE SIMPLER SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE INDIVIDUAL. I. The discussion in this chapter is intended to carry forward the points emphasized in Chapter II concern- ing the social phases of the life and activities of the individual. The instincts may be subdivided into two groups: those which are concerned directly with pro- moting the welfare of the individual, and those which serve to continue and to enlarge group life. In the latter class, there are innumerable overlappings, but certain tendencies stand out as being social in their sig- nificance. Of these, the gregarious, sex and parental, inquisitive, combative, acquisitive, and play instincts and tendencies will receive attention. The gregarious instinct rests upon the satisfaction of being one of a herd or group, and the uneasiness—lead- ing to wild distraction—of being alone, or separated from the group. The animal which becomes separated from the herd will risk its life in order to get back. Social Characteristics 39 Rural people on a holiday rush to the places where the crowd is to congregate. Urban people herd together in the already overcongested districts. Prisoners sub- jected to solitary confinement suffer so greatly that penologists now consider this form of punishment too cruel. The gregarious instinct possesses a definite sur- vival value in that it keeps individuals together and fur- nishes a basis for co-operative work. In the long process of the struggle for existence, those individuals survive best who co-operate best. The sex and parental instincts are so closely related that they will be considered together. The sex instincts make the race possible. Without them, no large group could long survive. Their power is great and their regulation is one of the greatest of social problems. In fact, this problem has come to be known as the social evil. As an outgrowth of the sex instincts, the parental impulses are to be rated highly. With the develop- ment of the parental instincts, there followed histori- cally the rise of the institution of the family. Without parental care, the offspring early begins the struggle for existence, and has little opportunity for develop- ment. With one parent to give a protecting and direct- ing care, the offspring has a better chance for self-de- velopment and becomes a more useful member of the group. But when both parents co-operate in the process of family-building, the children are thus given the ad- vantage of the experience of two elders; and are pro- 40 Social Psychology tected from the harsher phases of the struggle for ex- istence, for a time sufficient to enable them to become mature individuals, and to learn the meaning of many fundamental principles of co-operative living. The importance of the parental instincts (and of the institution of the family) is so great that children who grow up outside of the family, rarely become real social members of society. The writer in studying the home conditions of delinquents has found that the broken-up home of one sort or another is found in the majority of delinquent cases.1 As a member of the family, the child learns funda- mental rules of social conduct. He acquires respect for law; he learns rudimentary principles of co-operation. Since the family is, in a sense, a social microcosm, the child in a normal family receives an excellent start along all lines which make for co-operation in society at large. The parental impulses result beneficially, also, as far as the parents are concerned. The instincts lead to conduct which is essentially altruistic. The parental impulses when in operation are constantly overcoming the self-gratifying desires. There is a continuous strug- gle between the parental tendencies on one hand and the egoistic interests on the other. In order to protect 1 “A Study of Juvenile Delinquency and Dependency in Los Angeles County for the Year 1912,” Jour, of Crim. Law and Criminol., Sept., 1914, p. 395. Social Characteristics 41 itself and to further the parental impulses, the group (and society) has built up powerful sanctions. The list includes moral rules, e. g., Honor thy father and thy mother. Then there is the institution of marriage which has become a guardian of the parental impulses. Taboos upon celibacy, upon divorce, upon immoral sex life are powerful agents supporting the parental in- stincts. Ancestor worship has emphasized the parental impulses, and thus assisted other factors in giving to China a long life. Emphasis upon sound family life has enabled, to a positive extent, the Hebrew race to perpetuate itself and to survive countless obstacles and innumerable destructive factors. The inquisitive instinct leads to inquiry into those phenomena which are moderately different from those of regular occurrence. Events which are not different do not attract notice; events which are markedly dif- ferent from anything that has been known to occur arouse fear. Animals which have been led astray by any- thing that is very strange have probably lost their lives. Those individuals, either animal or human, which are never attracted by anything new remain mediocre mem- bers of the group. But those who show an interest in things which are moderately strange, avoid destruc- tion on the one hand and mediocrity on the other. Thus the curiosity impulses have taken form. Groups of reasonably curious individuals survive best. It seems true that scientific research, and even intel- lectual study, as a rule, is based definitely upon the 42 Social Psychology curiosity impulse. Research work in its purest form is largely motivated by the curiosity instinct. All in- tellectual progress, and thus social progress, depends in a degree upon the curiosity instinct. The combative instinct operates as a force which calls for broad-minded consideration. In a primitive group, the fighting members survived; the others per- ished. In primitive society, the fighting tribes suc- ceeded best; the others were destroyed. Thus through a long period of time, the combative instinct has been at a high survival premium. It has become deep-seated in human nature. It is accompanied by the emotion of anger. It oper- ates when any obstacle appears in the way of the other instinctive tendencies, or of the habitual activities, or of the newly aroused and currently conscious desires. The combative instinct—including the accompanying emo- tion of anger—serves to energize the individual, to concentrate his energies, and to drive him ahead over the obstacles that are in the way. In its crudest forms, it shows itself in the snarl and rush of the dog, in the clenched and striking fists of the boy, in the “hanging” episodes of the mob, in the brutal atrocities wdiich are committed in the name of organized warfare. The fighting instinct has been undergoing modifi- cations. While it is a heritage from the days of tooth and claw, it is, in modified terms, an essential factor in civilization. In early days it was expressed in a physi- cal way, for example, in the physical combat. Today, Social Characteristics 43 in a civilized state, individuals no longer, as a rule, resort to a physical clash in order to settle disputes, but turn to organized courts and to discussion and concilia- tion. Their individual fighting abilities are thus not used destructively, but are turned from physical com- bat to spiritual interchange of opinion. In fact, it appears that the fighting instinct may be subjected to certain intrinsic changes. Its very nature may yet be made over by the operation of intellectual phenomena, such as discussion; of social organizations, such as courts of justice; and of the highest spiritual virtues, such as love. The biological struggle in the human world as manifested in militarism and commercialism of the de- structive type is probably doomed to give way ulti- mately before “the quiet, creative influences of the spiritual virtues,” chief of which is love. The “fittest,” as a class, are undergoing an evolution from the lowest types of personified physical might and power to types of mental power and efficiency, and then to rational types controlled by the social principle of love. One of the latest indications of the turning belief in regard to the need for vital modifications of the fighting instinct comes from no other source than the University of Berlin. According to The Nation (Jan- uary 17, 1918), Professor G. F. Nicolai of the Univer- sity of Berlin published in 1917 a book entitled Die Biologie des Krieges.2 This daring German writer * Quoted from the IV estminster Gazette, 44 Social Psychology (imprisoned for his views) holds that the hitherto ineradicable fighting instinct, in the modern world, is a survival of tendencies which, though at one time use- ful, are now positively dangerous. The need for a change in the nature of this instinct is imperative. One race of animals after another has perished because it could not change its instinctive ways; hence, is mankind to die out because it cannot change the fighting instinct ? Will mankind literally kill itself off? In Chapter VI, the discussion of the psychology of war is continued under a study of group conflicts, especially of conflicts between modern states, and of the evolution in the nature of group conflicts. Nations, however, still resort to physical combat. It is a hope and a working ambition that the time will soon come when nations can develop an international court (including an international constabulary) for set- tling disputes upon such effective and just bases, that they no longer must live under the continual shadow of possible war. When war becomes historic, there will still be a great need for the combative impulses. Then, nations and individuals will be called upon to fight social evils and sins. They will be constrained to fight, not the best people of competitive, sovereign groups; but the evil in all people, under a social, plane- tary, and international order. The struggles against social wickedness will always demand, as far as one can now see, the exercise of the fighting instinct in a modified form. Thus, the combative tendencies are not Social Characteristics 45 to be eliminated, but to be directed, modified, and made subservient to world-wide welfare. The acquisitive instinct, also, involves serious social implications. The tendency to acquire is an individual- izing force, in fact, to such an extent that its operation has to be checked by public action. It lies at the basis of all acquisitions of land and other forms of material wealth. Men go on accumulating riches long after they have acquired enough property for their own needs and for the needs of their dependents. Nations have fought to acquire land. Nearly all of the extensive wars that have been waged by monarchical governments have been related to the national desire for more land. When monarchial forms of government pass away, it is prob- able that territorial wars will be known no longer. A fundamental problem of the day is that of regu- lating, nationally, the operation of the acquisitive in- stinct. On one side are those who argue that these desires should be crushed out and that the government should own all rent-producing land and interest-pro- ducing capital. On the other hand, there are the peo- ple who hold that the acquisitive impulses are too deep- seated to be ruled out, that it would not be wise—if it were possible—to eliminate them, and that they should be allowed to operate, but subject to governmental regu- lation in line with public welfare. Their operation as- sumes on occasion, however, a force which defies effi- cient governmental control. The regulation of the 46 Social Psychology acquisitive instinct constitutes one of the most serious problems of the day in our country. Another characteristic which is innate, instinctive, and of vast social significance is the tendency to play. It is complex enough to permit of various explanations and of markedly different classifications. It functions as a highly valuable socializing agency. As a member of the play-group, the child learns co-operative lessons of fundamental, life-long, and social importance; he learns to obey, to lead, to evaluate himself as a group- member, and to appreciate the larger meanings of team- work. The normal exercise of the play impulses renews life. It rehabilitates and it re-creates life; it offers relaxa- tion, and at the same time brings the individual to a balanced attitude toward the world of living, chang- ing, and developing people. Further, the emphasis is being placed today upon eight hours for work, eight hours for sleep, and eight hours for recreation. The formula is not followed regularly, but it does indicate that a large portion of life is given over to amusements and recreation. The questions arise: “Does it matter how one plays?” and, “Is it anybody’s business how one spends his leisure hours?” The answer is positive and affirmative from the standpoint of group welfare. It matters decidedly from the viewpoint of the group how the individual plays, whether he wastes or builds up his energies when he is not at work. Social Characteristics 47 In this age, commercial enterprise has entered the field of providing amusements of all sorts and for all classes and all moods of individuals. The provision is made in ways, primarily, to secure profits, not to build up those who are seeking recreation. Hence, the type of appeals that is made to the play instincts becomes a problem of vast social moment.3 II. Another social phase of the life of the individual is that of sympathy. As the word implies, sympathy refers to the characteristic of “feeling with” others. Sympathy helps an individual to understand the experi- ences, attitudes toward life, and states of mind of other people. As an example of an elemental form of sym- pathetic emotion, the immediate and appropriate re- sponse of the brood of chickens to the warning cry of the mother hen may be cited. The vigorous crying of a baby is followed by simultaneous crying on the part of near-by infants, even though they do not have the slightest idea as to the cause of the crying of the first child. A scream of terror on the part of an adult evokes a similar pang on the part of bystanders, even though the bystanders do not know the cause of the cry. The characteristic of “feeling with” others varies with individuals. In an extreme form, it decreases the efficiency of the individual. In a weak degree, it 3 Bogardus, E. S., Introduction to Sociology, Ch. V. 48 Social Psychology results in egoistic, selfish living on the part of the in- dividual. When an important question is to be settled, the side which is successful in enlisting the feelings and the sympathies possesses a marked advantage. As in the case of the feelings, sympathy is not closely related to the reasoning phase of consciousness, and hence is liable to express itself in highly irrational ways. Its mani- festations are usually found on the conservative side of an issue. The sympathies are generally allied with the old, the tried, and the true. They are stabilizing forces; but when they become attached to certain out- worn habits, the sympathies become a stumbling- block in the way of progress. On the other hand, every new reform movement seeks to reach and to gain the sympathies of the people. If it is able thus to do, its success is assured. Sympathy has been described as a social cement, because it assists in holding the members of a group together, and in forming new groups. The emotions as social factors represent such en- larged phases of the feeling side of the mind that they are accompanied temporarily either by a wild, blind ex- hibition of the volitional nature, or by an inhibition of the will power. The emotion of anger, for example, results in concentrated, but frequently irrational ac- tivity; while, on the other hand, the emotion of subjec- tion (and dejection), which occurs in relation to defeat and vital losses, tends to paralyze all volitional effort. Social Characteristics 49 Emotions arise in connection with blocked interests. Whenever an obstacle appears in the path of a human desire, a mental conflict ensues, accompanied by emo- tional manifestations. The emotion is, in a way, the affective phase of the conflict. When conflicts occur, the emotions arise; but when no conflicts exist, ennui develops. An emotion and ennui represent opposite ends of the pole of interest. Emotions, in other words, heighten and give color to the obstacles of life. The sentiments are psychic forces with social mean- ings. A sentiment is a complex of emotional reactions which appears in organized ways. The sentiments are organized emotions with social values. Admiration, for example, involves another person, that is, the admired as well as the one who admires. It implies a certain degree of wonder, of humility, and of generosity. Ad- miration plus fear constitutes awe; awe with the addi- tion of gratitude leads to reverence—the highest known religious emotional expression.4 Love and hate are organized emotional complexes which enter into human reactions concerning nearly every phase of life, social as well as individual. Love, like its fundamental feeling and emotional elements, is a stabilizing, conserving, and drawing force of im- measurable power. In its social expressions, it may be more or less purely sexual, and may lead to sexual 4 See McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psy- chology, Ch. V. 50 Social Psychology vice. A higher form is that known as romantic love, the subjects of which are impelled to extensive under- takings in behalf of the ones who are loved. Conjugal love develops out of the fact that husbands and wives experience great joys and intense sufferings together. Maternal love is the keenest, deepest, and most concen- trated form of the love of one person for another, i. e., the love of a mother for her child. Consanguineal love refers to the affection of children for one another, pri- marily, in a given family, and of the father for his offspring. Consanguineal love may extend itself be- yond blood relationships, and in its highest sense, it may assume planetary proportions and give content to a doctrine of brotherhood of man.5 Hate is a sentiment of anti-social import when di- rected blindly against people. Its social strength appears when it is hurled against social sin, rather than against people, as such. Closely related to hate is the sentiment of jealousy which often works as a narrowing influence. The individual is justified in being jealous only of that which in the long run is in line with public welfare. The list of the sentiments is long, and includes in addition to those mentioned, emotional complexes such as shame, pity, surprise, re- spect, and anxiety. Character is more than organized sentiments, and more than disposition and temperament. Disposition s See Ward, L. F., Pure Sociology, pp. 377 ff. Social Characteristics 51 refers to the sum total of one’s instinctive tendencies, while temperament is more complicated and involves the constitutional ways of evaluating the various experi- ences of life.6 The disposition and temperament of the individual are largely determined for the individual by the laws of heredity. Character, however, is built upon the disposition and temperament and along lines determined to an extent by the individual himself. Character involves the extent and the way in which the individual organizes his ways of acting. Dependable- ness of character is a vital social attribute. In a world of group interactions, honesty, reliability, chastity, and dependableness of character are essential. III. The development of the self is largely a social pro- cess.7 To the young child, everything is at first objec- tive. Even his fingers and toes seem to belong to the outside world. But through experience, for example, of pain and of suffering, the infant begins to give to his fingers and toes a self valuation. Through his ex- periences, he comes to distinguish between the ego and the alter, and to set up a self-world in apposition to an others-world. The idea of the self develops in rela- tion to the concept of others; it is in part a reflection of “McDougall, Win., An Introduction to Social Psychology, pp. 258 ff. T Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. I, and Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Chs. I, II. 52 Social Psychology other selves in a world of group relationships. The interaction between the growth of the ego and the alter in the mind of the child is constant. The process may be considered as one, and the ego and the alter as oppo- site ends of the same pole. The child’s social conscious- ness arises simultaneously and to an extent prior to the development of his self-consciousness. In fact, if it were not for the presence and activities of others, his self-con- sciousness would remain undeveloped. The leading stimuli which call forth self-consciousness are those which come from contact with other selves. When the child is learning the meaning of life through his experiences, he is simultaneously reading those meanings into the experiences of others. He pro- jects himself and his experiences into the world of life about him, a stage in the development of the self which the writer would call projective. Each fact of life to the growing individual is first objective and with little meaning, then subjective and full of personal signifi- cance, and finally projective and social. The process is essentially one of social self-development. In this connection, the looking-glass self is a term which conveys a valuable truth.8 The looking-glass self is the impression of oneself that he sees in the minds and estimates of others. The conduct of every person is continuously conditioned by the presence and 8 Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, pp. 152 ff. Social Characteristics 53 opinions of others, and especially by the judgments of his friends. The young man who, although not inter- ested in missions, subscribes liberally to a missionary collection, because by his side sits a young lady who is active in missionary enterprises and whose favor he wishes to secure, is an example. Or, a business man may boast of a shrewd transaction to one whom he knows would approve of said shrewdness, while he may re- frain from mentioning the deal to another friend whose standards are higher. At every turn of life, the choices of a person are partially determined by the image of himself which he sees in the judgments of others. PROBLEMS (The Social Instincts) 1. What is meant by the gregarious instinct? 2. Does the gregarious instinct exist in the hermit? 3. Why do the working classes on holidays rush to the places where the crowds are? 4. Why is the country considered dull by so many people ? 5. What are the leading forces opposing the paren- tal impulses? 6. What kind of impulses sets off the curiosity in- stinct ? 7. Are women more curious than men? 8. What connection is there between the curiosity instinct and the scientific tendencies? 54 Social Psychology 9. What instinctive tendencies impel a person to run to see a fight? 10. Is it necessary to get angry in order to fight well ? 11. Do conscripted soldiers fight with the same spirit as volunteer soldiers? 12. Are the fighting impulses to be ruled out en- tirely, or to be directed along higher lines? 13. What methods can you suggest whereby the fighting instinct may be directed along higher paths? 14. What is meant by “righteous indignation”? 15. Is the fighting instinct necessary to social prog- ress? 16. What has rendered unnecessary the bodily com- bat of individuals in order to settle disputes? 17. Most men do not want war, yet we have it; why? 18. Is the operation of the acquisitive instinct to be eliminated, or to be directed along social lines? 19. What are the social values in play? 20. Explain: A mason who is piling up brick is working, and a boy who is piling up blocks is playing. 21. Why is work hard, and play easy, to a child even when the latter requires the expenditure of more energy ? 22. Why is clearing brush and weeds from a lot for a baseball diamond considered as play by a boy, while clearing the same lot at his parent’s command is work ? Social Characteristics 55 (Sympathy, the Emotions, the Sentiments, Character) 23. Why is one’s sympathies toward a fellow coun- tryman more keen in a foreign country than when one is at home? 24. Why is it not enough for a business man to be a sympathetic husband, parent, and neighbor? 25. Explain: Every citizen should indulge in a spell of capricious and sympathetic giving. 26. Is sympathy a necessary qualification for a suc- cessful social worker? 27. Distinguish between the emotions and the feelings. 28. Is anger a good guide to action? 29. Is national anger a correct guide to national action ? 30. Why are children afraid of the dark? 31. Describe the physical expression of (a) a happy face, (b) a sad face, (c) an angry face; and explain the relation of the given facial expression to the given emotion. 32. How would you define the sentiment of love? 33. What is the social value of love? 34. What is the social utility of hate? 35. Define jealousy. 36. Illustrate: Character is socially essential. 56 Social Psychology (The Social Self, the Looking-Glass Self) 37. Distinguish between the individual self and the social self. 38. Give an original illustration of the looking- glass self. 39. Why is it that many individuals can talk to two or three persons with ease, but cannot talk to twenty or thirty persons without embarassment ? 40. In what ways does the looking-glass self of a student affect his recitation in a class? 41. How far should one allow his looking-glass self to determine his character? 42. Who are the more sensitive to the looking- glass self, men or women? 43. What causes a little boy to become ashamed of his curls? 44. Are the wealthy or the poor more sensitive to the looking-glass self? 45. Which is the greater factor in arousing the de- sire of a girl “to make a sorority”, the gregarious in- stinct, or the looking-glass self? READINGS (Social Instincts) Ellwood, C. A., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch IX. Groos, K., The Play of Animals. The Play of Man. Social Characteristics 57 Kirkpatrick, E. A., Fundamentals of Child Study, Chs. VII, IX-XI. Kropotkin, P., Mutual Aid; a Factor in Evolution. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Sect. II. Morris, C., “War as a Factor in Civilization,” Popular Sci- ence Mon., XLVII: 823-34. Novicow, J., Les luttes entre societes humaines. Patrick, G. T. W., The Psychology of Relaxation, Chs. II, IV. Ribot, Th., The Psychology of the Emotions, Pt. II, Ch. VI. Smith, W. R., An Introduction to Educational Sociology, Ch. V. Thomas, W. I., “The Gaming Instinct,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., VI: 650-63. Trotter, W., The Instincts of the Herd in Peace and War, pp. 23-66, 101-213. Veblen, T., The Instinct of Workmanship, Ch. II. (Sympathy, the Emotions, the Sentiments, Character) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. VIII. Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, Ch. IV. Social Organization, Chs. XVI, XVII. Ellwood, C. A., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. XI. Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Ch. XIV. Kirkpatrick, E. A., Fundamentals of Child Study, Ch. IX. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Chs. IV, V, XV. Ribot, Th., The Psychology of the Emotions, Pt. II, Ch. IV. Ross, E. A., Social Control, Chs. II, III. Smith, A., A Theory of the Moral Sentiments. -* In Carver, Sociology and Social Progress, Ch. XVI. 58 Social Psychology Thorndike, E. L., The Original Nature of Man, Ch. XI. Wallas, G., The Great Society, Chs. IV, IX. (The Social Self, the Looking-Glass Self) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. II. Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, Chs. V, VI. Social Organization, Chs. I, II. Giddings, F. H., Elements of Sociology, Ch. IX. Inductive Sociology, Pt. IV, Ch. III. Hobhouse, L. T., Mind in Evolution, Ch. XVII. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Chs. VII, VIII. Ormund, A. T., “The Social Individual,” Psychological Bui, VIII: 27-41. Chapter IV. THE SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE INDIVIDUAL ( Continued) IV. Language is a social product. It arises on the basis of the need for intercommunication; it involves a sym- bol and a meaning for the symbol. The significance of the symbol must be clear to the individual with whom communication is held. The symbol is always a ges- ture of some sort, either pantomimic (of the hands and shoulders), or mimetic (of the face), or vocal. In every case, the gesture represents the beginning of a whole act. As soon as the second party recognizes the whole act of which the given gesture is the begin- ning, conversation has begun. The response will con- sist of another gesture—the beginning of another act— and thus the conversation of attitudes and appropriate responses will continue. Hence, language is a social phenomenon, and consists in an interchange of gestures and suitable responses between individuals—a theory of 60 Social Psychology language which has been described best by Professor G. H. Mead.1 V. At first thought, the subject of laughter does not seem to be of a nature serious enough for scientific dis- cussion. It, however, is a phenomenon which mani- fests itself constantly in daily life. In a physiological sense, it may express simple relief from strained situ- ations, as in the case of school children who, when re- leased from hours of study and recitation, start home- ward in a laughing mood. It may represent mere ex- uberance and joy in living, as seen frequently on the part of children. Or, the nervous reaction from over- strain of any sort may culminate in hysterical laughter. In a psychological sense, laughter involves frequent- ly the expression of sympathetic emotion. As a mem- ber of a group, one laughs more easily than when alone. Furthermore, much laughter is caused by the expres- sion of unusual ideas, such as logical inconsistencies, ungrammatical constructions, statements involving a sudden change “from the sublime to the ridiculous,” suggestion not intended, the pun, and moderate exag- geration or understatement. Laughter is due, also, to causes that are directly social. Much laughter arises directly from observing the incongruous actions of others. The Charlie Chap- 1 “Social Consciousness and the Consciousness of Meaning,” Psychological Bui., VII: 397-405. Social Characteristics 61 lin films succeed because of the portrayal of incon- gruous motions, actions, and situations. Further, there is the laughter of the person who laughs in order not to be conspicuous, of the person who laughs to cover his embarrassment, and of him who laughs in order to make others laugh, e. g., the professional actor. Then there is social ridicule. The group laughs at the mistake or idiosyncrasy of the individual. This group laughter may be either (a) spontaneous, (b) delayed, or (c) concerted. The last type involves malicious- ness. Every markedly new idea must run the gauntlet of social laughter. There is some overlapping and some unsurveyed ter- ritory in the accompanying classification of the causes of laughter. It will be observed that the writer ac- cepts neither the single theory explanations of laughter, such as those of Aristotle, Kant, Schopenhauer, or Sully, nor the analysis in toto as made by Sidis, but pre- fers a two-fold classification based upon (1) physio- logical and (2) psycho-sociological elements, and in- cluding parts of the analyses by Sidis and others. Causes of Laughter. A. Physiological. 1. Tickling. 2. Relief. 3. Exuberance. 4. Hysteria. 62 Social Psychology B. Psycho-sociological. 1. Unusual ideas. (1) Logical inconsistency. (2) Grammatical error. (3) Sublime to the ridiculous. (4) Suggestion not intended. (5) The pun. (6) Moderate overstatement or under- statement. 2. Group contagion. 3. Incongruous actions of others. 4. In order not to be conspicuous. 5. To cover embarrassment. 6. To make others laugh (professional). 7. To make fun of others (social ridicule). (1) Spontaneous. (2) Reflective. (3) Concerted. VI. Suggestion and imitation are here considered as dif- ferent phases of the same process. Suggestion is the initiating part and imitation is the responding phase of the phenomenon. Suggestion develops out of the motor character of ideas, or from the dynamic nature of thought. If some one merely mentions apple pie, even between meals, I am quite certain to feel hungry for apple pie. If some Social Characteristics 63 one casually refers to a baseball game that is in prog- ress while I am writing these lines, I may find myself unconsciously laying aside the pen and looking for my cap. Further, I will go to the game, if there are no seriously inhibiting impulses, either instinctive, habitual, or conscious. Suggestion, then, refers to the conscious or unconscious intrusion to the mind of an idea which is accepted uncritically and carried out more or less immediately, unless inhibited by opposing impulses. This definition differs from that of Sidis 2 who holds that the suggested idea meets at first with more or less opposition. Suggestion may be direct or indirect. If direct, it comes usually in the form of a command, from one who is older and whose word is accepted as authority. The child in responding to parental command illus- trates direct suggestion; a hypnotist in giving his orders is another example, for the subject responds within instinctive and organized limits in an uncritical and immediate way to the suggested action. Hypnotism is characterized by abnormal suggestibility, and as a phenomenon for study, comes within the purview of abnormal psychology, rather than of social psychology. As a social phenomenon, hypnotism is as yet too little understood to be of much social value. Indirect suggestion operates unrecognized by the sub- ject. It has been described by Professor E. A. Ross 2 The Psychology of Suggestion, p. 15. 64 Social Psychology as “slantwise” suggestion and as representing a flank movement, rather than a frontal attack, as in the case of direct suggestion. The adult mind is frequently more easily influenced by this method than by any other plan. The use of the terms direct and indirect suggestion has reference to the way in which the suggestion gains entrance to the mind. Another set of terms, namely, immediate, mediate, and contra-suggestion, has been used by Mr. Sidis and indicates the ways in which sug- gestions are carried out in action. If a suggested idea is acted upon at once and in line with its impulses, the phenomenon comes under the caption of immedi- ate suggestion. If time elapses and modifications occur, the type is called mediate suggestion. Some persons and many children respond in an opposite way to that which is suggested, and illustrate the principle of con- tra-suggestion. The last mentioned form is usually connected with an exaggerated sense of individuality, and with an inadequate opportunity to learn the les- sons of give-and-take in play groups. Suggestibility refers to the degree to which a person is open to suggestion. Normal suggestibility involves fixation of attention, elimination of inhibitory impulses, and immediate consummation. All minds are suggestible, but in varying degrees. The variations in suggestibility of an individual have been carefully discussed by Mr. McDougall and Pro- fessor Ross. Their analyses have been co-ordi- Social Characteristics 65 nated in the following statement. Suggestibility varies: (a) with the age of the individual, (b) with tempera- ment, (c) with sex, (d) with the degree of fatigue and illness, (e) with the amount of organized facts con- cerning the suggested idea, (f) with the extent of pres- tige or authority which centers about the person who gives the suggestion, and (g) with the degree of crowd or group emotion that prevails. VII. What has been said of suggestion, applies also to imitation, the motor part of the same process. Some so-called imitation is nothing more than a phase of communication. The small boy who clenches his fist when he faces the clenched fist of another boy, is not imitating the act of the second youth, but is simply making an appropriate response. The suitable response which is called forth happens to resemble the com- bative attitude, but is not an imitation thereof. Imitation may be either conscious or unconscious. These two types have been considered in the paragraphs upon direct and indirect suggestion. Direct suggestion usually leads to more or less conscious imitation, while indirect suggestion finds expression through uncon- scious imitation. Imitation is always accompanied by invention.3 Nothing is imitated exactly according to copy, because 3 Baldwin, J. M., The Individual and Society, p. 149. 66 Social Psychology of the personal equation of the imitator. In imitating, a person will vary somewhat from the copy, even if the process is unconscious. It may be said that even so- called pure invention consists in modifying copies. In invention, the imitation and modification of the old is a larger element than the creation of something entirely new. The inventions of Mr. Thomas A. Edison, for example, are based upon past discoveries. Every imita- tor is at the same time an inventor, and every inventor is also an imitator. Inventions will receive further attention in a subsequent chapter. Imitation is primarily a conserving factor in society. It secures the continuation of established ways of doing, and also, of new methods. Imitation is a leading phase of the educational process, by which knowledge is passed on from generation to generation. Imitation assumes two forms of social expression: (a) custom imitation, which has reference to the imi- tation of past ways of doing; and (b) contemporary imitation, which involves the copying of current ideas and actions. Contemporary imitation may be either non-competitive, or competitive. The former class is closely akin in its operation to custom imitation; the latter form, known as fashion imitation, manifests special characteristics, such as the imitation of the new and the novel, under special group conditions. Rational, or merit imitation, includes those cases of custom imitation and fashion imitation which are rea- sonable under the attendant conditions. Many customs, Social Characteristics 67 but only a small percentage of fashions, will pass the test of serviceability. Rational imitation, as well as the larger fields of custom and fashion imitation, will constitute the central themes of the next chapter. PROBLEMS (Language) 1. What is meant by the social origin of language? 2. Illustrate the various forms of language, (a) mimetic, (b) pantomimic, and (c) vocal. 3. Explain: A gesture is a syncopated act. (Laughter) 4. Is laughter a topic important enough for serious discussion? Why? 5. Is it worth while to develop the habit of seeing the humorous side of life? 6. Why do people laugh? 7. What are the physical expressions of a hearty laugh ? 8. What is Shakespeare’s meaning when he speaks of being “stabbed” with laughter? 9. What does Milton mean when he writes of “laughter holding both his sides”? 10. What are the earliest causes of laughter in the child ? 11. Why do we laugh at incongruous or degrading experiences of others? Why should we not feel grieved ? 68 Social Psychology 12. Why does a group of school children released from the class room burst forth into boisterous laughter ? 13. What are the psychological effects of a good laugh ? 14. Why is a city dude in the country a mirth- producing object? 15. Why is a “hayseed” in the city the subject of laughter ? 16. Is man more afraid of social ridicule than of actual severe punishment? 17. Explain: “Laughter can kill innovations.” 18. Explain: “The true hero is one who can ig- nore social laughter.” 19. Why do people laugh at stories involving stut- tering? 20. Why is the walking of a drunken man consid- ered laughable by many persons? 21. Why does a wry face (without pain) cause the spectator to laugh ? 22. What is the cause of laughter on the part of the onlooker in the following cases: (a) The entrance of a dog into a lecture room filled with students; (b) The waves dashing unexpectedly over a person who is walking on the beach; (c) A person falling upstairs; (d) The comic sheet; (e) A chair that breaks down during a lecture; (f) A trivial interruption that occurs during a prayer service. 23. What is the most common cause of laughter? Social Characteristics 69 24. Why are deaf people, and not blind people, used in comedies? 25. Distinguish between humor, comedy, and wit. 26. What is the leading social value in laughter? (Suggestion) 27. Is every normal mind suggestible? 28. What is the relation of the motor character of ideas to suggestibility? 29. What rule may one follow in driving a nail in order to avoid hitting his thumb? 30. What is meant by “muscle reading”? 31. Explain: Your throat will ache “after listen- ing to a speaker who forms his voice badly.” 32. What is the difference between impulses and suggestions ? 33. Distinguish between direct and indirect sug- gestion by the use of original illustrations. 34. Illustrate slantwise suggestion. 35. What is the suggestion involved in the politi- cian’s slogan: “Pass prosperity around”? 36. What difference does it make whether clerks ask, “Shall we send the package?” or, “Shall we send the package, or will you take it with you?” 37. Which is the better sign: “Keep off the Grass,” or “Why not keep on the Sidewalk”? 38. How does the following sign affect you: “La- dies and Gentlemen will not pull the Flowers; others must not.” 70 Social Psychology 39. Find in Shakespeare an illustration of indirect suggestion. 40. How do you explain “the deadliness of the innuendo”? 41. Why is faint praise more damaging than down- right depreciation? 42. Why is it true that sometimes the best way to get the offer of a coveted position is not to seem anxious for it? 43. Is a person suggestible when asleep? 44. Give an original illustration of auto-suggestion. 45. Illustrate how suggestibility varies according (a) to species, (b) to prestige, (c) to fatigue, and (d) to races. 46. Why are the French or the Italians more sub- ject to suggestion than the English or the Germans? 47. Are women more suggestible than men? 48. Is an under-fed person more suggestible than a well-fed person? 49. Account for the moral influence of certain teachers and the lack thereof of others, equally well-in- tentioned. 50. What is the danger in talks “on sex hygiene be- fore the segregated pupils of the public schools” ? 51. Is it safer “on meeting a formidable animal to stand than to run”? 52. Explain the suggestion in the statement “He doth protest too much.” Social Characteristics 71 53. Distinguish between normal and abnormal sug- gestibility. 54. When is a person most suggestible? 55. When is one least suggestible? (Imitation) 56. Give an original illustration of imitation. 57. Distinguish between conscious and unconscious imitation by the use of illustrations. 58. Illustrate: Imitation is a conserving factor in society. 59. Explain: Imitation is a vital factor in social progress. 60. Explain: “Everybody in the same village walks on an average at the same rate of speed.” 61. Illustrate: “We are most imitative in the things not the object of conscious attention.” 62. Is “sentiment more electric than opinion” ? 63. Is an ideal a better religious nucleus than a dogma ? 64. Why is the moral responsibility of the novelist great ? 65. Who is the more dangerous to society, the dis- seminator of wrong ideals, or of wrong opinions? 66. Should there be censorship of motion pictures? 67. Does art need censorship more than science? 68. Explain: “The vortical suction of our civiliza- tion is stronger than ever before.” 72 Social Psychology 69. Explain psychologically: Nothing succeeds like success. 70. Whom does a child imitate the more, other chil- dren, or adults? 71. Which is the more subject to imitation, (a) in- dolence or ambition; (b) saving or spending? READINGS (Language) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, pp. 137-48. Froebel, F., The Education of Man, pp. 208-25. Mead, G. H., “Social Consciousness and the Consciousness of Meaning,” Psychological Bui., VII: 397-405. Preyer, W., Mental Development in the Child, Ch. VII. Tarde, G., The Laws of Imitation, pp. 255-65. La logique sociale, Ch. V. Tylor, E. B., Anthropology, Chs. IV, V, VII. Wundt, W., Elements of Folk Psychology, pp. 53-67. (Laughter) Bergson, H. L., Laughter. Patrick, G. T. W., The Psychology of Relaxation, Ch. III. Sidis, B., The Psychology of Laughter. Sully, J., An Essay on Laughter. (Suggestion and Imitation) Baldwin, J. M., Mental Development, Chs. VI, IX, XII. Binet, A., La suggestibilite. Carver, T. N., (ed.), Sociology and Social Progress, Ch. XXI. Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, Ch. II. Davis, Jr., M. M., Psychological Interpretations of Society, Chs. IX, X. Social Characteristics 73 Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Ch. XIII. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. X. Fry, E., “Imitation as a Factor in Human Progress,” Con- temp. Rev., LV: 658-77. Gowin, E. B., The Executive and his Control of Men, Ch. XII. Gumplowicz, L., “La suggestion sociale,” Riv. ital. di social., IV: 545-55. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sect. II. Keatinge, M. W., Suggestion in Education. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, pp. 96-107, 325-45. Miinsterberg, H., On the Witness Stand, pp. 96-107. Paulhan, F., Psychologie de l’ invention. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Ch. II. Social Control, Chs. XIII, XIV. Schmidkunz, H., Psychologie der Suggestion. Tarde, G., The Lavas of Imitation. Social Lavas. In Carver, Sociology and Social Progress, Ch. XXI. Thorndike, E. L., The Original Nature of Man, Ch. VIII. Chapter V. THE SOCIAL OPERATION OF IMITATION I. Individuals imitate their contemporaries; they also copy their predecessors. In either case, imitation may be conscious, or it may be unconscious, on the part of the individual. In either instance, the individual may imitate blindly, and hence in an irrational way, or he may imitate in a highly rational manner. Contemporary imitation is either competitive or non- competitive; if it is of the latter type, it possesses psy- chical characteristics similar to those of custom imita- tion, which will be discussed in subsequent paragraphs in this chapter. But contemporary imitation is often competitive, in which case it is known as fashion imitation. Fashion imitation rests fundamentally upon at least five elemental factors: First, there is the imitation pro- cess itself, through which fashions become current. By the imitation of the example of others, the individual is led upon the road that others are traveling; his interest Imitation 75 in social adaptation is satisfied; and union with others of his class is established.1 In the second place, fashion gratifies the opposite de- sire for self-individualization and for differentiation, the desire to give oneself an individual stamp, and the desire for variation, contrast, and the new, through “a con- stant change of contents.” No one wishes to be consid- ered mediocre, or, everyone desires to be at least a little different from the mass, and to be credited with some degree of individuality. Fashion not only gives one a feeling of unity with his own group, but it also sets one off from other groups; it not only unites but it also sepa- rates ; it settles at one and the same time the demand for unity and for segregation; it meets simultaneously the needs of class unity and of individual distinction. A third element is that of novelty. In those coun- tries, of course, where customs are almost literally wor- shipped and custom imitation rules, the new and the novel gain prestige with difficulty. But where fashion imitation has once gained standing, then the prestige of the novel assumes large proportions. The importance attached to novelty rises concomitantly with the devel- opment of fashion imitation itself; one movement accel- erates the other. Reputability is a fourth factor. The current knowl- edge that people are imitating a new style, or are ready 1 Simmel, G., “Fashion,” International Quarterly, X: 133 if; see also Tarde, The Laws of Imitation, pp, 244 ff., and Ross, Social Psychology, Ch. VI, 76 Social Psychology and waiting to do so, gives the new style a full-fledged start. The fact that one’s acquaintances have endorsed or adapted a new idea implies that this idea must have worth. When a petition is presented for my signature, I will sign more readily if the names of my friends are already attached. The larger the number of such names upon the list, the more reputable does the enterprise seem, and the more readily do I add my name. The implications, however, may be entirely unfounded. In the fifth place, the activity of commercial designers and promoters furthers fashion imitation. There are people whose business is that of creating new styles which they think will please the pace-setters and the fashion clientele; before a given style has been adopted, other styles are being designed. Then there are the promoters and the advertisers (and the “fashion shows”) who create an atmosphere of expectancy and of favorable anticipation. Fashion imitation applies to all forms of activities; items of wearing apparel are simply outstanding illus- trations. There is imitation—especially in the United States—of every new way of thinking or of acting. The variety of things which gives way to the caprice of fashion is without limit. A striking expression of any new idea will result in the gathering together of a num- ber of people, in the formation of an organization, and in the promulgation of propaganda. In our American “hustle” civilization, fashion imi- tation often partakes of the form of what Professor Ross Imitation 77 has called social racing. Someone sets the pace with reference to a new style, others immediately follow suit, and still others do likewise. As soon as the fashion be- comes somewhat extensively adopted, the originators of it, or the pace-setters, devise a new style, perhaps defi- nitely modifying the initial idea, or perhaps going to an opposite extreme. They set the pace in a new direc- tion, and immediately the preceding style is dropped and the new trail is taken up. In this way, fashion imitation acquires a faster and faster pace. The horde of imitators try to catch up with the pace-setters, while the latter speed themselves up in trying to “sidestep” the pursuing multitude. Gabriel Tarde was the first to develop the under- lying law of fashion imitation in the statement that the superior are imitated by the inferior.2 The su- perior in wealth set the pace in matters pertaining to the consumption of goods. Society women are the idols of debutantes and of “sub-debs.” Famous preach- ers are imitated by the would-be renowned orators. Charlie Chaplin has his clientele of ambitious public entertainers. City people are copied by rural folk. The college upper-classmen set the pace for the lower classmen. The craze and fad are concentrated exhibitions of fashion imitation. The craze is characterized by a 2The Laws of Imitation, pp. 213 ff. 78 Social Psychology large amount of excitement. Under such a spell, peo- ple will temporarily adopt almost any wildcat scheme. If the necessary excitement can be created, the result in terms of imitation can be predicted with a fair de- gree of accuracy. The fad, on the other hand, is a closely related phe- nomenon which arises in connection with novelty, rather than with excitement. Something conspicuously new appears, and because of the prestige accorded to nov- elty, a large number of people adopt the innovation, without giving thought to its merit. Almost any fashion, based on novelty, and suddenly adopted to an extreme extent will serve as an example of fad. There are evidences which show that fashions are changing more rapidly than ever. The pace, in cer- tain quarters, is increasing, due to improved methods of communication, the development of a “hustle” civil- ization as a moving basis, and the rise of inexpensive methods of making imitations of all sorts. On the other hand, the opposition to the tyranny of fashion is gaining ground. Not only is there an in- creasing number of independent voters in this country, but there are likewise growing groups of independent thinkers with reference to fashion changes. There are increasing numbers of individuals who place worth of character above willingness to become slaves to fashion imitation. Fashion, however, serves to accelerate progress. Every invention runs the gauntlet of fashion imita- Imitation 79 tion. If it has worth, as a few fashions do, it becomes adopted universally and permanently, or, until a bet- ter invention in that field is made. It then passes over from the classification of the fashionable to that of the universal. II. Custom imitation is especially common in the years of childhood and adolescence, although it operates, of course, throughout life. Even after one reaches ma- turity, he does things in a customary way without often asking why. The fact that a way of doing has been followed successfully in the past implies present use- fulness. But utility in the past is not in all cases a guarantee of present-day serviceableness, because con- ditions and needs may have changed. Hence, even customs of high standing should be tested occasionally by current needs. A written constitution, for example, may be excel- lent for its day, but a hindrance in some particulars in a later period when new needs—calling for new rules, or direct modification of old rules—have ap- peared. Individuals have established endowments by will for worthy purposes; but changes have ensued and the endowment legacy no longer meets real needs, but cannot be changed if in the meantime the giver has become deceased. Endowments for teaching chil- dren to card, spin, and knit were worthy at the time, but when inventions were made and the occupations 80 Social Psychology of children were changed, the endowment lost its serviceability. It may be asserted that there is a normal tendency for a crust of custom to form over the psychic life of every group. There is a continuous carrying forward of past ways of acting. The group, thus, has to safe- guard itself against stagnation by encouraging a cer- tain amount of inquiry and of questioning in regard to customary activities. If this protective measure is not continuously emphasized, dynamic forces within the group will well up until a revolutionary break is made at some point through the enveloping crust of custom. Revolutions of serious nature may follow; new ideas may thus gain group recognition and adop- tion. This has been a common method of group progress, but one which has been exceedingly costly in terms of human suffering and bloodshed. Under a regime of custom imitation, the leaders are usually elderly men,—at least they are men who have stood “pat” with reference to methods that have grown hoary with prestige. Under the sway of fashion imi- tation, the leaders include a great many men who are not yet in their prime, who are still climbing, and who are willing to try new ways of doing. In physically isolated sections of the earth, custom imitation governs. Likewise in the socially isolated divisions of society, custom imitation rules. In both sets of circumstances contact with and stimulation from the new is lacking. Also, in the socially iso- Imitation 81 lated sections of the individual’s own life, custom imi- tation predominates. In matters of feeling, custom imitation lives long. New ideas, in other words, do not readily affect the feeling side of life. Where cus- tom imitation is strong, new ideas must appear in the garb of the old; where fashion imitation operates, old ideas try to appear new. Custom imitation is non-competitive in the same sense that a certain proportion of contemporary imita- tion is non-competitive. This latter field has been analyzed at length by Professor Ross under the title of conventionality.3 Imitation of the conventional is marked by a type of more deliberate imitation—al- though uncritical for the most part—than custom imi- tation. Otherwise, its nature is psychically similar to imitation of customary ways of doing. The impulse which leads a person to imitate customs more or less blindly impels him to adopt without much analysis the conventional standards. Conventionality shares, however, the field of contemporary imitation with fashion. It is related in the chronological sense to fashion imitation, but in psychic similarities somewhat directly to custom imitation. III. Rational, or merit imitation, includes phases of both customary and fashionable activity. Since customs are 3 Social Psychology, Chs. VII-XI. 82 Social Psychology ways of doing which have stood the test of years, and since human needs change slowly, customs are, as a rule, meritorious. To the degree that customs are adopted, not blindly, but on the ground of servicea- bility, the process is rational. A large portion of cus- tom imitation is rational—but not always deliberately so—on the part of the individual. This generaliza- tion applies, but in a much less degree, to convention- ality imitation; and only in a minority of cases, to fashion imitation. Because fashion imitation rests its case so largely upon novelty and reputability, it signifies irrational ac- tivities. Only to a small degree is fashion imitation worthy of commendation. Of a hundred new fashions chosen at random, less than ten per cent can stand the test of genuine merit. Rational imitation includes a rather large percent- age of custom imitation and a small proportion of fashion imitation. Customs must be submitted con- tinuously to present-day tests, or else they will block social progress. Fashions likewise need to be tested and criticized severely, or they will entail tremendous social losses and dissipations. A small but vital and growing phase of rational imi- tation is socio-rational imitation; this implies the appli- cation not only of standards of merit, but also of tests involved in the term human welfare. Rationality and efficiency, but not necessarily socio-rational tests, are applied in the business world. Strength of char- Imitation 83 acter and efficiency are terms which connote rational methods of living and of working, but they may both be used in highly destructive and disastrous ways to society; strength of character and efficiency are not adequate standards. Socio-rational imitation adds the test of social welfare to that of psychological efficiency. If the latter standard is justified psychologically, the other test has equally strong support sociologically. The two types of tests must be fused and the hyphen removed from the term which has been used in this paragraph to represent them. A socio-rational way of imitating is the most valuable method of imitation known to social psychology. Certain sections of life, both societary and indi- vidual, fall under the control of custom,—as in matters of language and religion, and in those fields where the feelings operate powerfully. Other phases of group life and of the individual’s life are subject to the ca- pricious tyranny of fashion, e. g., questions of dress, amusement, and social conduct—these are externals in which are concerned no “really vital motives of human action.” Other portions of life—both group and indi- vidual—are under the rigid control of rational stand- ards, e. g., the methods for attaining business success. In still other ways, all groups and individuals re- spond sometimes to the highest type of rational imi- tation, namely, that which adds sociality to rationality. Such a standard has been expressed in the principle that “the world must be made safe for democracy.” 84 Social Psychology PROBLEMS (Fashion Imitation) 1. Distinguish between contemporary and custom imitation. 2. Distinguish between the competitive and non- competitive forms of contemporary imitation. 3. Explain: “The telegraph and cash register are universal but not fashionable.” 4. Define (a) fashion, (b) a “style.” 5. Distinguish between fashion changes and prog- ress. 6. Why has Paris been the center from which new fashions have emanated? 7. Explain: “Nothing is fashionable till it be de- formed.” 8. How do you account for the fact that fashions tend to the extreme? 9. Explain: “Fashion is base don novelty.” 10. Explain: “Fashion is based on reputability.” 11. Illustrate how fashion is based in part on the desire for self-individualization. 12. Explain and illustrate: The fashion process has two movements: (a) imitation and (b) differentia- tion. 13. Do you see any connection between fashion imi- tation and the term, “social racing”? 14. Do fashions change more rapidly than formerly ? Imitation 85 15. Would you say that fashion imitation refines or debases one’s tastes? 16. Why is the high gloss of a gentleman’s high hat considered more beautiful than “a similarly high gloss on a thread-bare sleeve”? 17. Why is a given fashion often considered beau- tiful when in style, and unsightly when out of style? 18. Are things beautiful in proportion as they are costly ? 19. Who are the more subject to present-day fash- ion changes, persons guided chiefly by their feelings, or by their reason? Why? 20. “Who are more responsibile for fashion ab- surdities, the women who wear them or the men who are pleased by them?” 21. To whom are the “fashion shows” the greater benefit, the merchant, or the customers? 22. How would you explain the fact that there is less rivalry in consumption of goods “among farmers than among people of corresponding means in the city” ? 23. Why is it easier to save money in the country than in the city? 24. Is it true that the standard of living rises so rapidly with every increase in prosperity “that there is scarcely any let-up in the economic strain”? 25. What is a craze? Illustrate. 86 Social Psychology 26. Who are more susceptible to craze, “a hope- ful, prosperous people,” or “a hopeless, miserable people”? 27. Is a dynamic society more craze-ridden than one which moves along the lines of custom? 28. What is a fad? Illustrate. 29. Make a list of the five leading fads in your community at the present time. (Custom Imitation) 30. Give an illustration of the persistence of a custom. 31. Illustrate: Physical isolation favors the sway of custom. 32. Illustrate: Social isolation favors customs. 33. Why is a custom so powerful in matters of feeling? 34. Why has the dress suit for men remained more or less the same the world over? 35. Why may a man wear the same dress suit for years, whereas a woman must have a new dress for almost every formal occasion? 36. What survivals (no longer useful) do you see in the quaintly cut dress coat? 37. Why are generals retired at about sixty-two years of age? 38. Why are popes and judges generally ap- pointed when past middle age? Imitation 87 39. Do you agree: “The law library is the labora- tory of the law student” ? 40. Why is the display of good manners a custom among the leisure classes? 41. What are the good phases of the caste system? 42. Is it possible for a federal constitution, no matter how well adapted to conditions when written, to become in time an incubus? 43. Illustrate: “Almost everywhere propriety and conventionality press more mercilessly on woman than on man, thereby lessening her range of choice and dwarfing her will.” 44. Explain: “Such generally admired beauties of person or costume as the bandaged foot, the high heel, the wasp waist, the full skirt, and the long train are such as incapacitate from all useful work.” 45. Whence did the idea arise that “manual labor is degrading” ? 46. Why do so many people believe that pecuniary success is the only success? 47. From whs* countries has the United States in- herited its customs? 48. Can you name any customs which have devel- oped in this country? 49. Why are people in old countries more inter- ested in culture than people in new? 50. Does the study of languages tend to develop the habit of conformity to custom? 88 Social Psychology 51. How does the mastery of the classics affect one’s social stability? 52. Why should the foundations of true culture be laid in the classics? 53. How does the ownership of property affect one’s social stability? 54. Is “the proverbial individualism of the farmer’’ the same as individuality? 55. Explain: “Majorities do not necessarily stand for truth and justice. They stand for the customs and convictions of the past.” 56. Why are we blind to the extent of our in- debtedness to society and “therefore apt to imagine our individuality much more pronounced than it actually is” ? 57. Explain: Manners get worse as one travels from East to West,—they are best in Asia, fairly good m Europe, bad in America. 58. Explain: “The neophobia of the old.” (Rational Imitation) 59. What is meant by rational imitation? 60. Mention a custom that is rational; also, a fashion that is rational. 61. Is it irrational to follow authority? 62. Indicate a rational way “of ascertaining wom- an’s sphere.” 63. What are the most difficult foes of new, ra- tional ideas and methods? Imitation 89 64. Which develops a more open, rational mind, the laboratory method, or the text-book method? 65. Is it rational for a religious leader to require his followers “to renounce the extravagancies of fash- ion and to dress simply”? 66. How does the study of hygiene, psychology, and sociology help one to become crank-proof? 67. Why do American women criticize Chinese women for compressing their feet longitudinally when they themselves try “to escape the stigma of having normal feet” by “a formidable degree of lateral com- pression” ? 68. Why do we ridicule the customs and fashions of other people while we remain oblivious to the weak- nesses of our own customs and fashions? 69. What effect does knowledge of the customs and beliefs of other peoples have upon your own cus- toms and beliefs? 70. Does one’s manner of living, or manner of work change the more rapidly? Why? 71. If you were trying to induce “Jews and Christ- ians, Orangemen and Catholics, Germans and Slavs, Poles and Lithuanians” to sink their enmities, how would you proceed? 72. Who has the wider outlook and the freer mind, the average teacher, or the average parent? 73. In what sense is rational imitation radical? In what way is it conservative? 90 Social Psychology 74. In what sections of our lives does (a) custom imitation, (b) fashion imitation, and (c) rational imi- tation prevail? 75. Illustrate: “One of the greatest pains to human nature is the pain of a new idea.” 76. Explain: “Most of us jump into our beliefs with both feet and stand there.” 77. If everybody should become a rational imita- tor, would progress cease because of the lack of people to try out strange and peculiar ideas? 78. Why in this civilized country do so many fashions lack utility? 79. If you had made an invention and wanted finan- cial support, to whom would you apply: a wealthy farmer, a rich merchant, or a well-to-do educator? 80. Does education always imply rational imita- tion ? READINGS (Fashion Imitation) Biggs, A. H., “What is ‘Fashion’?” Nineteenth Cent., XXXIII: 235-48. Foley, C. H., “Fashion,” Econ. Jour., Ill: 458-74. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sect. XI. Linton, E. L., “The Tyranny of Fashion,” Forum, III: 59-68. Patrick, G. T. W., “The Psychology of Crazes,” Popular Science Mon., LVII: 285-94. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Chs. VI, XI. “Acquisitive Mimicry,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 433-45. Imitation 91 “The Principle of Anticipation,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 577-600. “Estrangement in Society,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 350-58. Shaler, N. S., “The Law of Fashion,” Atlantic Mon., LXI: 386-98. Simmel, G., “Fashion,” International Quarterly, X: 130-55. Tarde, G., The Laws of Imitation, Ch. VII. Veblen, T., The Theory of the Leisure Class, Chs. Ill, IV, VII. (Custom Imitation) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. X. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Chs. XVIII, XX. Hearn, W. E., The Aryan Household, Ch. XVII. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sects. XII, XIII. Lang, A., Custom and Myth, pp. 10-28. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Chs. XII, XV. Social Control, Ch. XV. Sumner, W. G., Folkways. Veblen, T., The Theory of the Leisure Class, Chs. IX, XI. (Rational Imitation) Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sect. XIV. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Ch. XVI. Chapter VI. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE GROUP I. Human groups may be classified as permanent and temporary. Permanent groups include the family, the play group, the neighborhood group, the school group, the occupational group, the employees’ and the em- ployers’ groups, the fraternal, the political, the re- ligious groups, the racial and the sex groups, and the planetary group. These groups suffer change, but maintain their general form from year to year, and in the case of nearly all of them, from decade to decade, or for long periods. Permanent groups have arisen out of temporary groupings; the relationship is filial. The order of de- velopment is somewhat as follows: first, human needs, then a temporary grouping to meet those needs, finally, the evolution of a permanent group or social organi- zation. Out of countless temporary groupings, a few permanent types have attained historical prominence; but continuously subject to change and to the laws of social evolution. The Group 93 For example, the family has developed in response to the needs of race continuance; it has gone through the metronymic and patronymic stages and is now in a transitional period, out of which equality of sex authority in the family relation is attaining promi- nence; it has run the gauntlet of polyandry, polygyny, and other forms of marriage, and has achieved a worthy degree of usefulness through monogamy. Or, the psychology of occupations shows an evolu- tion of the following order: human needs, crude ways of meeting these needs, the invention of methods and tools, the rise of specialization, the conscious, uncon- scious, or accidental gravitating of certain individuals into the given occupational group, the appearance of a definite occupational or caste consciousness, and the es- tablishment of an occupational ethics and of occu- pational associations. From another angle, permanent groups may be sub- divided into sects, castes, classes, and states,—a classi- fication which has been outlined at length, by such Continental writers as Tarde1 and Sighele.2 The group in the sense of a sect (la secte) is composed of individuals who differ markedly, but who are united by a common ideal and faith; of this type are religious denominations, political parties, and scientific organi- zations. 1L’opinion et la foule, pp. 177 ff. 3 Psychologie des sectes, pp. 45 ff. 94 Social Psychology The caste is based upon identity of profession; it is the most compact of all social organizations. After one has entered a profession and has become established therein, he has become a member of an existing caste and is more or less under its esprit de corps. For con- crete and tangible illustrations, witness the public atti- tude toward the man who essays to change from the ministry to a business career, from the law to brick- laying, or from teaching to dairying. The class possesses a psychological bond in a unity of interests. The class is less precise in its limits, but more “formidably belligerent in its attitude than the caste.” In this connection, consider the outstanding class distinctions of the day, such as the laboring and capitalistic classes, with their bickerings, strifes, in- trigues, and underlying hatred. States are the most extensive group organizations yet evolved; they rest logically upon common bonds of language, national values, and national prestige. In the closing sections of this chapter, the rise of group values, of a national consciousness, and of national and international patriotism will be outlined. From the genetic point of view, a third classification of permanent grouping is possible. There are instinc- tive and purposive societies. The best illustration of purely instinctive grouping is found among animals, e. g., insect societies. The primitive horde and the family are less instinctive than an insect society, but show a few signs of conscious purpose. The modern The Group 95 state with its constituent groups such as political par- ties, economic corporations, or philanthropic societies is purposive in character; educational associations are strikingly telic. But purposive grouping in its purest sense is still in an incipient stage of development; it always and consciously aims, not simply to benefit the given group, but to advance the interests of society. Permanent groups, thus, include the purely instinctive groups at the lowest extremity of the social scale, the transitional types between, and the purely telic groups at the highest points of civilized development. II. Temporary groups are represented by the crowd, the mob, the assembly, and the public (a quasi-temporary group). The crowd is the most frequently mentioned form of temporary grouping. Some crowds are heterogeneous, i. e., are composed of persons who at the given time have various and con- flicting purposes. A crowd of persons at a busy street corner is a heterogeneous group, i. e., its members have varied purposes and are going in different direc- tions. But the real crowd is homogeneous; its mem- bers at the time have a common purpose. Further, each individual is aware that the other members are moved by the same purpose as is he. The homogeneous crowd always has a leader, but if perchance it is without a guide, it selects one. The members suffer a lessened sense of individual respon- 96 Social Psychology sibility, because responsibility is distributed and shared by all. Anonymity tends to prevail. Excitement may reign; feelings may rise; and rational processes of thought may be blocked. The members experience a heightened state of suggestibility. People are likely to act less rationally while under the crowd influence than when away from it; feelings, rather than reason, secure control. The crowd is said to be recidivistic; the mem- bers revert to lower standards of action than under ordinary influences. Freedom of speech is rarely tolerated by a crowd ; the crowd hoots down any one who attacks its follies. A crowd of financiers would not listen to the harangue of an I. W. W. leader; neither would a crowd of I. W. W. adherents sit quietly under the lashings of a capitalist. A decision which is made entirely under the influ- ence of the crowd has a hard struggle before it. Such decisions must usually be followed up with personal, steady, and sincere attention on the part of interested people. On the other hand, the crowd serves many useful purposes. Time, expense, and energy are saved in unifying people by getting them together in a crowd and by addressing them as a unit, rather than as sepa- rate individuals scattered over a large territory. More enthusiasm for a given project can be created in a crowd than by any other method. Crowd conditions function oftentimes in dragging people out of selfish, The Group 97 individual habits into an open avowal of group aims, into financial support of group movements, and into active group participation. The crowd arouses indi- viduals from lethargy; it gives them new desires; it develops in them new enthusiasm; it unites them in behalf of common purposes. The mob is a homogeneous crowd in an unusually high state of suggestibility. It is a crowd that has be- come frantic, and which has lost its reason. It is fre- quently a crowd which is committing a crime. It is a relic of barbarism and does not have a place in a civi- lized state. An assembly is a group of people harnessed by cul- tural habits, including a set of parliamentary rules of order. These rules of order, according to Professor Ross, serve as a straight jacket upon a monster which is in constant danger of breaking loose. Rules of order function in keeping the feelings down and the reason in charge. Personalities are taboo, the chair must al- ways be addressed, the voting must be by aye and nay, and order must be observed. An assembly is a group of people, in which ideas, rather than feelings are struggling with one another for supremacy. The public is a communicating group with marked permanency and is to be sharply distinguished from a face- to-face group. The communicating group is made possible by the development of the printing press, the railroad, the telegraph, and the telephone. The press has been credited by Sighele with having created the 98 Social Psychology public and having substituted it for the crowd.4 The same author also recognizes the part that the railroad and the telegraph play in developing a public. The railroad has shortened distances, and newspapers thus reach remote places in a comparatively short time; the telegraph has almost eliminated distance, because thereby any news whatever can travel over thousands of miles in a few minutes. Hence the railroad and the telegraph give wings to the press and the feeling of actuality to a public.5 Each reading public tends to develop its own type of journalism; tends to produce newspapers which have its own good and bad qualities, and which are its own creations.6 Large numbers of people, spread over a large territory, read regularly the organs of the given public to which they belong, feel simultaneously the same way in regard to the destruction of anything that belongs to that public, and express their feelings and opinions simultaneously, being aware at the same time that the other members of that public are experi- encing the same feelings and giving expression to the same opinions. 4 La joule criminelle, p. 225. 0 Ibid., pp. 225, 226; cf. Tarde, G., Le public et la joule, Ch. I. 6 Ibid., p. 241. “Sans doute chaque public produit les jour- nalistes qui ont ses instincts, ses tendencies ses qualities, et ses defauts, qui sont, in un mot, es creatures.” The Group 99 The public is not subject, however, to all of the weaknesses of the crowd. An individual can belong to only one crowd at a time, but he usually can claim membership in several publics at the same moment. He may belong simultaneously to a Roosevelt public, a Billy Sunday public, a Ty Cobb public, and a John McCormack public. His interests as a member of one public may run counter to his interests as a member of another public; hence, he will act more normally than when a member of a single face-to- face group. This century is becoming an “era of pub- lics” ; the public is superseding the crowd as a preva- lent form of grouping. III. Conflicts between groups are outstanding social phenomena; they arise from the operation of the fight- ing instinct (discussed in Chapter Three). This in- stinct in the individual when joined with the same tendencies in other individuals assumes mass propor- tions, organized methods, and group significance. Families compete against families for social standing, business vies with business for trade, and nations struggle against nations for territorial expansion and commercial advancement. Conflicts go on continually between the individual and his group. An individual becomes a leader of a clientele and the conflict becomes one between a mi- nority section and the parent group. A new idea 100 Social Psychology springs from some one’s mind, finds expression, and immediately individuals begin to allign themselves with or against the new propaganda. The leader and the adherents of the new program enter into conflict with the parent organization. Conflicts between groups are sometimes open and announced, as in the case of political parties in a na- tional election. They are frequently conducted under cover and behind apparently friendly advances, e. g., rivalries between certain business houses. Even in open political fights, it is often difficult to learn the attitude of various influential organizations, because of secret alliances and agreements. Certain conflicts are highly destructive; others are mutually advantageous. The conflict between a pow- erful corporation and a competitive individual entre- preneur usually ends in the destruction or at least the absorption of the small business of the individual competitor by the corporation. Two neighboring farmers, however, who are competing for honors in regard to corn yield per acre will both gain, as well as society; two granges in competition will also both reap advantages with no losses because of the competition. Conflict between groups is an element of progress, unless the conflict becomes too unequal, unless it as- sumes the form of competitive consumption of goods, both economic and uneconomic, or unless it fails to rise to high, open, and meritorious levels. Conflict be- tween marked unequals results in the annihilation of The Group 101 the lesser unequal, and in no appreciable gain to the other. Competition in the consumption of socially val- uable goods instead of competiton in the production of human values, is socially disintegrating. Conflicts which resort to deception, to physical combat, instead of to increased openness and to a national measuring of values, lead to barbarism and savagery. No conflict would mean no group progress. But conflicts must be kept within the lines of progress, di- rected and made to serve socially constructive ends exclusively. It is at this point that Professor T. N. Carver’s theory of social progress through conflict should be discussed.7 While Professor Carver recog- nizes an evolution in the forms of conflict, he assumes that the group, and particularly a territorial group, is an end in itself. He starts with the elemental type of conflict, mainly destructive, and familiar to us as war, sabotage, and duelling. A higher form is deception, which like the first type is a characteristic of brutes, and is found among human beings in skillful ways of swindling, counterfeiting, and adulterating. A third, higher, and distinctly human form of conflict is persuasion, such as legal (litigating), as political (cam- paigning), as erotic (courting), and as commercial (selling). Then there is competitive consumption, competitive bargaining, and competitive production in the economic realm. Competitive production of goods 7 Essays in Social Justice, Ch. IV. 102 Social Psychology always works well; both competitors and the given social group gain. Beyond this point, the analysis does not go; it needs to be developed in its psychological and sociological phases. It emphasizes the biological bases of conflict; it stresses the survival of the fittest in the sense of the survival of the strongest; it deals little with conflicts between motives, moral standards, and societary values. It is difficult to see how a group whose highest activity is competitive production of eco- nomic goods could avoid the present world-wide con- demnation that has fallen upon Germany. As the economic struggle for existence bulks large in Professor Carver’s writings, so psychological conflicts are stressed by Tarde.8 To Tarde there are three lead- ing forms of conflict, or opposition, namely, political, economic, and social; or war, competition, and discus- sion,—terms which are used to indicate a decreasing degree of destructive action and an ascending scale of constructive opposition. War and competition are usually destructive (the social value of competitive pro- duction of economic goods is underrated) ; discussion is generally constructive (the deception which some- times underlies discussion and the wasteful character of much discussion are not clearly indicated by Tarde). Professor F. H. Giddings has shown how conflicts between groups that are somewhat evenly balanced (secondary conflicts), lead to progress, because out of 8 Vopposition universelle. The Group 103 conflict between more or less equal forces arise toler- ance and compromise, then co-operation, alliance, and mutual aid.9 Conflict as being the best way to remove the dualism between opposing social forces and to ar- rive at some form of unity 10 is a satisfactory statement, providing it is understood that the conflict is to take place upon ascending levels of rationality. Professor G. Simmel speaks of the whole history of society in terms of the striking conflicts which have occurred be- tween socialistic adaptation to society and individual- istic departure from its demands,—a duality which is manifested biologically in the contrast between hered- ity and variation, also, a duality between heredity and environment, or, as Tarde put it, a duality between imitation (heredity) and opposition (environment). Professors G. Ratzenhofer 11 and A. W. Small12 have treated conflict and co-operation as more or less cor- relative terms in the processes of social adjustment. The element of conflict always appears in every new situation, but the line of progress is from a maximum of conflict to a maximum of reciprocity.13 Durkheim has pointed out how opposing groups (subdivided so- 9 Principles of Sociology, pp. 100 ff. 10 Simmel, G., “Sociology of Conflict,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., IX: 490. 11 Die sociologische Erkenntniss. 13 General Sociology. 18 Ibid., p. 325. 104 Social Psychology cial interests) find it necessary to combine in order to advance.14 In a brief summary it may be said that group con- flicts function as means to a social end, operate in the long run upon an ascending scale (war, competition, discussion), and give way to the rise of co-operation, alliance, and mutual aid. They arise out of the fight- ing tendencies, and run the gamut from brutal ruth- lessness to that high type of corrective effort which is an expression of love. They may personify the “tooth and claw” ways of the jungle, or the kindly, but firm methods of a loving parent, and the high-minded, inter- national convictions of a great state; or any of the inter- gradations. IV. In every group, no matter how many intra-groups it may harbor, there is continuous expression of the gregarious instinct, of the spirit of toleration, and of the desire for co-operation. There is an ever present group, or social consciousness; and in a sense, a social mind. It is in the play-day of childhood that social sym- pathy, a social sense, and social habits are evolved.15 It is assumed here that man is inherently social, that he is, in a sense, a product of group life, and that be- neath anti-social activities there is a deep-seated gre- 14 De la division du travail social. 15 Giddings, F. H., Principles of Sociology, pp. 117 ff. The Group 105 garious nature. In associating with others, we de- velop our dormant social nature. Through association, there arises toleration; what we first oppose, we may later learn to tolerate. In associating and in toler- ating, we learn to appreciate the merits of others, to understand their weaknesses, and to develop a fellow- feeling. In associating with the fellow members of our own groups, we learn that they have the same feelings, the same thoughts, and the same willingness to act as we. As a result of this reorganization of attitude we learn to feel, think, and act in harmony with others; by a like process, others acquire similar harmonious reac- tions to us. Public feelings, opinions, and actions, thus, assume tangible form and force. The opinions of the group tend to survive; the most useful do survive. The best current opinion becomes the established opinion in later years; it gathers pres- tige with time. Into this mass of integrated past public opinion and of formulating current opinion, the child is born; in this psychic environment he grows up, and from it his own thinking receives its direction. Later, his individual reaction against some of the elements in this combination of past and present group opinion gives him an opportunity for leadership. Or, he may develop into a leader through championing some factor in the established group opinion. The subject of lead- ership, however, will receive analysis in the next chapter. 106 Social Psychology Integrated past opinion and floating, misty, current public opinion center about certain vital phases of group life. Specific social values arise. A fundamental social value is the existence of the group itself; each group holds its own life as an elemental social value. A corollary to the preceding proposition is that the group will fight for its own unity,—witness the case of the United States in 1861 to 1865. Lack of group unity presages group disintegration. Group life, group unity, and distinctive group pos- sessions, material and spiritual, compose the triune of social values thus far developed in group life. The most important form of the material possessions of the group is its geographic territory. “An abiding affec- tion for the fatherland” and for principles of liberty, of opportunity, and of fraternity which the group may have worked out, represent the highest social values.16 Patriotism is a tangible group response when any one of the social values is attacked. Real patriotism con- sists in a continuous expression of group loyalty, whether the group values are under special attack or not. It shows itself in socially constructive ways in the daily activities of life, and in continuous attention by individuals to the evolution of group values. Patriotism means group loyalty. It originally re- ferred to loyalty to the family, or more particularly, loyalty to the pater, or patriarchal head of the family. “ Op. cit. pp. 147 ff. The Group 107 Patriotism was synonymous with patriarchalism and with “familism.” In the hey-day of tribal society, patriotism implied loyalty to the tribe; patriotism and tribalism were interchangeable terms. With the rise of the nation, a still broader type of group loyalty de- veloped, namely, nationalism, or patriotism in the nine- teenth century sense of the term. Familism and tribal- ism in a modified way take subordinate but vital places in the enlarged form of group loyalty known as nationalism. The most powerful group consciousness that has yet developed is that form of national patriotism which arises in connection with national defense and national attack, which at first is usually highly emotional and charged with electrical feelings but which later settles down, if need be, into a stubborn struggle for group existence. It not infrequently suffers abuse; pseudo- patriotism is common. The pseudo-patriot hoists the flag but locks up coal in his mines while women and children sicken and die from the cold; he buys up foodstuffs, and holds them while prices rise and people starve. Pseudo-patriotism exhausts itself in applause of the flag, or in patriotic statements. It whines when asked to observe meatless days; it squanders money in luxury; it wastes strength in sinful living. It carries the flag as a form of camouflage—to cover up profiteer- ing and self indulgence; it is a hypocritical loyalty that may be as vicious as traitorism. 108 Social Psychology In addition to loyalty to family (familism), to local district, or community (a modified type of tribalism), and to nation (nationalism), the trend of social evolu- tion is bringing a still higher type of patriotism into the foreground, namely, internationalism, or loyalty to the world group. We are on the verge of forming an international consciousness and a sense of planetary values; the new point of view is developed in recent books such as those by Professors Edward Krehbiel17 and Thorstein Veblen.18 President Wilson’s now fa- mous plea for world-wide democracy is a forerunner of the rise of planetary loyalty. Further, philosophy and religion have formulated still more comprehensive group loyalties. For exam- ple, Christianity has dared to project a loyalty which includes not only the present world society, but also that unnumbered host who have run well and finished this earthly race, in fact a vast society of which the earthly group is but a single manifestation. Christian- ity has been so radical that unto familism, tribalism, nationalism, internationalism, it has added universal- ism in the sense of a loyalty to a society (the Kingdom of God) incomprehensive in size and character, without beginning and without end. 17Nationalism, War and Society. lsThe Nature of Peace. The Group 109 PROBLEMS (Permanent Groups) 1. Define a group. 2. Distinguish between a permanent group and a temporary group. 3. How are the sexes different psychically? 4. How is a fraternal group different from a neigh- borhood group psychically? 5. What is meant by the psychology of an occupa- tion? 6. What are the psychical differences between a rural and an urban group? 7. Explain: “The high potential of a city.” 8. Would you say that the capital of a common- wealth should “be its chief city, or some centrally lo- cated town”? 9. Distinguish between the psychical characteristics of Boston, New York, and Washington, the intellec- tual, business, and political capitals, respectively, of this country. (Temporary Groups, the Crowd) 10. Define a “crowd.” 11. Distinguish between a homeogeneous and a heterogeneous crowd. 12. Why does the crowd generally have a leader? 13. If a crowd does not have a leader, what will it do? 110 Social Psychology 14. Why is one’s individuality “wilted in a dense throng” ? 15. Why do feelings run through a crowd more readily than ideas? 16. In order to unify men, why is it necessary to touch the chord of feeling? 17. Why is the crowd-self irrational? 18. Explain: “In a psychological crowd, people are out of themselves.” 19. Explain: The crowd is recidivistic. 20. Does a crowd tolerate freedom of speech ? Why ? 21. Why is the crowd-self ephemeral? 22. What is the relation of Roberts’ Rules of Order to the impulses of the crowd? 23. Explain: “The squeeze of the crowd tends to depress the self-sense.” 24. Is a jury a crowd, or a mere group of indi- viduals? 25. Explain: “A great deal of so-called patriotism is but the crowd-emotion of the nation.” 26. Are your highest emotions aroused more easily when you are in a crowd than when you are alone? 27. What effect will your study of the psychology of the crowd have upon your attitude toward the crowd ? 28. Distinguish between a crowd and a mob. 29. What is the meaning of the word, mob? 30. Is a holiday jam in a railroad station a mob? The Group 111 31. Is the psychology of a mob of Hottentots the same as the psychology of a mob of college professors? 32. Where can the blame for mob action be justly placed ? 33. What are the best means for bringing a mob to a rational point of view ? 34. Define an assembly. 35. Why is it easier to speak to an audience of 200 people than to a group of twenty persons? 36. Define a public. 37. Explain: This is an era of publics. 38. Illustrate a conflict (a) between an individual and his group, (b) between two groups of equal strength, (c) between a small group and a large group of which the small group is a part. 39. Illustrate competitive consumption of goods. 40. Illustrate competitive production. 41. Why do battles always take place between two armies, or between two sets of opposing armies, and not between three or four mutually opposing armies? 42. Why is discussion able to “hurry conflicts to a conclusion?” 43. When is a discussion profitless? 44. What are the leading foes of new ideas? 45. Would you expect to find the truth of the matter in a given discussion with either extremist? 46. Should a false dogma be attacked directly, or undermined “by marshalling and interpreting the ad- verse facts”? 112 Social Psychology 47. Should the chief basis for religious fellowship be “agreement in belief or agreement in ideal” ? 48. Should a conflict between types of water fil- tration or armor plate be referred to the voters? 49. What types of public questions should be sub- mitted to the voters for a decision? 50. Why have theological controversies been more bitter than scientific controversies? 51. What are the strong and weak points of com- promising? (Group and Social Consciousness) 52. What is meant by “group memory” ? 53. Why does the morality of diplomacy and war lag behind the morality of individuals? 54. Why do woman’s legal rights “lag behind her generally acknowledged moral rights”? 55. Define group loyalty. 56. What is a traitor? 57. What is the history of patriotism? 58. What are the leading types of patriotism? 59. Can a good patriot be a bad citizen? 60. How do you rate the patriotism in the senti- ment: “My country, right or wrong.” 61. Do you agree with Professor Veblen’s state- ment: “Patriotism is useful for breaking the peace, not for keeping it.” 62. Distinguish between instinctive and reflective patriotism. The Group 113 63. What is the relation of patriotism to nation- alism ? 64. Distinguish between nationalism and inter- nationalism. READINGS (Permanent Groups) Brinton, D. G., The Basis of Social Relations, Pt. I, Chs. II-IV. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Ch. Ill, Pt. IV. Fouillee, A., Equisse psychologique des peuples europeens. Giddings, F. H., Democracy and Empire, Ch. XIX. LeBon, G., The Psychology of Peoples. Maciver, R. M., Community, Bk. I, Ch. II; Bk. II, Chs. II, III. McComas, H. C., The Psychology of Religious Sects. Ross, E. A., Foundations of Sociology, Ch. VI. Simmel, G., “The Persistence of Social Groups,” (tr. by A. W. Small), Amer. Jour, of Sociol., Ill: 662-89, 829-36, IV: 35-SO. Smith, W. R., An Introduction to Educational Sociology, Chs. IV-VI. Thomas, Helen T., “The Psychology of Sex,” Psychological Bui., XI: 353-79. (Temporary Groups) Conway, M., The Crowd in Peace and War. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Ch. XIV. Davenport, F. M., Primitive Traits in Religious Revivals, Ch. III. Galsworthy, J., The Mob. Gardner, C. S., “Assemblies,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XIX: 531-55. 114 Social Psychology Hamilton, C., “Psychology of Theatre Audiences,” Forum XXXIX: 234-48. Howard, G. E., “Social Psychology of the Spectator,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVIII: 33-50. LeBon, G., The Crowd. “Psicolgia della folia,” Riv. ital. di sociol., Ill: 166-95. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Chs. III-V. Foundations of Sociology, Chs. V, VI. Sedwick, H. D., “The Mob Spirit in Literature,” Atlantic Mon., XCVI: 9-15. Sidis, B., “A Study of the Mob,” Atlantic Mon., LXXV: 188-97. Sighele, S., La foule criminelle. Tarde, G., The Laws of Imitation, pp. 154-73. Vopinion et la foule, Chs. I, II. Tawney, J. A., “The Nature of Crowds,” Psychological Bui., II: 329-33. (Group Conflicts) Bagehot, W., Physics and Politics, Sects. II, V. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Chs. XXVIII-XXX. Giddings, F. H., Principles of Sociology, pp. 100-196. Gumplowicz, L., Der Rassenkampf. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology, (syllabus), Sect. III. LeBon, G. E., The Psychology of Revolution. Morse, J., “The Psychology of Prejudice,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, XVII: 490-506. Novicow, J., Les luttes entre societes humaines. Ross, E. A., “Class and Caste,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXII: 461-76, 594-608, 749-60; XXIII: 67-82. “Estrangement in Society,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 350-58. Simmel, “Sociology of Conflict,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., IX: 490-525. The Group 115 Thomas, W. I., “The Psychology of Race Prejudice,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., IX: 593-611. Vincent, G. E., “The Rivalry of Social Groups,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVI: 469-84. (Group and Social Consciousness) Brown, H. C., “Social Psychology and the Problem of a Higher Nationality,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, XXVIII: 19-30. Coleman, J. M., Social Ethics, Chs. VI, VII. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization, Pt. VI. Giddings, F. H., Principles of Sociology, pp. 100-196. Democracy and Empire, Ch. IV. Hayden, E. A., The Social Will, Ch. I. Howerth, I. W., Work and Life, Ch. XI. Kropotkin, P., Mutual Aid; a Factor in Evolution. Lloyd, A. H., “The Social Will,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol. VIII: 336-59. Maciver, E. M., Community, Bk. Ill, Ch. IV. Ross, E. A., “The Organization of Will,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 145-58. Spencer, Herbert, The Study of Sociology, Ch. IX. Stewart, H. L., “Is Patriotism Immoral?” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXII: 616-30. Veblen, T., The Nature of Peace, Ch. II. Vincent, G. E., The Social Mind and Education. Chapter VII. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF INVENTION AND OF LEADERSHIP I. Invention and leadership are closely related phe- nomena. Leadership, in the broad sense, includes in- venting, discovering, prophesying, organizing, and di- recting natural and social forces. The psychology of leadership rests upon the psychology of invention. Invention means “coming upon,” “seeing into,” and perceiving new relationships. The history of invention is concerned not “with the unoriginal moments of any man’s life, nor with the stolid procession that never had a thought of their own,” but with the bright, happy, creative moments of the best minds of all races and with the most beneficent contributions to the progress of mankind.1 Invention and imitation are opposite poles of the same process; every imitation results in at least a slight modification, or invention. The copying of the 1 Mason, O. T., Origins of Invention, p. 28. Invention and Leadership 117 actions of another involves the personal equation of the one who imitates; to the extent that the individuality of the imitator finds expression in the process, the imi- tation is accompanied by invention. As suggestion is the initiating phase of imitation, so imitation is often the introductory element in invention. As the imita- tor sees life at a different angle from the imitated, he may unconsciously, if not consciously, incorporate new elements into the process,—which is the essence of invention. Invention arises out of individual needs, out of prob- lems, out of attempts to extricate oneself from diffi- culties. The starting point is always a problem; the next essential is a desire to solve the problem; then collection and analysis of data are necessary; finally, a new and useful relationship becomes clear. The inventor may come upon an entirely unexpected rela- tionship; the invention may not be the one for which the long search has been made. But the seeking, searching, inquiring is fundamental to all invention and discovery. A desire, or a problem, concentration of attention, the trial and error method, finally the anticipated, or an unanticipated discovery of relation- ships,—such is the nature of invention. The possibil- ities of making worthy inventions are open, hence, to any energetic mind. An invention is generally not much more than an improvement upon some past method of doing. It is said that the United States Patent Office uses a two- 118 Social Psychology fold classification, namely, improvements, and inven- tions, and that nearly all new ideas and appliances that come to the Patent Office fall under the heading of improvements rather than of inventions. The invention of the steam engine was not made in the year 1769 by James Watt, neither was it made on the day that the attention of Watt was centered upon the rising lid of the tea-kettle. The invention of the steam engine goes back to the aelipile made by Hero of Alexandria (second century, B. C.), to a type of steam windmill made by G. Branca (1629), to the steam apparatus made by the Marquis of Worces- ter (1663), to the application of steam power to various kinds of machines by Thomas Savery (about 1700), to Papin’s idea of the piston, to Newcomen’s piston en- gine, a model of which Watt was repairing when in 1763 he set to work to eliminate the waste of steam due to alternate chilling and heating of the cylinder. With this problem before him, Watt worked for six years before he had perfected the separate condenser in 1769, the date at which it is popularly said that the steam engine was invented. This invention involved more than observation of the lid of a tea-kettle; it included countless improvements made by many minds through a long period of time. The improvements which constitute inventions are of three classes: (1) natural evolutions, (2) transfor- Invention and Leadership 119 mations, and (3) marked deviations;2 the order of statement forms an ascending scale, qualitatively, and a descending series, numerically. Inventions that are natural evolutions of previously discovered relation- ships are the easiest to make and the most common. Some inventions are complex combinations of known relationships; the results are transformations of the constitutent ideas, methods and principles. Then there are the marked deviations from current ideas; these are the outstanding inventions; they involve the recog- nition of relationships apparently unrelated; they in- clude the brilliant conceptions of geniuses. There is psychologically no essential difference be- tween inventing and discovering. Consider the dis- covery of America: first, there was a problem, namely, to travel by direct route to India; then, the brilliant idea that Europe was related to or connected with India by the western seas; then, the search, the long journey, the steadfast westward gaze; finally, land, not India, but a new continent. II. To invention and discovery, leadership may now be added. The leader is the social inventor and dis- coverer. Leadership involves societary problems, con- centrated attention upon these problems, trial and error methods, seeking and searching for correct solu- 2 Paulhan, F., Psychologie de Vinvention, livre II. 120 Social Psychology tions, and the discovery of new societary programs. It is the aim of this section to consider the types of lead- ership, and of the following section to state and explain the qualities of leadership. From one point of view, leadership is of two types, that which drives others and that which draws others. In a military, autocratic country, the former type pre- dominates; in a democratic nation, the latter form re- ceives recognition. He who drives others is usually a representative of a powerful organization. He person- ifies borrowed force; he appropriates ways, frequently autocratic ways, from the institution which fosters him. On the other hand, the leader who draws, must be human. He must be one of the herd and like a good shepherd; he must not get too far ahead of the group lest its members fail to recognize him as one of their kind. In another way, there are two types of leaders; the executive, and the intellectual. The executive is usual- ly characterized by greater physical force, “push,” and energy, but by less breadth of knowledge and by less depth in theoretical thinking than the intellectual leader. He is more interested in people, is in closer contact with life, and is more red-blooded. He gen- erally commands the higher salary, and receives recog- nition from society sooner than does the leader in the fields of literary and scientific endeavor. The intel • lectual leader usually works for ends farther removed, Invention and Leadership 121 leads a less exhaustive life, commands greater freedom and enjoyment, and often is rated higher by succeeding generations. In a third sense, leaders may be divided into four classes: (a) the-crowd exponent, (b) the crowd repre- sentative, (c) the crowd compeller, and (d) the group builder. This classification adds a fourth type of lead- ership to the three-fold division made by Sir Martin Conway.3 The crowd exponent observes the needs of the group, crystalizes the vague desires, and leads the group in obtaining satisfaction for these wants. He is sensitive to the group emotions and is able to express clearly the inchoate group desires and, by oratorical methods, to obtain wide popularity. The crowd representative functions in expressing the more or less clearly expressed will of the people. A judge is a crowd representative. Under the demo- cratic form of a republican type of government, the legislator is expected to represent public opinion. The crowd compeller leads the group, frequently, after false gods, and for purposes of personal gain and glory; Caesar and Napoleon are outstanding examples. The crowd compeller forces his will upon the group, instead of being the spokesman of the will of the group (the crowd representative), or the personification of the unexpressed feelings of the group (the crowd ex- ponent). The crowd compeller hypnotizes his constit- 3 The Crowd, in Peace and War, Chs. VI-VIII. 122 Social Psychology uents, drives them hither and yon at vital sacrifices to themselves, and not infrequently ends his career in failure. His strength is in his hypnotic influence; when that fails he is lost. No leader can finally succeed who stamps out or smothers the self-expression of the group members. The group builder, in the deepest sense of the term, has the best interests of the group uppermost in his mind and heart; his interest is in the welfare of indi- viduals and in the permanent advancement of the group. He analyzes the needs of the group, finds out the lines of probable progress, and dares to lead. He determines the causes of group maladjustments, out- lines steps of reconstruction, and pilots the way. No better illustration could be given than by referring to Washington. III. Of the various specific characteristics of a success- ful leader, (1) a fine physique is essential for certain types of leadership, and helpful in all. Napoleon, however, was compensated for the lack of physique by possessing tremendous energy. A high degree (2) of physical energy and endurance is a corollary of lead- ership. More important, perhaps, is (3) what Ward has aptly called the localization of psychic energy.4 The genius is a person whose psychic energy is highly focal- 4Pure Sociology, p. 36; Ch. XVIII. Invention and Leadership 123 ized. If the process has been carried out by nature, the born-genius is the result; if effected by the indi- vidual himself, the genius by virtue of hard work and concentration develops. The first is a genius by in- heritance; the second, by his own initiative. The former are relatively few and society is often wasteful of them; the latter are more numerous, but the per- centage who attain prominence is small. If nature has not focalized one’s psychic energy for him and made him a genius, he may have the opportunity of focaliz- ing his own psychic energy and of becoming a genius. A leader must be, also, (4) a “moral dynamo,” if he is to succeed in a way worth while. He must com- mand confidence and respect, in a greater degree than does an ordinary person. Ideally, he must be master of himself, before he can command the respect and ac- tions of others. To the extent that he is not in su- preme control of his own passions and desires, he is handicapped in controlling other people. He must have (5) faith in himself, possess a certain initiative and daring, exhibit poise, indifference, and self-control under danger, and by virtue of this superior faith and poise, remain somewhat inscrutable. The leader must be (6) a seer. He must see clearly the real needs of his times; he must perceive them more clearly than do his fellows. His foresight must be su- perior to that of his fellows. If he can see through the problems of his group to their adequate, practical solu- tion, his leadership is at once assured. 124 Social Psychology The biologists and students of heredity have not offered, as yet, a satisfactory explanation of the appear- ance of special talent and of genius. The underlying causes are not known; special ability is as likely, or almost as likely, to appear in a child born in a tene- ment as in one born in a mansion. Its appearance is not confined to one sex; no one knows how much abil- ity is possessed by woman, for, historically, woman has not had opportunity to translate her latent talent into achievement. In addition to the inherited and acquired individual characteristics of leadership, there are social conditions which determine whether or not talent will actually materialize in the form of achievement. It is gen- erally agreed—a point of vast importance—that more geniuses are born than ever attain a place of eminence. The position of Galton that potential genius does not exist, or that every genius will overcome his envir- onment and attain prominence,5 is generally discred- ited. The contention of Lombroso that the genius is a pathological phenomenon, to be treated as a men- tal degenerate, or even as an insane person,6 is without scientific standing. Granted, then, that more geniuses are born than become eminent, the question arises: What are the necessary conditions for the maturing of genius? Sev- 5 Hereditary Genius. ' The Man of Genius. Invention and Leadership 125 eral studies of considerable extent and keen insight have been made; a fairly satisfactory answer is avail- able. Odin7, Ward8, and recently, G. R. Davies9, have shown with increasing exactness that the decisive fac- tors in transforming inherited talent and special ability of all degrees into actual achievement are four: “(1) centers of population containing special intellectual stimuli and facilities; (2) ample material means in- suring freedom from care, economic security, leisure, and the wherewithal to supply the apparatus of re- search; (3) a social position such as is capable of pro- ducing a sense of self-respect, dignity, and reserve power which alone can inspire confidence in one’s worth and in one’s right to enter the lists for the great prizes of life; (4) careful and prolonged intellectual training during youth, whereby all the fields of achieve- ment become familiar and a choice of them possible in harmony with intellectual proclivities and tastes.”10 Genius, then, appears in all classes and strata of so- ciety. Genius tends to create its own opportunities, but often fails. Education, however, of all classes will create more opportunities for the development and ex- pression of talent and genius than talent and genius can make for themselves. 7 Genese des grands hommes. 8 Applied Sociology, Pt. II. 9 Social Environment, Ch. IV. 10 Ward, ibid., p. 224. 126 Social Psychology In times of unrest, change, and transition, leader- ship is at a premium. In times of grave social dis- tress, the autocratic type of leader is the hero; in periods of gradual social evolution, the leader of the “attracting” type is the effective director of human events. Because so much of the world’s history has been marked by social revolution, because the world loves the heroic and spectacular, the hero type of lead- ership has been exalted and the “attracting” type under- rated. Under any circumstances, the problem-solver becomes the effective leader, and the world’s problem- solvers become the world’s leaders. The world, on the other hand, must provide society-wide education and related advantages in order that problem-solving ability may have adequate opportunities of expression. The world’s problem-solvers who succeed farthest in turning human achievement into human improvement, and who are the most successful in enriching the quality of human experience are the world’s greatest leaders. PROBLEMS (Invention) 1. What psychical characteristics are basic in the make-up of the inventor? 2. Can inventiveness be taught? 3. If so, what method of instruction should be used ? Invention and Leadership 127 4. Which is the chief inspiration of the inventor: (a) to secure personal satisfaction; (b) to earn money; or, (c) to render public service? 5. Explain: “Invention is as natural as imitation.” 6. Explain: Inventions are rare but “those who are really qualified to use inventions are also rare.” 7. Distinguish between invention, discovery, and leadership. 8. Distinguish between copying and adapting the methods of others? (Types and Qualities of Leadership) 9. How would you define leadership ? 10. Whom do you consider the five greatest lead- ers in the United States at the present time? 11. What is meant by “individual ascendency” as opposed to “social ascendency”? 12. What is the relation of physique and of energy to leadership? 13. Explain: A leader represents a localization of psychic energy. 14. Explain: It is the work of a leader “to pull triggers in the minds of his followers.” 15. Explain: “The successful shepherd thinks like his sheep.” 16. Distinguish between the intellectual and the executive type of leadership. 17. Are boys reared in wealthy homes, or in poor homes, “the more likely to become executives”? 128 Social Psychology 18. Should a leader draw or drive people? 19. Does progress in social stability and security “lessen the hero values of the leader, and exalt his directive capacity”? 20. Which is the better type of leadership, that which presents fully developed programs to the people, or that which stimulates the people to suggest and de- velop programs themselves? 21. Can a student do a high-grade of assigned and mapped-out work in several college classes, and at the same time develop qualities of leadership? 22. Should an elected representative of the people really represent the wishes of his constituents, or should he exercise his own judgment? 23. Is “the control of patronage” a source of strength to a statesman? 24. Should a general go to the front when techni- cally he can direct the fighting better from the distant headquarters ? 25. How can a leader of splendid ability but of “dissolute habits” be prevented from “demoralizing” the group? 26. Explain: Leadership assumes maximum im- portance in times of transition. 27. What are the basic qualities of a successful pub- lic speaker? 28. What are the main characteristics of a suc- cessful advertiser? Invention and Leadership 129 29. What are the differences in convincing an indi- vidual in the class-room, and in convincing him when he is a member of a crowd? 30. What is meant by the aristocracy of achieving? 31. What is meant by the “great man” theory of progress ? 32. Explain: There is no such person as a self- made man. 33. Have “all advances in civilization” been due to leaders? 34. Would you say that “the obtrusiveness of per- sonality and temperament in literature, painting, and music is a sign of advancement or a mark of back- wardness” ? 35. Should leadership in the family be centered in one person, or should the leadership be divided? 36. Do women generally vote as their husbands? Should they vote in an opposite way? 37. Are the rural or the urban communities in the United States in the greater need of leadership? 38. Why are some of the world’s most valuable leaders unpopular? 39. When should a leader be an agitator, and when should he be a compromiser? 40. Would a course in Leadership, or in the Psy- chology of Leadership, have a useful place in the col- lege curriculum? 130 Social Psychology READINGS (Invention) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Chs. Ill, IV. The Individual and Society, Ch. V. Mach, E., “On the Part played by Accident in Invention and Discovery,” Monist, VI: 161-75. Mason, O. T., The Origins of Invention, Ch. I. Paulhan, F., Psychologie de I’invention, livre II. Tarde, Gabriel, La logique sociale, Ch. IV. Taussig, F. W., Inventors and Money-makers, Chs. I, II. Thomas, W. I., Source Book for Social Origins, Part III. Ward, L. F., Psychic Factors in Civilization, Chs. XXVII- XXXI. Applied Sociology, Part II. Pure Sociology, Chs. XVIII, XIX. Warren, W. P., “Edison on Invention and Inventors,” Cen- tury, LXXXII: 415-9. Wissler, C., “Relation of Culture to Environment from the Standpoint of Invention,” Popular Science Mon., LXXXIII: 164-68. (Types and Qualities of Leadership) Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Ch. V. The Individual and Society, Chs. I, V. Brent, C. H., Leadership. Bruce, H. A., Psychology and Parenthood, Ch. III. Bristol, L. M., Social Adaptation, Chs. XII, XIII. Carlyle, T., Heroes and Hero Worship, Lect. I. Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, Ch. IX. Social Organization, Ch. XXIII, XXIV. “Genius, Fame, and the Comparison of Races,” Annals of the Amer. Acad., IX: 317-58. Invention and Leadership 131 Davies, G. R., Social Environment, Ch. IV. Davis, Jr., M. M., Psychological Interpretations of Society, Ch. XV. Fiske, J., “Sociology and Hero-Worship,” Atlantic Mon., XLVII, 75-84. Galton, F., Hereditary Genius. Gowin, E. B., The Executive and His Control of Men. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (syllabus), Sect. XX. James, Wm., The Will to Believe, pp. 216-54. “Great Men, Great Thoughts, and the Environ- ment,” Atlantic Mon., XLVI: 441-59. Joly, Henri, Psychologie des grands hommes. Le Bon, G., The Crowd, Ch. III. Leopold, T., Prestige. Mason, O. T., Origins of Invention, Ch. I. Mumford, E., “The Origins of Leadership,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., Vol. XII, 216-40, 367-97, 500-31. Nisbet, J. F., The Insanity of Genius. Odin, Alfred, Genese des grands hommes, T. I. Robertson, J. M., “The Economics of Genius,” Forum, XXV: 178-90. Ross, E. A., Social Control, Ch. XXL Tarde, G., La logique sociale, Ch. IV. Terman, T. M., “The Psychology and Pedagogy of Lead- ership,” Pedagog. Sem., XI: 113-51. Ward, L. F., The Psychic Factors in Civilization, Ch. XXIX- XXXI. Pure Sociology, Chs. XVIII, XIX. Applied Sociology, Pt. II. Chapter VIII THE PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL CONTROL AND OF SOCIAL PROGRESS I. Individual initiative continually conflicts with social standards. As a consequence, the individual is subject to many types of social restraint. Many of these social controls have arisen from past group experiences, but these experiences are not always safe guides with ref- erence to limiting current individual action. Nearly all social controls have arisen blindly and have been put into operation without foresight. Social restraint has been rarely telic, in the sense that it is exercised with reference to carefully ascertained standards of group welfare. Nearly all forms of social control, however, embody more wisdom than their haphazard manner of development would imply. Social control is essential to progress. Every group exercises control over its members as a matter of group self-protection and in order that the energy of the members may not be directed in socially disintegrating ways. It is an encouraging sign when a group no Social Control and Progress 133 longer relies entirely upon the blind use of controls, but begins to determine the positive, constructive, and purposive lines of progress. It is a hopeful day when a group sets about to learn the direction in which its greatest usefulness and development lies and con- sciously begins to direct its methods of social restraint and of social encouragement to those ends. Social controls are usually too rigid in certain par- ticulars, too lax in other ways, and too haphazard in many regards. Because social controls generally oper- ate as objective instruments, the individual is frequently misjudged, is coerced unjustly, and is inadvertently en- couraged to foment centers of social sedition; more- over, he is inadequately stimulated to make his best contributions to his group and to society. Consequently, these exceedingly practical and vital questions from the standpoint of group advance con- tinually arise: (1) How much social control shall a group exercise over its members in regard to a given new idea? (2) What shall be the nature of this con- trol? (3) How shall it be applied? If too much con- trol is exercised, individual initiative is stifled and progress halted; if too little restraint is employed, group cohesion is endangered, and social chaos is a pos- sible result. The problem is not only one of quantity of control, but also a matter of quality of control. For example, what kind of control shall a parent use over a child who objectively is telling frstories,” but subjectively is giving his imagination free rein? Shall 134 Social Psychology the teacher use the same variety of control in handling a mischievous boy who is bubbling over with energy as in dealing with one who is deceitful? Shall society use the same controls in prescribing treatment for an obstreperous fanatic as for a delinquent corporation? Also, shall controls be applied bluntly and directly, or shall they be exercised through those who are to be controlled, and hence indirectly? Public opinion is a powerful agent of control. Its merits and demerits have been pointed out by Ross1, Tarde1, Sighele8, and others. Public opinion func- tions immediately; there is no delay as in the case of law. It is an inexpensive control. It is preventive, for individuals fear it, and accordingly modify their con- duct. It is less mechanical than law, strikes into the hidden and secret places of individual conduct, attacks motives. On the other hand, public opinion is frequently mud- dled. It is not precise; it is not codified. It possesses “a short wrath and a poor memory.” It rarely repre- sents group unanimity; an offender can always find some members of the group in whose opinions his of- fense is condoned, excused, or even praised. When responsibility is shifted as is done sometimes in the case of corporate conduct, public opinion becomes confused 1Social Control, Ch. X; also, Social Psychology, Ch. XXII. 3 Vopinion et la foule. 3 La foule criminelle. Social Control and Progress 135 and the guilty persons escape Its lash. Public opinion is primitive, fitful, and “passional” in its methods. Law as a form of social control has already been mentioned in comparison with public opinion. The strong points of law as a method of exercising social restraint are that it is codified, that it is preventive, and that it operates with a certain surety. Its ma- chinery often moves with provoking slowness; it does not search the subjective phases of conduct; and its power is frequently paralyzed by the financial, social, or political power of the offender. Personal beliefs of the individual operate as a social control. Through his home, religious, and educational life, the individual acquires certain personal beliefs which determine his conduct to a large extent. As a result of these beliefs which he holds, he prides himself upon making his own decisions, whereas, commonly, the various groups of which he has been a member— through their teachings—have fundamentally made his decisions for him; he is not “self-made” to the degree that the term implies. Personal religious beliefs, according to which the in- dividual lives continuously under the direction of an all-powerful Being whose eye “seeth in secret,” operate with effectiveness. Law and public opinion can be dodged, but not a Judge who is all-seeing, all-knowing, and all-powerful. Ceremony, ritual, taboo, and art forms are used as methods of control. The prestige of custom is a strong 136 Social Psychology factor in group coercion. Conscious and unconscious imitation of the factors in the current social environ- ment is a positive element of control. Unquestionably the best treatise upon the subject of social control has been written by Professor Ross.4 But in this and similar discussions, the constructive function of group control has been under-emphasized. While historically it is true that social control has amounted to little more than social restraint, there is increasing evidence to show that group control ought to function as a form of social encouragement. It has made itself felt directly upon the borderlines of the group where individuals of initiative are conspicuous, but only in indirect ways upon the central mass. A proper function of social control is that of stimulating every member, not only to contribute to group welfare, but also to make the given group socially useful. It has operated as an instrument of repression, pro- hibition, and negation; it should assume organized and rationalized constructive tendencies. It has carried the role of a “Thou Shalt Not” throughout human history; it is beginning to take on characteristics of social con- struction. In other words, negative social control must share the field with positive social control,—to use terms which have been suggested to the writer by Pro- fessor George Elliott Howard. * Social Control. Social Control and Progress 137 The importance of a positive social control has been overlooked by many well-known authors. Social con- trol of a positive type stimulates individuals to con- tribute their best efforts in support of the development of society; in the highest sense, it is rational, scientific, and telic; it is synonymous with the constructive phases of “social telesis.” It seeks to discover the underlying principles of progress; it works out programs of social advancement; it encourages all individuals everywhere to subordinate standards of individual success and power to ideals of societary welfare; and it strives constantly to change all anti- and non-social impulses into social attitudes. Positive social control stimulates individuals to con- tribute to group welfare; it influences groups to subor- dinate their own advancement to that of the larger groups of which they are parts, and to society itself. Negative social control, on the other hand, has exer- cised a needed but inadequate and often misplaced in- fluence upon individuals. It has unintentionally made the need stand out for a positive social control that would give to individuals in all strata of society ade- quate opportunities for self expression along all social- ized lines of activity. II. Throughout the three preceding chapters and the first half of this chapter definite hints have been given from the standpoint of social psychology of a theory 138 Social Psychology of social progress. There remains, in this concluding section, the need of summarizing and of stating more exactly this theory. Social progress depends upon the amount, quality, and method of application of social control, upon the degree of encouragement as well as of restraint which the group exercises with reference to individuals, and upon the extent, quality, and per- sistence of individual initiative, inventiveness, and lead- ership. Too much restraint means the development of a social crust and of group stupefaction. If there be sufficient individual vitality and initiative, unrest will arise, revo- lutions will ensue, and the social crust will be broken. Thus through revolutions involving suffering, loss of life, and social chaos, the group will progress. If, however, individual enterprise be too weakened and if the body politic has become too flabby, then the crust will thicken and group life will be smothered. If too little or too inadequate control be employed, the cen- trifugal forces will gain undue power, anarchistic ten- dencies will increase, and social disintegration will result. Social progress depends, then, upon individual initia- tive and leadership and upon the forces of social con- trol. These two sets of forces interact in countless ways. The individual, upon the basis of the cultural development of his day, comes upon, accidentally, or finds after a carefully directed search, a new idea, or Social Control and Progress 139 method. This new idea must pass the test of social criticism. If its adoption means the giving up of a considerable section of customary methods on the part of the group members, then a conflict follows. The new is championed by leaders; the old likewise is rep- resented by chivalric defenders. The conflict may be long and drawn out, or short and swift. If the degree of social control be at all normal and if the new pro- gram be of genuine superiority, then it will win its way to group adoption. Upon the basis of this new cul- tural advance, still better ideas will be discovered, at which time the process that has been described must be repeated over again. Thus, the individual initiates, invents, and leads, and the group follows, adopts, and supports new methods. Conflict is necessary for both individual and social progress; it is conflict which gives zest to life, which prompts the highest expressions of individual initia- tive, which brings forth co-ordinated action. The conflict must not take place between forces markedly unequal, lest the weaker be destroyed, and the stronger grow soft and flabby through lack of com- petition. To be most advantageous, conflict must occur between somewhat equal forces. Further, conflict must be subject to social rules, or else it will degenerate and end upon the basis of the lowest forms of physical might and power. These regulations and agreements must keep conflict within 140 Social Psychology the bounds of productive effort—upon physical, eco- nomic, mental, spiritual planes. Within groups, conflicts must be kept open between the official forces and the unofficial organizations. Pri- vate associations must always be free to compete with the public or governmental organizations. The politi- ical party in power needs continuously to face the honest criticism of parties not in power. Two sets of economic enterprise are essential, governmental owner- ship and private ownership; but neither in itself alone contains the elements of prolonged social progress. If taken together, the one works for the public interests, the other secures a high degree of private initiative. But if either were to come into supreme power, group retrogression and degeneration would set in. With all economic resources owned and operated by the gov- ernment a powerful class control would result and individual initiative would be enslaved. With all eco- nomic resources owned by a few gigantic interlocking monopolies, the government would be shackled and put in economic chains, and public welfare would be made subservient to the caprices of the privileged few. The dual existence and operation of public and private organizations must be maintained. The conflict be- tween them must be circumscribed by rules which will keep the competition away from the plane of physical might and upon the high levels of productive and social merit. Neither complete socialism nor complete indi- vidualism alone will guarantee progress. Neither alone Social Control and Progress 141 allows for that degree of conflict which is essential to group advancement. Private associations are needed to experiment with new ideas, to initiate new move- ments, and to prod up the public authorities, keeping them upon high levels of efficiency. The public, or official, organization is needed for the maintenance of the public point of view. In a similar way, the progress of the whole world depends upon a balanced co-operation between large group, or national, units, and the international group, or mankind as a whole. Any world order is unstable that rests upon forty-eight sovereign groups, each de- ciding what is right, honorable, and just for the other forty-seven, and regulated by no inclusive or inter- national authority. The nature and direction of human progress during the past millenium indicate clearly the need for the establishment of a world or international authority, an international constabulary, a planetary public opinion, a sense of planetary values, and a telic program for world progress. National units must give up a definite portion of their present sovereign authority to an international au- thority; national conflicts must not go on upon the destructive levels of physical combat or the destructive planes of economic competition, but upon levels of pro- ductive competition and social benefit. The national units must share their power with a new world-inclusive organization which shall set the rules for all intra-con- flicts; each intra-unit must then play according to the 142 Social Psychology rules of the game and within the bounds determined by economically productive and socially meritorious standards. The group in order that it may progress steadily, must determine the direction which its development may best take. It must also decide continually upon the types of control which it shall use in regard to each group problem that arises. It must, further, put a premium upon individual initiative, new ideas, new methods, new inventions along the lines of its chosen path of development. The highest lines of telic ad- vances lie in the direction of world-wide, planetary human welfare. Such a trend involves the rise of sociocratic thinking, according to which all processes, even the most intellec- tual, must be subordinated to the standard embodied in the development and satisfaction of the progressive and socialized needs of others. PROBLEMS 1. What is the meaning of social control? 2. Give a concrete illustration in which you have felt the effect of group coercion. 3. Is more social control needed in a dense or in a sparse population? 4. In a homogeneous or heterogeneous population? 5. In time of war or of peace? 6. In a society stratified by classes or in a society not so stratified? Social Control and Progress 143 7. Is more or less social control needed in the United States today? 8. What are the dangers (a) of too little social control; (b) of too much control? 9. On what occasions does public opinion arise? 10. What are (a) the advantages of public opin- ion, and (b) the disadvantages of public opinion as a means of social control? 11. Is the sardonic newspaper cartoon more effec- tive in moulding public opinion than the good-natured cartoon ? 12. Which is the more effective in forming public opinion, the cartoon or the editorial? 13. What are (a) the advantages and (b) the dis- advantages of law as a form of social restraint? 14. Why are the laws in the United States so easily broken? 15. What are the strong and the weak points of custom as a type of control? 16. To what leading customs are you subject at the present moment? 17. Does a religious institution or a business organi- zation bind “its members more closely to custom” ? 18. What is meant by the protective philosophy of a group? 19. Is it true that the members of a small group, no matter how meritorious its side of the question may be, are always called “traitors” and other opprobrious names, by an overwhelming majority? 144 Social Psychology 20. Explain: The tyranny of the majority. 21. Distinguish between “the tyranny of the ma- jority” and “the fatalism of the multitude.” 22. Why are opprobrious names applied to refrac- tory members of a group? 23. How do personal beliefs operate as a means of social restraint? 24. How generally are individuals aware of being under social control? 25. Wherein lies the need for social control? 26. Explain and illustrate: The state is more ra- pacious than it allows its citizens to be. 27. Who are the professionals whose business it is to keep up the social order? 28. Distinguish between caste control in India and class control in the United States. 29. Which standards do people think about the more, those of their own class, of the class above them, or, of the class below them? 30. What is the best way to estimate the volume of social control at any time in a given society? 31. Is there reason to believe that in years to come social control wTill be less necessary than now in the United States? 32. Is persecution a good method of securing con- trol? 33. Is there a larger place for authority in settling public questions than in settling private questions? Social Control and Progress 145 34. Is it wrong to punish those who persist in folly that hurts only themselves? 35. Illustrate: “There never has been a society which did not tolerate or approve some conduct that was bad for it.” 36. Which has the greater influence in developing a student, a large university, or a small college? 37. Why is education “the most efficient form of social control in modern society” ? 38. What would be the effect of no social control on progress? 39. In what ways is there too much social con- trol in this country, from the standpoint of securing progress ? 40. In what connection would you urge more con- trol in the United States with reference to accelerating progress ? 41. Are the needs of the individual always in line with group advancement? 42. Are the needs of the nation always in harmony with international progress? 43. Why is it unwise to be either an “individual- ist” or a “socialist”—as these terms are commonly understood—in matters involving humanity-wide progress ? 44. Is there any reason for thinking that the prog- ress of civilization in the United States “narrows one’s options in believing and judging,” but increases one’s opportunities for doing and enjoying? 146 Social Psychology 45. What is meant by natural social progress? Illustrate. 46. Explain and illustrate telic social progress. 47. Distinguish between causes of social revolution and social evolution. READINGS (Social Control) Blackmar and Gillin, Outlines of Sociology, Pts. Ill, IV. Cooley, C. H., Social Organization. Davis, Jr., M. M., Psychological Interpretations of Society, Ch. XIV. Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Chs. VIII, IX, XVIII. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Ch. XII. Foulke, W. D., “Public Opinion,” Nat’l Munic. Rev., Ill: 245-55. Hayes, E. C., Introduction to the Study of Sociology, Pt. IV. Jenks, J. W., “The Guidance of Public Opinion,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., I: 158-69. Patten, S. N., The New Basis of Civilization, Ch. VIII. Ross, E. A., Social Control. Social Psychology, Ch. XXII. “The Principle of Balance,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 801-20. Shepard, W. J., “Public Opinion,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XV: 32-60. Sighele, S., La foule criminelle, Pt. II, Ch. III. Smith, W. R., An Introduction to Educational Sociology, Chs. Ill, XIII. Social Control, Vol. XII, Publications of the American Sociological Society. Social Control and Progress 147 Vincent, G. E. “The Rivalry of Social Groups,” Amer. Jour. of Sociol., XVI: 469-84. Ward, L. F., Dynamic Sociology. Woolston, H. B., “The Urban Habit of Mind,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVII: 602-14. Yarros, V. S., “The Press and Public Opinion,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., V: 372-82. (Social Progress) Bogardus, E. S., Introduction to Sociology, Ch. XVI. Bosanquet, H., “The Psychology of Social Progress,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, VII: 265-81. Dewe, J. A., Psychology of Politics and History, Ch. I. Drummond, H., The Ascent of Man. Ellwood, C. A., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Chs. IV-VIII, XIII. Giddings, F. H., Democracy and Empire, Ch. V. Hayes, E. C., Introduction to the Study of Sociology, Pt. III. Keller, A. G., Societal Evolution. Kelsey, Carl, The Physical Basis of Society, Ch. XI. Kidd, Benjamin, Social Evolution. Maciver, R. M., Community, Bk. III. Tarde, G., Social Laws, Ch. III. Urwick, E. J., A Philosophy of Social Progress, Chs. IX, X. Ward, L. F., Dynamic Sociology, Vol. II, Ch. X. Pure Sociology, Ch. XX. Yarros, V. S., “Human Progress; The Idea and the Real- ity,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 15-29. SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY OF BOOKS Angell, J. R., Chapters in Modern Psychology, Longmans, Green: 1912. Baldwin, J. M., Social and Ethical Interpretations, Macmil- lan: 1906. The Individual and Society, Badger: 1911. Mental Development, Macmillan: 1895. Bergson, H., Laughter, Macmillan: 1914. Bianchi, R., L’etica e la psicologia sociale, Turin: 1901. Binet, A., La suggestibility, 1900. Blackmar and Gillin, Outlines of Sociology, Macmillan: 1915. Boas, F., The Mind of Primitive Man, Macmillan: 1911. Branford, V., Interpretations and Forecasts, Kennerly: 1914. Brent, C. H., Leadership, Longmans, Green: 1917. Brinton, D. G., The Basis of Social Relations, Putnam’s: 1902. Bruce, H. A., Psychology and Parenthood, Dodd, Mead: 1915. Butler, N. M., The International Mind, Scribner’s: 1912. Coe, G. A., The Psychology of Religion, Univ. of Chicago Pr.: 1916. Conway, M., The Crovsd in Peace and IVar, Longmans, Green: 1915. Cooley, C. H., Human Nature and the Social Order, Scrib- ner’s: 1902. Social Organization, Scribner’s: 1909. Cutten, G. B., The Psychological Phenomena of Christianity, Scribner’s: 1908. The Psychology of Alcoholism, Scribner’s: 1907. Bibliography 149 Davis, Jr., M. M., Psychological Interpretations of Society, Columbia Univ. Studies: 1909. Dewe, J. A., Psychology of Politics and History, Longmans, Green: 1910. Ellwood, C. A., Sociology in its Psychological Aspects, Ap- pleton: 1912. An Introduction to Social Psychology, Appleton: 1917. Finot, J., Race Prejudice (tr. by Wade-Evans), London: 1906. Fouillee, A., Psychologie du peuple francais, Paris: 1898. Esquiesse psychologie des peuples europeens, Paris: 1903. Galsworthy, John, The Mob, Scribner’s: 1904. Giddings, F. H., The Principles of Sociology, Macmillan: 1907. Gilbreth, Mrs. L. M., Psychology of Management, Sturgis and Walton: 1913. Gowin, E. B., The Executive and His Control of Men, Mac- millan: 1915. Gross, H., Criminal Psychology, Little, Brown: 1911. Hayden, E. A., The Social Will, Lancaster, Pa.: 1909. Hayes, E. C., Introduction to the Study of Sociology, Apple- ton: 1915. Hobhouse, L. T., Mind in Evolution, Macmillan: 1901. Morals in Evolution, Holt: 1907. Hollingsworth, H. L., Vocational Psychology, Appleton: 1916. Howard, G. E., Social Psychology (an analytical reference syllabus), Univ. of Nebraska: 1910. Joly, Henri, Psychologie des grands hommes, Paris: 1891. Kropotkin, P. A., Mutual Aid; a Factor in Evolution, Knopf: 1917. Lacombe, P., La psychologie des individus et des societes, Paris: 1906. 150 Social Psychology Le Bon, G., The Crowd, London: 1903. The Psychology of Peoples, Macmillan: 1909. The Psychology of Socialism, Macmillan: 1899. The Psychology of Revolution, Putnam’s: 1913. The Psychology of the Great War, Macmillan: 1916. Leopold, L., Prestige, London: 1913. Maciver, R. M., Community, Macmillan: 1917. McComas, H. C., The Psychology of Religious Sects, Revell: 1912. McDougall, Wm., An Introduction to Social Psychology, Luce: 1914. Miinsterberg, H., Psychology, General and Applied, Apple- ton: 1914. Psychology and Industrial Efficiency, Houghton, Mifflin: 1913. Psychology and Social Sanity, Doubleday, Page: 1914. On the Witness Stand, Doubleday, Page: 1909. The Americans, McClure, Phillips: 1914. Novicow, J., Les luttes entre societes humaines, 1904. Odin, Alfred, Genese des grands hommes, Tome I: Paris, 1895. Orano, Paolo, Psicologia sociale, Bari, Lacerta: 1901. Patrick, G. T. W., The Psychology of Relaxation, Houghton, Mifflin: 1916. Paulhan, F., Psychologie de Vinvention, Paris, 1901. Ribot, Th., The Psychology of the Emotions, Scribner’s: 1911. Ross, E. A., Social Psychology, Macmillan: 1908. Social Control, Macmillan: 1910. Foundations of Sociology, Macmillan: 1905. Rossy, P., Les suggesteurs de la foule; psychologie des meneurs, Paris: 1907. Sarfatti, G., Contriheto alio studio della psicologia sociale, 1910. Bibliography 151 Scott, W. D., Psychology of Public Speaking, Pearson: 1907. Psychology of Advertising, Small, Maynard: 1912. Seashore, C. E., Psychology in Daily Life, Appleton: 1913. Sidis, B., Psychology of Suggestion, Appleton: 1911. Psychology of Laughter, Appleton: 1913. Sighele, S., Psychologie des sectes, Paris: 1898. Simmel, G., Uber so dale Differ enzierung, Leipzig: 1890. Social Control, Vol. XII, Publications of the American Sociological Society. Sully, J., Essay on Laughter, Longmans, Green: 1907. Sumner, W. G., Folkways, Ginn: 1907. Tarde, G., The Laws of Imitation, Holt: 1903. Social Laws, Macmillan: 1907. L’opinion et la foule, Paris: 1901. La logique sociale, Paris: 1895. Etudes de psychologie sociale, Paris: 1898. L’opposition universelle, Paris: 1897. Thomas, W. I., Source Book for Social Origins, Univ. of ' Chicago Pr.: 1909. Sex and Society, Univ. of Chicago Pr.: 1907. Trotter, W., Instincts of the Herd in Peace and War, Mac- millan: 1916. Veblen, Th., The Theory of the Leisure Class, Macmillan: 1912. The Instinct of Workmanship, Macmillan: 1914. The Nature of Peace, Macmillan: 1917. Vincent, G. E., The Social Mind and Education, New York: 1907. Wallas, G., The Great Society, Macmillan: 1914. Ward, L. F., Dynamic Sociology, Appleton: 1915. Psychical Factors in Civilization, Ginn: 1906. Applied Sociology, Ginn: 1916. Schmidkunz, H., Psychologie der Suggestion, Stuttgart: 1892. 152 Social Psychology Wundt, W., Elements of Folk Psychology (tr. by Schaub), London, 1916. SELECTED ARTICLES Allen, Grant, “Genesis of Genius,” Atlantic Mon., XLVII, 371-81. Biggs, A. H., “What is Fashion?” Nineteenth Cent., XXXIII, 235-48. Bosanquet, Helen, “The Psychology of Social Progress,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, VII: 265-80. Brown, H. C., “Social Psychology and the Problem of a Higher Nationality,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, XXVIII: 19-30. Cooley, C. H., “Genius, Fame, and the Comparison of Races,” Annals of the Amer. Acad.., IX: 317-58. Dewey, John, “The Need for Social Psychology,” Psycho- logical Rev., July, 1917, 264-77. Folsom, Joseph K., “The Social Psychology of Morality and its Bearing on Moral Education,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 433-90. Foley, Caroline A., “Fashion,” Econ. Jour., Ill: 458-74. Foulke, W. D., “Public Opinion,” Nat’l Mimic. Rev., Ill: 245-55. Fry, E., “Imitation as a Factor in Human Progress,” Con- temp. Rev., LV, 558-75. Gault, R. H., “Psychology in Social Relations,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXII: 734-48. Gumplowicz, L., “La suggestion sociale,” Riv. ital. di sociol., IV: 545-55. Hall, G. S., “Social Phases of Psychology,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVIII: 613-21. Howard, G. E., “Social Psychology of the Spectator,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVIII: 33-50. Bibliography 153 James, William, “Great Men, Great Thoughts, and the En- vironment,” Atlantic Mon., XLVI: 441-59. Jenks, J. W., “The Guidance of Public Opinion,’” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., I: 158-68. Kline, L. W., “The Sermon: A Study in Social Psychology,” Jour, of Relig. Psychol, and Education, I: 288-300. Lazarus, M. and Steinthal, H., “Einleitende Dedanken iiber Volker-Psychologie,” Zeitschr. fiir Volker-Psychologie, I: 1-73. Leuba, J. H., “Psychology and Sociology,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XIX: 323-42. “Methods and Principles in Social Psychology,” Psychological Bui., XIV: 367-74. Linton, E. J., “The Tyranny of Fashion,” Forum, III: 59-68. Mach, Ernst, “On the Part played by Accident in Invention and Discovery,” Monist, VI: 161-75. Maciver, R. M., “What is Social Psychology,” Sociological Rev., VI: 147-60. Mead, G. H., “Social Psychology as a Counterpart to Physio- logical Psychology,” Psychological Bui., 401-408. “Social Consciousness and the Consciousness of Meaning,” Psychological Bui., VII: 397-405. Morse, Josiah, “The Psychology of Prejudice,” Intern. Jour, of Ethics, XVII: 490-506. Mumford, Eben, “The Origins of Leadership,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XII: 216-40, 367-97, 500-31. Ormund, A. T., “The Social Individual,” Psychological Bui., VIII: 27-41. Patrick, G. T. W., “The Psychology of Crazes,” Popular Science Mon., LVII: 285-94. Patten, S. N., “The Laws of Social Attraction,” Popular Science Mon., LXXIII: 354-60. Ross, E. A., “Acquisitive Mimicry,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 433-45. 154 Social Psychology “The Principle of Anticipation,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 577-600. “Class and Caste,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 67-82. “Estrangement in Society,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 350-58. “The Principle of Balance,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXIII: 601-20. Shaler, N. P., “The Law of Fashion,” Atlantic Mon., LXI: 386-98. Shepard, N. J., “Public Opinion,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XV: 32-60. Sidis, B., “A Study of the Mob,” Atlantic Mon., LXXV: 188- 97. Simmel, G., “Fashion,” International Quarterly, X: 130-55. Spender, H., “Is Public Opinion Supreme?” Contemp. Rev., LXXXVIII, 411-23. Tawney, G. A., “The Nature of Crowds,” Psychological Bui., II: 329-33. Terman, L. M., “A Preliminary Study of the Psychology and the Pedagogy of Leadership,” Pedagog. Sem. XI: 413-51. Thomas, W. I., “The Psychology of Race Prejudice,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., IX: 593-611. “Province of Social Psychology,” Congress of Arts and Science, V: 860-68 ; and in Amer. Jour, of Sociol. X: 445-55. “The Gaming Instinct,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., VI: 650-63. Tosti, G., “Social Psychology and Sociology,” Psychological Rev., V: 347-81. Vincent, G. E., “The Rivalry of Social Groups,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XV: 469-84. Bibliography 155 Wilcox, D. F., “The American Newspaper. A Study in Social Psychology,” Annals of the Amer. Acad., XVI: 56-92. Woolston, H. B., “The Urban Habit of Mind,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XVIII: 602-14. Yarros, V. S., “The Press and Public Opinion,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., V: 372-82. “Human Progress: The Idea and the Reality,” Amer. Jour, of Sociol., XXI: 15-29. INDEX Acquisitive instinct, 45 Admiration, 49 “Alter,” the, 51 ff. Agreeable tone of conscious- ness, 26 ff. America, discovery of, 119 American Sociological So- ciety, 18 Angell, J. R., cited, 26 Anger, emotion of, 42, 48 Animal life, 24, 41, 44 Assembly, 97 Baldwin, J. M., cited, 101 Beliefs, personal, 135 Biological struggle, 43, 102 Builders of groups, 122 Carver, T. N., cited, 101 Caste, 94 Character, 50, 51 Child life, 46 Christianity, 108 City, high potential life of, 29 Class, the, 94 Cognition, 28 ff. Combative instinct, 42 Communicating group, 97 ff. Competition, 101, 102 Conduct, social, 40 Conflict, 49, 103, 139 ff. Between groups, 99 ff., 139 ff. Constructive, 100 Destructive, 100 Consciousness, appearance of, _ 24, 26 Social, 52 Tone of, 26 ff. Contemporary imitation, 66, 74 Conventionality imitation, 81 Conway, Sir Martin, cited, 12i Cooley, C. H., cited, 17 Craze, 77 Crisis, 24 Crowd, 95 ff. Crowd compeller, 121 Crowd exponent, 120 Crowd representative, 120 Current opinion, 110 Custom, 83 Custom imitation, 66, 79 ff. Davies, G. R., cited, 125 Davis, Jr., M. M., cited, 17 Direct suggestion, 63 ff. Disagreeable tone of con- sciousness, 26 ff. Discovery of America, 119 Disposition, nature of, 50 Durkheim, E., cited, 103 Education, social value of, 125 Efficiency, 83 Ego, 51 ff. Egoistic interests, 40 Ellwood, C. A., cited, 17, 26 Index 157 Emotion, nature of, 48, 49 Of anger, 42, 48 Ennui, 49 Environment, a favorable, 125 Era of publics, 99 Executive type of leader, 120 Exponents of groups, 121 Fad, 78 Familism, 107 Family, institution of, 39, 40, 93 Fashion imitation, 66, 83 Five factors in, 74 ff. Irrational, 82 Tyranny of, 78 Feeling, 26 Fighting instinct, 42 ff. Modification of, 44 Fittest to survive, 43 Focalization of psychic en- ergy, 122 Freedom, margin of, 30, 31 Of speech Function of social control, 136 I Galton, Francis, cited, 124 Genius, 122 ff. Gesture, basis of language, 59 Giddings, F. H., cited, 102 Governmental ownership, 140 Gregarious instinct, 38, 39, 104 Group life, 14 Group loyalty, 106 ff. Group stupefaction, 138 Groups, leaders of, 116 ff. Builders of, 122 Exponents of, 121 Representatives of, 121 Habit, 24 Advantages of, 25 Hate, 50 Heredity and genius, 124 Heterogeneous crowd, 95 Hobhouse, L. T., cited, 18 Homogeneous crowd, 95 Howard, G. E., cited, 17, 18, 136 Hypnotism, 63 Idea, dynamic nature of, 30 Imitation, 62 ff., 116 Social operation of, 74 ff. Socio-rational, 82 Indirect suggestion, 63 ff. Individual initiative, 138, 140 Individualism, 45 Instinct, 22 ff., 24, 38 ff. Acquisitive, 45 Combative, 42 ff. Gregarious, 38 Inquisitive, 41 Parental, 39, 40 Sex, 39 Instinctive society, 94 Intellectual progress, 41 Internationalism, 108 Inquisitive instinct, 40 Intellectual type of leader, 120 Invention, 65 ff., 116 ff. Of steam engine, 118 Krehbiel, E., cited, 108 Language, origin of, 59 Laughter, 60 Causes of, 61, 62 Law, 135 Leadership, 105, 116 ff. Le Bon, Gustave, cited, 18 Lombroso, C., cited, 124 Looking-glass self, 52 Love, types of, 49 ff. Loyalty, group, 106 ff. 158 Index McDougall, Wm., cited, 16, 23, 64 Mead, G. H., cited, 60 Merit imitation, 66, 81 ff. Militarism, 43 ff. Miller, I. E., cited, 26 Mind in action, 22 Mob, nature of, 97 Moore, E. C., cited, 15 Moral dynamo, 123 Nationalism, 107, 140 Negative social control, 136, 137 Nicolai, G. F., cited, 43, 44 Normal suggestibility, 64 Novelty, 75 Occupation, psychology of, 93 Odin, Alfred, cited, 125 Organizations, public, 140 Private, 140 Opinion, current, 105 Past, 106 Public, 134 Parent, functions of, 39, 40 Parental instinct, 39 ff. Patriotism, 106 ff. False type of, 107 Permanent group, 92 Personal beliefs, 135 Play, nature of, 46 Commercialization of, 47 Positive social control, 136, 137 Potential genius, 124 Potential of city, high, 29 Private organizations, 140 Private ownership, 140 Problem-getting method of study, 15 Problem-solving, 126 Progress, nature of, 132, 137 ff. Pseudo-patriotism, 107 Psychic energy, focalization of, 122 Psychology, 22 Of invention, 116 ff. Of leadership, 105, 109 ff. Of occupation, 93 Of social control, 132 ff. Of social progress, 137 ff. Of the crowd, 95 ff. Of the group, 92 ff. Of war, 43 ff. Public, the, 97 Era of, 99 Public opinion, 134 Public organizations, 140 Purposive society, 94, 95 Racial experience, 28 Racial history, 27 Rational imitation, 66, 81 ff. Ratzenhofer, G., cited, 103 Reason, 29 Religious beliefs, 135 Representative leaders of groups, 121 Reputability, 75 Ross, E. A., cited, 16, 29, 63, 64, 76, 97, 134, 136 Scott, W. D., cited, 25 Seashore, C. F., cited, 25 Sect, nature of, 93 Seer, 123 Self, development of, 51 Self-consciousness, 52 Self-individualization, 75 Sentiment, nature of, 49 Sex instinct, 39 Sidis, Boris, cited, 61 Sighele, S., cited, 93, 97, 134 Simmel, G., cited, 103 Index 159 Slantwise suggestion, 64. Small, A. W., cited, 103 Social conduct, 40 Social consciousness, 52 Social control, nature of, 132 ff. Social criticism, 139 Social environment, 28 ff. Social instincts, 13 Social nature, 105 Social progress, 41, 137 ff. Social psychology, 13, 14, 22 Literature of, 16 “Social racing,” 77 Social restraint, 132 Social self, 51 Social values, 106 Society, 94 Sociocratic thinking, 142 Socio-rational imitation, 82, 83 Special ability, 124 Speech, freedom of, 96 State, the, 94 Struggle, biological, 43, 102 Style, nature of, 75 Suggestion, 62 ff. Direct, 63 ff. Indirect, 63 ff. Immediate, mediate, con- tra, 64 ff. Suggestibility, 64, 96 Variability of, 65 Sumner, W. G., cited, 18 Sympathy, 47, 48 Talent, 124 Tarde, Gabriel, cited, 17, 77, 93, 102, 103, 134 Telic progress, 142 Temperament, 51 Thomas, W. I., cited, 18, 24 Tribalism Veblen, Thorstein, cited, 108 Volition, 30, 31 Wallas, Graham, cited, 18 War, psychology of, 43 ff., 102 Ward, L. F., cited, 122, 125 Washington, George, 122 Watt, James, 118 Wilson, Woodrow, cited, 108 World’s problem-solvers, 126 World society, a, 141