ARMY MEDICAL LIBRARY WASHINGTON Founded 1836 Section. Number ..//JbJjJ^. bpo 3—10543 Form 113c, W. D.. S. G. O. (Revised June 13, 1936) THE ANATOMY OF THE BRAIN, A GENERAL VIEW OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM. By J. G. SPURZHEIM, M. D. IT* Of the Universities of Vienna and Paris; Licentiate of the Royal Col. of Phys. in London. TRANSLATED FROM THE UNPUBLISHED FRENCH MS. BY R. WIL- LIS, MEMBER OF THE ROYAL COLLEGE OF SURGEONS, LONDON. WITH AN APPENDIX, AND EIGHTEEN PLATES. SECOND AMERICAN EDITION REVISED BY sf CHARLES H. STEDMAN.M. D. Physician and Surgeon to the United States Marine Hospital, Chelsea. A^/I^n. BOSTON: MARSH, CAPEN & LYON. 1836. Entered according to Act of Congress, in the year 1834, by Marsh, Capen & Lyon, in the Clerk's Office of the District Court of Massachusetts. WL Printed by William A. Hall & Co. CONTENTS. PAGE. Preface to the American Edition.............................v Preface to the English Edition.............................xiii Section I.—General Considerations...........................9 Of the Existence of the Pulpy and Fibrous Substances......10 Of the Structure of the Two Nervous Substances...........11 Of the Use of the Pulpy Nervous Substance................14 Of the Origin of the Nervous System.....................17 Of the Mode of Formation of the Nervous System..........22 Of the General Form of the Nervous System...............24 Of the Structure and Use of Ganglions....................25 Section II.—Division of the Masses composing the Nervous System..............................................29 Section III.—Of the Nervous Masses of Voluntary Motion, and of the External Senses................................41 Of the Nervous Mass of the Vertebral Column.............54 General Considerations on the Spinal Cord of Man and Ani- mals ..................................................54 Particular Considerations in regard to the Spinal Cord......64 Of the Pretended Cerebral Nerves, &c.....................70 Of the Accessory Nerve.—Spinal, or Spinal Accessory of Wil- lis.—3d Branch of the 8th pair of J. Bell.—Superior respiT ratory of C. Bell......................................73 Of the Pneumogastric Nerve.—Par Vagum.—Middle Sympa- thetic.__Vocal.—Pulmonary 2d branch of the 8th pair.......74 Of the Glossopharyngeal Nerve...........................75 Of the Hypoglossal Nerve.—9th pair of Willis.—Gustatorious of Winslow.—Lingualis of Vic d'Azyr.—Sublingual.......75 Of the Abductor Nerve of the Eye.—6th pair of Willis.—Ex- ternal Motor of Meckel.................................75 IV CONTENTS. Of the Facial Nerve.—Portio Dura of the 7th pair.—Nervicus Communicans faciei of J. Bell.........................• • 76 Of the Motor Nerve of the Eye.—3d pair of Willis.—The Com- mon Motor of Meckel...................................76 Of the Nerve of the Superior Oblique Muscle of the Eye.—4th pair or Patheticus of Willis.—Trochlearis.—Internal Motor of Meckel..............................................77 Of the Trigeminal Nerve.—5th pair of Willis.—Trifacial of Meckel................................................77 Of the Auditory Nerve.—Portio Mollis of the 7th pair.—Acous- tic Nerve..............................................79 Of the Optic Nerve.—2d pair of Willis ....................80 Of the Q,uadrigeminal Bodies ...........................84 Of the Olfactory Nerve.—1st pair of Willis.—Ethmoidal Nerve..................................................91 Section IV.—Of the Best Method of Dissecting the Brain.....95 Section V.—Of the Cerebellum............................105 Section VI.—Of the Brain.................................135 Decussation of the Anterior Pyramids.....................140 Two Portions of the Crura and Cerebral Masses...........145 Comparative Anatomy of the Brain.....................152 Structure of the Convolutions___.........................155 Section VII.—Of the Commissures of the Nervous Appara- tuses...............................................167 Section VIII.—Of the Communications of the Nervous Appara- tuses ...............................................182 Section IX.—Of the Anatomico-Physiological Relations of the Nervous Apparatuses.................................190 Explanation of the Plates and Figures.....................200 Appendix....................................*..... 221 On the Brain, as an Aggregation of Parts..................221 The Parts of the Human Brain in the ordinary State of Health are essentially the same, and only modified in Size and Quality..........................................223 In certain Idiots, Individual Portions of the Brain are defect- ive, or even wanting...................................230 The Brain of the Ourang-Outang does not contain all the Parts of the Human Brain..............................232 Some Remarks on Mr. Charles Bell's Animadversions on Phrenology.................................. 23« PREFACE TO THE AMERICAN EDITION. In presenting to the public an American edition of Spurzheim's Anatomy of the Brain, the editor feels that the opportunity now offering should not be allowed to escape, without adding his feeble tes- timony to the facts herein advanced, — facts which have gone farther to establish, elucidate, and per- fect the physiology of the brain, than any discov- eries ever before made. What Harvey, J. Hunter, and Linnseus, in their several researches into the animal kingdom, have effected, the same have Gall and Spurzheim accomplished;—the establishment of a foundation, on which will rest all subsequent discoveries in their respective branches of science. But, alas! all this good acquired and bestowed, has not always met with that applause and cordi- ality of reception which was its due. Who does not know with what indifference and neglect the facts observed and proclaimed by the immortal Harvey, were treated — who lived to see scarcely a solitary convert to his opinions 1 The facts and observations of Hunter and Jenner were subjected B vi PREFACE. to nearly similar contumely and incredulity. So has it been with Gall, and his no less illustrious associate. It wruld have been astonishing, had not their labors likewise been held up to the world as profitless and vain. How, may it be asked, could Gall and Spurzheim expect to escape censure, with the fate of Galileo, Harvey, and other philos- ophers, before their eyes 1 The history of science swells with the multiplied accounts of the persecu- tion and neglect of its successful devotees; so much so, that in fact, the value of a discovery or inven- tion would seem to be in the ratio of the abuse and indignity heaped upon it. Though both Gall and Spurzheim lived through much reproach, yet long before death they enjoyed the bright satisfaction of witnessing the rapid march of their opinions. Concessions to the truth of their observations poured in upon them from all quarters; slowly, indeed, but with a steady and increasing progress. France has honored Gall, as she should, with that generous regret she always bestows upon the remains of those who have hon- ored her. Great Britain and New-England, who knew Spurzheim well, have striven to do justice to his character and remains, by wiping away the obloquy and reproach cast upon his successful la- bors, and by bestowing on him the highest and only honors the living can heap upon the dead. PREFACE. vii Already does Phrenology number among its ad- vocates in Great Britain some of the most scientific men of that country ; and its march has been such, that the Reviews and Magazines, once so bitterly opposed to ' German fancies,' to Spurzheim and his doctrines, have joined themselves to his friends, and heartily repent of their former inveterate hos- tility. Some injudicious, though warm and talented supporters of the new system of mental philosophy have chosen to elevate the admirable Gall at the expense of the character and invaluable services of Spurzheim: but Time, who proves all things, will judge between these eminent co-workers in the cause of truth. Time will prove that the founda- tion of the science of Phrenology was laid by Gall; but, that without the aid of Spurzheim, the super- structure had not been reared. As to the comparative merits of these two great men, it may be proper to subjoin the following ex- tract from the Edinburgh Phrenological Journal, No. 6, page 189-90 : 1 As Newton was the first to establish on a firm and solid basis the connexion of physical with mathematical science, so Gall has been the first to demonstrate on proper principles a connexion between physical qualities and the manifestations Vlll PREFACE. of mind. La Place verified, illustrated and per- fected the discoveries of Newton ; and Spurzheim has verified, illustrated, and brought to a state nearly as perfect, the discoveries of Gall.' Again, as to the respective value of their writings: — c When we enter upon the perusal of Gall, we feel as if we were in a country abounding with objects of the most striking sublimity, and rich in all the dread magnificence of nature. We wander untired through the boundless variety, and we perceive in every new scene something to excite our wonder and admiration. In the works of Spurzheim, on the other hand, we feel as in a garden; where all is regular and orderly; where all the different pro- ductions of nature are placed in an exact scientific arrangement; where we may study them leisurely and at our ease; and where we may see brought together, in a comparatively small space, the pro- duct of every zone and of every climate in the known world.' Again, in No. 9, page 107: < In his own [Gall's] science he never can have a rival......A man who is to be named only with the Harveys, and Galileos, and Newtons, has nothing to dread from any com- petitor; and in his own department there never can be found any similis a,ut secundus. The prox- imos illi honores have certainly been occupied by Dr. Spurzheim; and as Dr. Johnson has remarked PREFACE. ix of Milton in regard to epic poetry, so it may be said of Dr. S., that he is not the greatest of Phrenolo- gists, only because he is not the first. His contri- butions to the science betoken powers of the very first order; and his services to Phrenology no pos- sible circumstance can ever make us forget or un- dervalue, nor will posterity ever forget or under- value them.' It does not belong to us here, nor is there space to point out the particular share that Gall or Spurz- heim had in the anatomical portion of this work. The author has given Gall, as far as practical util- ity demanded, all the credit due him, and has taken nothing upon himself that was not rightfully his own. No one will question the unparalleled skill, ease, and perspicuity with which Spurzheim laid open to his anatomical classes the heretofore hidden myste- ries of the brain. In Boston, where we had been accustomed to the ancient method of exposing, or rather keeping out of view the structure of the cer- ebral masses by slicing the brain, each demonstra- tion of Spurzheim seemed to his attentive audience as a ray of light beaming through the mist, and dispersing the bewildering haze in which the sub- ject had before been enveloped. We had heard, indeed, that something of the kind had been accom- plished by this same Spurzheim in Europe; but x PREFACE. the strength of our incredulity had not been put to the test. The presence of this genuine anatomist actually demonstrating the brain, and displaying its organization, had not hitherto been granted us. At length, however, he came, and taught, until, with most, if not all his auditors, unbelief and prej- udice began to vanish by degrees, and the con- sciousness of truth to reign in their stead. No one, then present, can say in sincerity that he was not instructed; but rather, with the exception perhaps of two or three, all will pronounce themselves, not only instructed, but converted. The proof of the extension of Spurzheim's views in this country, may be found in the sale of his works, and in the continued call for further edi- tions. This edition of his Anatomy of the Brain may be considered somewhat in the light of a sec- ond American edition; there having been 250 cop- ies ordered by himself from England, which arrived after his death, all of which have been sold. Not only, professional men and anatomists, are anxious to obtain the < Anatomy of the Brain,' but the sci- entific lovers of truth not of the profession, likewise desire to examine the validity of the facts embraced in the work. So far as this has been done, convic- tion as to the grand points, has followed. For my own part, being so situated as to enjoy frequent op- portunities for dissecting the brain, I have carefully PREFACE. Xi investigated the cerebral masses according to this mode of examination. By this method, (and there seems to be no other systematic course,) the true structure of the brain, from its commencement in the medulla oblongata, to the termination of its di- verging fibres in the pulpy substance, and the union of its converging fibres, by which all parts are brought into perfect connexion, is discovered; to- gether with the reinforcement of these fibres, and their successive additions, by means of the gray substance distributed throughout all parts of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Thus the fact of the plurality of organs in the brain, is established, to- gether with their constant intercommunication. If, with the knowledge of this important anatom- ical disposition of the cerebral organs, we now connect the multiplied and multiplying facts derived from the examination of the exterior of the crani- um, and also the observations made upon the intel- lectual and affective faculties of man, together with the state of the brain and mental manifestations in disease of that organ, we cannot easily avoid the conclusions arrived at by Gall and Spurzheim, that the faculties of the mind are innate, and that they possess a habitation in the brain well characterized and defined. The share the editor has assumed in the republi- cation of this edition, is but small. It evidently ap- xn PREFACE. pears, from a careful perusal, that the translation and publication of the London edition was hurried ; for this reason, some passages, the true meaning of which was somewhat ambiguous, owing perhaps to the difficulty of translating literally, and at the same time correctly, from the French manuscript, have been altered, and their phraseology adapted to the character of our own language. Several typo- graphical errors have likewise been corrected; and a few almost unimportant notes added. Much more, unquestionably, could have been ef- fected by way of increasing the size of the book ; but the editor has many doubts as to the real value which might result from such an undertaking. Pages after pages might have been filled with spec- ulations and unestablished opinions; by which, how- ever, the real advancement of the knowledge of the anatomy of the brain would not have been effected. It has been preferred, therefore, to let the facts here displayed stand as they are, — in the simplicity and dignity with which they have been clothed by the immortal Spurzheim. U. S. Marine Hospital, ) Jan. 13, 1834. 5 PREFACE. The affective and intellectual faculties of man, both in their healthy and diseased condition, are un- questionably dependent on the body; and, among the various branches of anthropology, anatomy is the basis of all the others. The organic apparatus- es, which are indispensable to the mental manifes- tations, consist of the brain, cerebellum, spinal cord, and the nerves of the external senses and voluntary motion. To make known the structure of these parts, is the special object of this volume. History proves that the structure and destination of the nerves were long unknown. Hippocrates and Aristotle, for instance, confounded, under the same general title, ligaments, tendons, nerves, and even blood-vessels. Hippocrates believed that the nerves terminated in muscles and bones, and pro- duced voluntary motion. Herophilus, who lived nearly three centuries before the commencement of the vulgar era, was the first who discovered the con- c xiV PREFACE. nexion of the nerves with the brain, and who looked on them as the instruments of sensation. Erasistra- tus divided the nerves into those of sensation and those of motion; the first he derived from the brain, the second from the membranes. Galen held that the. nerves of sensation arose from the brain, and those of voluntary motion from the spinal cord. In the sixteenth and succeeding centuries, the brain and nerves were subjects of much research; but it is only in our own times that they have begun to be understood, — that their true structure has been discovered, and that new and unthought-of func- tions have been proved to belong to them. The nervous being the most delicate tissues of the body, necessarily required extremely careful and often-repeated examination to be understood; and this they could not receive during those ages when prejudice opposed insurmountable obstacles to the dissection of dead bodies. It is therefore easy to conceive why such slow progress was made in the anatomical knowledge of the nervous system. Dr. Gall is the original author of a new physiolo- gical doctrine of the brain. The discovery of the ground-work of this is all his own; and he had even gone very far in rearing the superstructure before the year 1804, when I became his colleague. From this period we continued laboring in common until 1813, when our connexion ceased, and each began PREFACE. XV to pursue the subject for himself. The works which Dr. Gall has published in his own name, fix the extent of his phrenological knowledge. My ideas are developed in my own publications : histo- ry will assign to each of us his share in the works that have issued under our joint names. It was in the year 1800, that I attended for the first time the private course of lectures which Dr. Gall had been in the habit of delivering occasion- ally at his house for four years. At this time he spoke of the necessity of the brain to the manifesta- tions of the mind, of the plurality of the mind's or- gans, and of the possibility of discovering the devel- opment of the brain by the configuration of the head. He pointed out several particular organs of different memories, and of several sentiments, but he had not yet begun to examine the structure of the brain.* Between 1800 and 1804, he modified his physiological ideas, and brought them to the state in which he professed them at the commence- ment of our travels, t Dr. Gall having met with a woman, fifty-four years of age, who from her infancy had labored * Exposition de la Doctrine de M. Gall, par Froniep, 3me edit. 1802. f Bischoff Exposition de la Doctrine de M. Gall, sur le Cerau et le Crane, Berlin, 1805; et Blcede, la Doctrine de Gall sur les Fonc- tions du Cerveau, Dresden, 1805. XVI preface. under dropsy of the brain, and who, nevertheless, was as active and intelligent as the generality of females in her own rank of life, and being convinc- ed that the brain wras the indispensable organ of the soul, expressed himself in terms similar to those which Tulpius had used before him, on observing a person afflicted with hydrocephalus, who exhibit- ed good intellectual faculties, viz., the structure of the brain must be different from what it is common- ly supposed to be.* He now felt the necessity of examining the mind's organ anatomically. As his medical practice occupied his time, he employed M. Niclas, a student to dissect for him; but the spirit of this gentleman's researches was merely mechanical, as is allowed in our joint work, enti- tled 'Anatomie et Physiologie, du Systeme nerveux en general, et du Cerveau en particulier.' t Having completed my studies in 1804, I became associated with Dr. Gall, and devoted myself espe- cially to anatomical inquiries. At this period, Dr. Gall, in the Anatomy, spoke of the decussation of the pyramidal bodies, of their passage through the pons Varolii, of eleven layers of longitudinal and transverse fibres in the pons, of the continuation of the optic nerve to the anterior pair of the quadri- geminal bodies, of the exterior bundles of the cru- * Lancet, Vol. x. p. 244. t Preface to the first vol. p. 16. preface. xvii ra of the brain diverging beneath the optic nerves in the direction which Vieussens, Monro, Vicq d' Azyr, and Reil,* had followed, the first, by means of scraping, the others, by cutting the substance of the brain. Dr. Gall showed, further, the continua- tion of the anterior commissure across the striated bodies ; he also spoke of the unfolding of the brain that happens in hydrocephalus. The notion he had conceived of this, however, was not correct; for he thought that the convolutions resulted from the duplicature of a membrane, believing that the cerebral crura entered the hemispheres on one side, expanded there, and then folded back on them- selves by the juxtaposition of the convolutions. The true structure of the convolutions, and their connexion with the rest of the cerebral mass, were not described until our joint Memoir was presented to the French Institute in 1808. The mechanical direction which the anatomical investigations had taken, did not appear to me sat- isfactory. Guiding myself in my inquiries by phys- iological views, always comparing structure with function, I discovered the law of the successive ad- ditions to the cerebral parts; the divergence in every direction of the crural bundles towards the convolutions; the difference between the diverging fibres and those of union; the generality of com- * Gren's Journal, 1795, i, p. 102. XVL11 PREFACE. missures; the true connexion of the convolutions with the rest of the cerebral mass, and the peculiar structure which admits of the convolutions being unfolded (an event that occurs in hydrocephalus of the cavities,) whilst the mass lying at their bot- toms, and belonging, for the most part, to the ap- paratus of union, or of the commissures, is pushed by the waters between the two layers composing them: lastly, I demonstrated the structure of the nervous mass of the spine; and I flatter myself with having arrived at the best method of dissect- ing the brain, and exposing its parts. What is my object then in publishing this vol- ume ? Our large work is too expensive for the generality of medical students; and, further, the method pursued in the discussions there, is only calculated for professed anatomists — whilst this book will be both less costly, and it will be adapt- ed to the student as well as to the more advanced anatomist. Moreover, many new ideas, possessing a great share of interest, may now be added; for, since Dr. Gall and I first published, the study of the nervous system has engaged the especial atten- tion of anatomists and physiologists. I have, my- self, continued inquiring, and conceive that I have made several new discoveries. I have, however copied some passages from the first volume of the large work already mentioned, and also given re- PREFACE. XIX duced drawings of several of its plates; because I think I have acquired a right to this volume, by its publication in our joint names, by my discoveries, that form its principal object, and by all I did in furtherance of its publication. All the drawings were executed under my superintendence from an- atomical preparations, made and determined on by me: the engraver worked by my directions: no plate was sent to press without my approval: the descriptions of the plates, and anatomical details are mine; and I furnished the literary notices in regard to the nerves of the abdomen and thorax, to those of the spinal column, of the five senses, of the cerebellum, and of the brain. Whoever desires more copious historical details than this volume will be found to contain, I refer to our Memoir addressed to the French Institute, and to the first volume of our great work, com- menced in common, and continued by Dr. Gall singly, after the middle of the second volume. The influence our labors have had on the study of the nervous system is incontestable. To be convinced of this, it is enough to examine the state of knowledge in regard to the anatomy, physiology, and pathology of the brain and spinal marrow, when Dr. Gall and I developed our ideas on these matters, whether it was by teaching orally, by dissecting publicly, or by means of our writings. XX PREFACE. I confess there is great satisfaction in the con- sciousness of having contributed to the important reform that has been effected in regard to the ner- vous system. I am only sorry to observe, that many of our ideas are appropriated by the authors of recent publications, without any mention of the source whence they were derived, or of the individ- uals who first struck them out, or reduced them to a certainty by direct proofs. We are commonly enough mentioned, it is true, when such of our as- sertions as appear weak are the subjects of criti- cism ; but our names are kept in the back-ground, when points of importance become the matter of discussion. The public, for instance, by referring to the proper place, may judge whether Mr. J. Cloquet, in his 'Anatomy of Man,' has been suffi- ciently explicit in stating that he has copied every one of the plates of the human brain contained in our large work. M. Serres, whose Memoir was deemed worthy of its prize by the Academy of Sciences of Paris, in the first volume of his work, uses our names no fewer than fifteen times in con- nexion with a single idea, which he fancies he can refute; and generally along with every fact that looks unfavorable to our opinion, he names us; but he always forgets to cite us in relation to very many fundamental conceptions which we had an- nounced at the same time. They who have writ- PREFACE. XXI ten to the following effect: —' M. Serres has proved clearly the erroneousness of M. Gall's observations, and replaced them by others,' may undeceive themselves by attending to the remark I have just made. M. Serres's publication forces me likewise to re- quest the reader to distinguish between a multitude of words and facts on the one hand, and the corol- laries which result on the other. I agree with those who, in works of science, pay especial regard to truths demonstrable to others, to ideas available in practical life, and to clearness and simplicity of style. What purpose can the following passage serve, which occurs in the preliminary discourse of M. Serres, where, after having said that a monster may be a vegetation of its like, that it may have two heads, two tails, and six or eight extremities, but that it would remain strictly confined to the limits of its class, he exclaims: —£ This wonderful phe- nomenon is undoubtedly connected with the general harmony of creation. What may be its cause ? We know it not, and in all likelihood ice shall remain ig- norant of it forever. It is one of the mysteries of cre- ation, whose surface is meted by man, but whose depths are sounded and known to God alone.' * * < Cet etonnant phenomene est sans doute lie a l'harmonie generate de la creation, auelle pent en etre la cause ? Nous l'ignorons et D xxii PREFACE. This phenomenon does not appear to me more extraordinary than that a kitten is not a puppy, or that a crab-tree does not produce pears. If the egg of a bird in its ordinary state cannot produce a mammiferous animal, why should the germ of this same egg, if it chance to be imperfectly developed, produce a deformity like to one of the mammalia'? Were the case thus, there would be some cause for amazement; but the universal fact of every animal producing its kind, is not, in my eyes, more aston- ishing than any other natural event. Further; the mass of facts cited, the number of dissections made, ought never to impose on us, nor be made a means for concealing the truth. Many of the anatomists who have lived before us, dissected some hundreds of brains, and they made a boast of their doings in this way; but they did not perceive that which I pledge myself to have discovered before I had dissected a dozen; for in- stance, the successive additions to the cerebral parts, and the two kinds of fibres, to wit, the di- verging, and the fibres of union. Anatomists and physiologists had certainly looked upon heads with- out number; but, before Dr. Gall's appearance, had failed to discover the seat of a single cerebral organ. vraisemblablement nous l'ignorerous toujours. C'estun desmyste- rA8 de la creation, dont I'homme mesure la surface, mais dont Dieu seul sonde et connait la profondeur.' PREFACE. xxiii A solitary individual, a beggar, enabled him to de- tect the organ of self-esteem, precisely as the fall of a single apple revealed the law of gravitation to Newton. Anatomists had seen many human brains without remarking any differences among them; these, however, are, to say the least, as constant as similarities. The point that essentially interests science is, the discovery of the truth, and this is then confirmed and established by all ulterior ob- servations. The anatomy of the peculiar system necessary to the affective and intellectual manifestations, as well as anatomy in general, admits of consideration in several ways. First, it is simply descriptive, that is, physical ap- pearances alone are examined, such as the form, the size, and color of parts, the tissues which com- pose them and their connexions. The nomenclature of the encephalon, of itself, suffices to show that such views had principally guided anatomists in their study of its structure. We still speak of the brain, of the cerebellum, of hemispheres, lobes, convolutions, and anfractuosi- ties; of a fornix, an infundibulum or funnel, of pisi- form and striated, quadrigeminal and pyramidal, olivary and harrow-shaped bodies; of a pineal gland, of a hippocampus's foot, of a writing-pen, and many other parts, — some having very offensive names. xxiv PREFACE. Such views are easily conceived to be of little use in medicine. This is the reason why the generality of practitioners are satisfied with knowing the membranous envelopes of the encephalon, the large blood-vessels, the sinuses, the great masses of the brain and cerebellum, and the principal cavities. Of these views, however, I shall only take such no- tice as may be necessary to recognise the parts spoken of in my physiological and pathological considerations. M. Serres, in the first page of his work on the Anatomy of the Brain, says, ' Up to the present time, (1824,) no one has dreamt of uniting into a body of doctrine all the knowledge acquired on the anatomy, the physiology, and the pathology of the brain and nervous system. I shall attempt the survey of this vast subject.' * Dr. Gall's and my own works disprove this assertion, and they have only to be consulted, to prove that all our inqui- ries were directed into this very channel. We have constantly insisted on the importance of studying the nervous system under all relations at once. From the year 1817 to 1823, I regularly delivered ' a Course of Lectures on the Anatomy, *'Jusqu'ace jour (1824) personne n'a songe a reunir en corps de doctrine les connaissances acquises sur l'anatomie, physiologie et la pathologie du systeme nerveux. Je vais essayer de parcourir ce vaste sujet.' PREFACE. XXV Physiology, and Pathology of the Brain and Ex- ternal Senses,7 twice a year. My course was al- ways so announced, according to the custom in Paris, by public placards; and my auditors must recollect that in my introductory discourse, I uni- formly insisted on the importance and necessity of studying these branches in connexion. From the above, it will be evident that M. Serres was mista- ken wiien he published himself' the first to attempt the survey of (essayer de parcourir) this vast sub- ject. Nevertheless, I most willingly allow that the principal consideration is not the having been the first to examine the nervous system: the true merit of the inquirer consists in that which he has ef- fected, that which he had discovered, and justice in these particulars will, in time, be assuredly ren- dered to all. If, at any time, I seem more especial- ly solicitous in showing the erroneousness of M. Serres's opinions, it is only because these have re- ceived the sanction of the French Institute, whose influence is great over the public mind. 2. Anatomy is physiological, when the structure of parts is studied in relation to their functions. This kind of anatomical knowledge is essential to practical medicine; for, without it, the seat of de- ranged functions cannot be understood. For this reason, therefore, my anatomical details will al- ways be given in harmony with the physiological XXV1 PREFACE. ideas I entertain of the apparatus destined to the manifestation of the affective and intellectual fac- ulties. 3. Anatomy is peculiarly human, or, it compre- hends the other beings of creation. In the latter event, it is entitled comparative anatomy, and this is a field that possesses much interest for the anat- omist, physiologist, and practical physician; I shall, therefore, enter upon it at frequent intervals, al- ways with the view of advancing the knowledge of the affective and intellectual nature of man. 4. Anatomy is entitled pathological, when it treats of the organic changes undergone by parts whether examined in connexion with, or independ- ently of, their deranged functions. Inquiries in this direction belong less to the present volume, than to that I have published on Insanity. To it, there- fore, I refer the reader for details. The object of this compendium, then, is to pre- sent the principal views that may be taken of the physiological and comparative anatomy of the ap- paratus destined to the affective and intellectual manifestations. It will be found divided into nine sections: in the first, I make some general reflec- tions on the nervous system; in the second, I speak of the division of the nervous apparatuses; in the third, I treat of the nerves of voluntary motion, and of the external senses; in the fourth, I discuss PREFACE. XXV11 the best mode of examining the structure of the brain ; in the fifth, I describe the cerebellum par- ticularly ; in the sixth, I do the like in regard to the brain; in the seventh, I examine the commis- sures; in the eighth, the communication of the nervous parts with each other; and in the ninth, I go into some anatomical points connected with physiology. The work will prove that I have ad- hered rather to philosophical views and principles, than to mere description of the physical qualities belonging to individual masses, although this last be the most common, I might almost say the only, plan that is generally pursued. I have endeavored, in an especial manner, to class together the parts that constitute particular apparatuses, a practice, which, to our predecessors was entirely unknown, as is abundantly evident from their nomenclature of the brain and its parts. ANATOMY OF THE BRAIN. Section I. General Considerations. Nerves are whitish cords that pervade the bod- ies of the most perfect animals ; they are always made up of many filaments, each of which, howev- er minute, is a tube that holds in its interior a pe- culiar pulpy substance, constituting one among the elements of organization. It is to a multitude of such tubular filaments, enveloped in a common sheath, that the term nervous cord, or nerve, is usually applied. These cords, as well as their component filaments; vary much in thickness and consistency. The last quality depends entirely on the texture of the enveloping membrane. The nervous system comprises two distinct sub- stances ; the one gelatinous or pulpy, and usually of a grayish or brownish hue; the other fibrous, and of a more or less perfect white color. They are commonly spoken of in books as the cineritious and medullary substances. 2 10 Of the Existence of the Pulpy and Fibrous Substances. Comparative anatomists are not agreed upon the constantly-conjoined existence of the two nervous substances. Some admit the presence of the white without that of the gray, especially in the spinal mass of many inferior animals, as reptiles and fish- es. The nervous ganglions of the asterice are also said to contain no gray matter. The above observation is evidently made in con- sequence of more attention being given to the color, than to the essential nature of the pulpy substance. Pulpiness, not color, is its distinguishing character. There are animals whose humors, or, as was for- merly said, whose blood is white. The essential consideration is evidently, in this case, the exist- ence, not the color, of the nutritious fluid. Should it not also be so in regard to the first or pulpy ner- vous substance, which in the majority of animals is of an ash-gray or brown color? Undoubtedly it should; for it is well known to vary in complexion, not only in different species of animals, but even in individuals of the same kind, according to their state of bodily health. The pulpy substance is commonly extremely pale in the brains of those who die of dropsy or pulmonary consumption.* * In the brain of a patient who died from anaemia supervening upon scurvy, the editor found the pulpy substance of the same shade of white as the fibrous possesses in the brain of those dying from acute disease; and this latter substance of a clearer white than is usually observed. It may be remarked in connexion with this sub- 11 The pulpy substance is found in the ganglions, and in the nervous masses of the head and spine of vertebral animals. In the mammiferous classes especially, it occurs on the surface of all the con- volutions of the brain and cerebellum, (a circum- stance from which it derives its title, cortical,) in the masses called striated bodies, and optic thala- mi, in the interior of the crura of the brain, or the annular protuberance, of the dentated body, of the cerebellum of the medulla oblongata, of the spinal cord through its entire length, and of all the gang- lions of the body. It never of itself composes an isolated unit or whole; it is always in connexion with the white or fibrous substance. Occasionally it lies in masses of varying magnitude ; and again it occurs in layers, or it runs along in slips, follow- ing the nerves in their course. Of the Structure of the two Nervous Substances. The first nervous substance is pulpy or gelati- nous, and of a color varying from deep brown to a pale ash-gray or white. Its intimate structure is unknown. Ruysh, Vieussens, and almost all the contemporaries of Haller, regarded it as a tissue of very fine blood-vessels. Ackermann of Heidelberg, and Walter of Berlin, in our own times, have held it to consist of an extremely attenuated prolonga- tion of vessels, in the course of becoming still more ject, that this patient, during his latter days, was of a very imbecile frame of mind, it being almost impossible to gather from him a correct history of his disease, or any profitable detail of its symp- toms. 12 minute, and ultimately composing the white or fibrous substance. This is a very ancient idea: it may be traced as far back as the age of Prax- agoras, who fancied that the nerves originated where the arteries ended. Albinus, and, at a later period, Scemmerring, have proved, by their injec- tions, that besides very minute blood-vessels, there also exists a peculiar substance in the cineritious nervous mass. The first or pulpy substance, there- fore, can only be said to have an immense quantity of blood-vessels distributed through it. Vicq d'Azyr believed that he could trace fibres in the pulpy substance; but what he saw was the white or truly fibrous substance, intermingled or uniting with the gray. The second, or white, nervous substance, is es- sentially fibrous, but it varies much in its degrees of consistency. Anatomists have differed extreme- ly in their notions of its intimate structure. Some have maintained it to be solid, others have said that it was tubular. Some have found it, like the pulpy substance, composed of globules; and whilst some have held that it possessed no blood-vessels, others have argued for its entire composition of these. Lewenhceck, Vieussens, and Steno, believed that the white nervous substance was fibrous. This is the opinion which Dr. Gall and I have espoused. By scraping it in the brain, according to the direc- tion of its fibres, these may be seen with the naked eye; and if the scraping motion be oblique or transverse to their course, they will be seen to be drawn from their natural direction, or to be torn. 13 If the brain be boiled in oil, or macerated in diluted nitric or muriatic acid, or in vinegar, or alcohol, or if it be frozen, the fibrous structure of its white substance will be rendered extremely apparent. Some, however, say, that the fibrousness is then the consequence of a chemical change. As the same result, however, is constantly obtained, and as the fibres, whichever of the processes be employ- ed, are regularly disposed in corresponding situa- tions in a similar manner, the fibrous structure must of necessity be recognised as natural and inherent. A few authors have attributed the fibrous appear- ance of the white substance to the impressions of blood-vessels. This mistake may be detected at once, by comparing the course of the blood-vessels with that of the cerebral fibres. The reality of the fibrousness of the white sub- stance is further opposed, by saying that when the brain is cut, it does not appear, and that it is pro- duced by the force employed to tear the tough cerebral masses asunder. To this I reply, that it is impossible, by means of a clean and smooth cut, to discover the structure of any extremely delicate and soft part whatsoever. Such a method is not even available in those cere- bral parts that are incontestably fibrous as the pyramidal bodies, the annual protuberance, the peduncles of the brain, &c. M. Bogros, of Paris, read a paper to the Acade- my of Sciences, on the 25th May, 1825, in which he maintained that every nervous fibre is perfora- ted by a canal, from its origin to its termination. 14 The accuracy of this statement is far^from being ascertained. The white nervous substance is generally called medullary. This name, however, ought to be dis- continued for two reasons : in the first place, the idea we form of marrow excludes the conception of fibrousness; and again, the functions of the ner- vous fibres are so superior to those of the marrow, that it is a pity to designate both by the same word. The name is an evidence of the error that was anciently committed, when every thing con- tained in an osseous cavity was considered as marrow. Of the Use of the Pulpy JVervous Substance. Different opinions have prevailed ever since the times of Vesalius and Piccoluomini, who directed the attention of anatomists in a particular manner to this subject, respecting the use and destination of the pulpy or gray substance. It has frequently been regarded as an organ of secretion, either of vital spirits or of a nervous fluid. Unity of senti- ment, it is probable, will not readily be obtained upon this particular point: but seeing that the ru- diments of each new shoot in trees are developed in a deposition of mucilaginous-looking matter; that the cartilages in animal bodies are successively li- quid, gelatinous, and cartilaginous, and that seve- ral turn into firm bone; that anatomists, in many instances, have agreed in deriving the nerves from ganglions; that the brain, too, is at first fluid, then 15 gelatinous, and ultimately fibrous; in fine, that the pulpy nervous substance is always found where the white fibres become more numerous; that these are implanted, so to say, into it, and that a great quan- tity of blood-vessels are expanded on it; Dr. Gall and I have said, that it appears to us to be the source or nourisher of the white fibres. Let us, however, distinguish in this, as in every other place, between facts and inferences. Supposing that our ideas concerning the inference were really inexact, the peculiarities I have mentioned, and the essen- tial importance of the pulpy substance to the ner- vous functions must ever be admitted. Having said that a gray color is not unessential in the character of the pulpy substance, no objec- tion to our notions of its uses can be derived from the fact of its supposed absence in the ganglions of the asterice. A gelatinous or pulpy matter does certainly enter into their constitution, and this is sufficient. The white substance is also said to be present in the brain and spinal cord before the pulpy or gray appears. If by this be understood the exist- ence of the nervous masses that become white prior to an evident separation into two substances, and to the development of the convolutions, I agree; but if the rudiments of the brain, cerebellum, and spinal cord, be said to be white, and not gray, in the first instance, I positively deny the assertion. The nervous masses of the head and spine are pulpy or gelatinous, and decidedly grayish in color before they are white. Neither Dr. Gall nor I 16 have ever thought of saying, that the portion of the completely-developed nervous system, which is pulpy and gray, gives birth to that which is white and fibrous. We did but intend to announce the fact of a gelatinous and grayish state of the brain preceding its fibrous and white condition, precisely as we should say of its entire mass, that it is liquid before it becomes gelatinous. Our idea of the formation of the nervous system, seems more especially plausible, as it appears to be perfectly analogous to what takes place in the osseous system. Bone begins by being gelatinous; it is then cartilaginous, and ultimately solid and earthy. I repeat, however, that no one can be more impressed than myself with the difficulty of drawing general conclusions : I am, therefore, very far from being anxious to impose such as I do infer, upon others. I only insist on the necessity of ex- actness in regard to the facts related. Putting our theory of the formation out of the question, entire- ly, Dr. Gall and I still assert our title to be consid- ered as the first who discovered and made known the general relation that prevails in man and the mammalia, between the pulpy and fibrous sub- stances of the brain and its several parts. Messrs. Foville and Pinel Grandchamp have of late inferred, from pathological observations, that the superficial cineritious substance of the brain presides over the intellectual functions, and its white and deep-seated gray mass over locomotion. I am disposed to set much store by pathological observations; yet I do not see that we dare place 17 unlimited confidence in them alone. Truth is, I conceive, universally harmonious; it consequently cannot have been attained in any case until anat- omy and physiology and pathology accord exact- ly. Now, who will maintain that the locomotive powers of animals are great in proportion as their brains contain more of the white nervous sub- stance, and as the striated bodies and the supposed origiris of the optic nerves (thalami) are large? Or, who will say that the locomotive capacities of inferior tribes^ in whose brains the gray substance predominates, are less remarkable than their intel- lectual endowments? These positions are alike untenable. Dr. Gall and I suppose that each ner- vous apparatus is composed of two peculiar sub- stances, the pulpy and the fibrous, and that both are necessary to produce an instrument adequate to the performance of a particular function. Of the Origin of the JYervous System. The brain has very generally been regarded as the sole and common origin of every part of the nervous system. Even the old anatomists, who classed the brain with the viscera of the chest and belly, and treated of its structure in their chapter of Splanchnology, mistook it, however strange the error may seem, for the source of all the nerves. In their eyes, the spinal cord was a prolongation of the cerebral mass, and the great sympathetic, and the nerves of the thorax and abdomen, were 18 continuations of the encephalon and spinal cord. This erroneous view, as it was espoused, was es- pecially defended, by observing that the commands of the will issued from, and that all. consciousness resided in, the brain. These facts, however, in truth, prove no more than the communication of the nervous masses of the body with those of the head. The muscular fibres, we see, are excited by the nerves, but they are not, therefore, continuations of the nervous filaments. The notion, however, was so palpably erroneous, that anatomists were not long in calling its sound- ness in question, more especially in regard to the nerves of vegetative life. Winslow,* for instance, separated the great sympathetic from the spinal cord and brain; he even regarded the bundles, that run between its ganglions or masses, as simple branches of communication; and went so far as to say, that all ganglions ought to be considered as peculiar origins of nerves, and, consequently as so many little brains. Scemmerring likewise observes, that the sympa- thetic, having an independent existence, may rather be said to go to, than to come from, the spinal cord. He adds, that it never forms a trunk in any wise proportionate to the number and size of its commu- nicating branches, and that it never loses itself among muscles, but follows the course of the blood- vessels.! Bichat has expressed his opinion in the most pos- * Anatomy. f Him und Nevern. 19 itive manner upon the subject. c The ideas of anat- omists,' says he,* ' upon this important nerve, seem to me very little accordant with what nature pro- claims to be just. All agree in representing it as a medullary cord, extending from the head to the os sacrum, sending various branches, in its course, to the neck, the chest, and the abdomen; having in short, a distribution analogous to the spinal nerve, from which, or from those of the neck, it is said by some to derive its origin. Whatever the name chosen to designate it may chance to be, sympathetic, intercostal, or trisplanchnic, the mode of considering it will still be found to remain un- changed. ' This mode I regard as altogether erroneous. In fact, there exists no such nerve as these names are used to signify. That which is taken for a nerve is, in truth, but a suit of communications between different nervous centres, situated at various dis- tances from each other. ' These centres are the ganglions, scattered through the different regions of the body. They have all an independent and isolated action. Each is a particular focus, sending a multitude of ramifi- cations, to carry into the respective organs the irra- diations of the centre whence they proceed. ' What anatomist,' he continues, ' has not been struck by differences among the nerves ? Those of the brain are larger, whiter, more dense, less numerous, and offering few varieties; whilst ex- * Sur la Vie et la Mort. 20 treme tenuity, great number, especially around the plexuses, grayish color, peculiar softness of tissue, frequently occurring varieties, are, on the other hand, the distinguishing characters of the nerves that issue from the ganglions, The only exception in either case are in the branches of communication between the cerebral nerves, and in a few of the twigs that unite the little nervous centres.' Bi- chat thought it essential to express these opinions in the descriptive anatomy. The view commonly given is not calculated to convey an exact notion, either of the nervous centres, or of the nerves that emanate from them. Comparative anatomy, and acephalic monstrosi- ties among the mammalia and man, furnish incon- trovertible proofs of the brain not being the origin of the nervous system at large, To compare the nervous systems of different tribes, is a task attend- ed with much difficulty ; but very many of the in- ferior animals have nerves,, although they have nothing that may be likened to a brain, Their nervous system, consequently, cannot have the ori: gin commonly assigned to it by authors. There are also many anatomical descriptions, to be found in books of acephalic monsters, among the more perfect animals, as well as the human kind, whose nervous system, notwithstanding the absence of brain, was quite perfect That nerves may exist without a brain, is therefore established as a truth beyond the sphere of doubt. Some writers, however, relying upon the author- ity of Morgagni, Haller, and Sandifort, have main- 21 tained that the brains of acephali exist in the first instance, but that dropsy of the cavities. destroys , the parts, which, at birth, are found wanting, along with the membranes and bony covering. But no one ever saw, an acephalic child whose brain and skull exhibited traces of such destruc- tion. The integuments of the upper part of the head, where the destructive process is principally supposed, are commonly entire and healthy. Nei- ther has any one, in these cases, discovered the cicatrices of ulcers, traces of erosion, or simply of absorption. The bones that compose the basis of the skull, when they exist in acephali, are smooth and round at the edges. The whole inferior part of the encephalio mass, as well as the optic, audi- tory, olfactory, and other nerves, occasionally occur in a perfectly sound and natural state. Howx did these nerves and tender cerebral parts resist the action of a fluid that dissolved or caused the ab- sorption of membranes and firm bone 1 It must be allowed, then, that the brain may be primitively wanting, just as may the legs or arms, and that the nervous apparatus of the body does not proceed from the brain. The first anatomical principle in regard to the nervous system therefore, is, that it is not an unit, but consists of many essentially different parts, which have their own individual origins, and are mutual in communication. This principle Dr. Gall and I regard as essential to our physiological researches and deductions. No anatomist before us was ever so much interest- 22 ed in demonstrating its truth. We conceive that we have proved it satisfactorily in regard to the brain, as Bichat had done before us, to the nerves of the chest and belly. In addition to our first anatomical principle, there are three subjects that require to be taken into consideration separately; these are, — 1st. The mode in which the individual parts of the nervous system are formed; 2d. The order of development of these parts; and, 3d. Their reciprocal relations. Dr. Gall and I, in our publications and anatom- ical demonstrations, have always spoken of the mode and order in which the cerebral parts are formed and developed; however, we never touched upon these points but in a general way, our atten- tion being more especially given to the considera- tion of the plurality of the nervous apparatuses, of their communications, and of their mutual relations, the whole in harmony with our physiological and philosophical inquiries. Mr. Tiedemann, and, after him, Mr. Serres, have treated in a particular man- ner of the formation and successive development of the several parts composing the nervous system. Of the Mode of Formation of the Nervous System. I have no intention of entering into any discus- sion upon Encephalogeny. My researches have not been of extent sufficient to qualify me either to ad- mit or to reject the opinions promulgated by M. Serres upon this subject. According to this gen- tleman, the spinal cord, the cerebellum, and the 23 brain, are developed from the circumference to- wards the centre, and not, as anatomists had hith- erto admitted, from the centre towards the circum- ference. He tells us, that there are many centres of formation, and that each apparatus is composed of several pieces, which are joined together, the extremities commencing, the middles terminating, the union. According to him, too,* the arteries exert a particular directing influence over the de- velopment of the nervous system. The spinal cord, he conceives to be formed under the guidance of the intercostal, the cerebellum under that of the vertebral, and the brain under that of the carotid arteries. The vessels of the spinal cord, he says, appear first, and the outline of this part is soonest appa- rent. The common carotids appear next, and then the internal vessels of that name, which are dis- tributed to the crura or legs of the brain, and to the bodies called quadrigeminal, and these are now evolved. The vertebral arteries reach the cranium last, and the cerebellum is the latest formed of all the nervous masses. 1 The brain,' he states, ' is developed from before backwards, and the cerebellum from behind for- wards, according to the direction of the blood-ves- sels. From this it comes that the callous body is evolved from behind forwards, as the arteria callo- sa gradually appears.' I intend in another place to cite M. Serres's no- * Tom. i. p. 568, et seq. 24 tions upon the formation of some particular appa- ratuses. Here I only observe that he differs from M. Tiedemann upon a very principal point; whilst M. Serres proclaims a successive development from the circumference towards the centre, M. Tiede- mann publishes the reverse of this as the truth. I also confess that a great number of M. Serres's opinions appear to me little probable. Dr. Baron * lately showed the body of an hydrocephalic child to the Academy of Medicine in Paris, and proved that though the carotid arteries existed, the ante- rior cerebral lobes were wanting. M. Serres had made too hastily a contrary report upon this very case to the Philomathic Society the day before Dr. Baron made the dissection. In recognising the whole importance of enceph- alogeny, or the doctrine of cerebral formation, it still appears to me that its laws cannot be at vari- ance with those that may be demonstrated in the brain, when it has arrived at maturity of growth. I farther opine, that conclusions drawn from the structure of the adult brain, are to be preferred to such as are founded on it in its embryotic sjtate. I shall, in more places than one, have occasion to refer to this argument, and especially when I come to treat of the anterior commissure and^ callous body, to the end that I may rectify the erroneous opinions promulgated by M. Tiedemann. My great and sole object is to know the structure of the brain, such as it may be demonstrated in harmony with physiology and pathology. * Bulletin des Sciences Medicales, Juin, 1S25, p. 175. 25 Of the general Form of the Nervous System. The form of the nervous system, it may b? con- ceived, varies according to the radiated, globular, or elongated configuration of the animal body, of which it is a constituent. It cannot be alike in the star-fish, worm, and caterpillar. (PI. i. fig. 1.) Generally, then, and even as its particular parts are concerned, the nervous system exhibits the greatest varieties of form. Its masses, too, occur aggregated, as in the skull, and spinal canal, or in the form of nerves properly so called, or collected into knots differing in form, size, consistency and color, entitled ganglions. Nerves frequently emanate from distant and oppo- site sources to unite, anastomose, or twine together, and then to separate and run off in different direc- tions. This is the particular arrangement to which the term plexus is applied. Ganglions and plex- uses often form an intricate tissue together. Of the Structure and Use of Ganglions. Ganglions are bodies composed of the two ner- vous substances: the white or fibrous, and the gelatinous or pulpy, into which the first is plunged. The pulpy matter of the ganglions is commonly of a gray color, of different degrees of intensity; some- times, however, it has a yellowish, a reddish, or a whitish cast: it is easily distinguishable from the nervous filaments which it surrounds. The fibres 4 26 of the white substance, the second element of the ganglions, anastomose repeatedly; they also cross each other frequently; sometimes in every direc- tion, or in their course parallel with that of the nerve upon which the ganglion is formed. Gang- lions of this last kind are commonly oval shaped, but those in which several nerves meet and inoscu- late, have for the most part very irregular forms. Anatomists have always entertained very differ- ent opinions upon the uses of the ganglions. Some, with Willis, have ascribed to them the secretion of what they style vital spirits; others, with Vieus- sens, conceive them destined to separate and to strengthen the nervous fibres: many of the mod- erns again, Bichat, Reil, and others, think with Johnstone, that they serve to isolate from the influ- ence of the brain the parts which receive their nerves from them. The latter authors divide the nervous systems into two parts, a cerebral and a ganglionary. The theory of the vital spirits is now forgotten. The ganglions are also proved to do more than merely separate or direct the nervous filaments, as Meckel the elder, Zinn, Scarpa, and others, have supposed. ' A glance,' says Bichat,* ' enables us to discover the greatest differences among them, (the ganglions.) There is evidently as great a dis- tinction between the ganglions and the nerves that issue from them, as there is between the cerebral nerves and the brain itself. There is difference of * Anatomie G6n6rale, T. 1. 27 consistency and other outward qualities, and there is difference of properties. Were the nerves that come from the spinal cord merely unravelled in their passage through the ganglions, this would be but a difference of form, and not of nature, and their properties, therefore, would remain the same. Why has not nature placed ganglions on the nerves of the limbs, as on those of internal parts? If there be only a resolution of nerves into finer fila- ments in ganglions, why is there no constant pro- portion between the fibres that enter on one side, and those that issue from the other? Did the nerve that penetrates the superior cervical ganglion from above expand in its interior, and having re- united its filaments form the cord that issues from below, it ought evidently to be of precisely the same size at its exit as at its entrance. Such a re- lation, indeed, between the nerves of the opposite extremities of ganglions, ought to be quite general. But a very cursory examination shows that a con- trary disposition almost invariably obtains. The size of the ganglions should be relative to that of the nerves, whose expanded fibres are said to com- pose them. Why then are the intercostal gangli- ons so small, and the trunks which unite, or rather, to use the common phrase, originate, and then leave them, so large ? And why, on the contrary, is the superior cervical ganglion so voluminous, and its nervous branches so minute? How can the frequent interruption among the ganglions in the human kind, which in a host of animals are quite regular, be explained, if the nervous filaments 28 which enter be continuous with those wThich issue from them ? How comes it that the ganglions and their nerves bear no exact proportion to the cere- bral nerves, if the latter give them birth or expand in them ? Why, in fine, has not the pain, that is transmitted by both kinds of nerves, the same char- acter?' Scemmerring has made reflections very similar to these of Bichat. Notwithstanding all this, the opinion in regard to the use of the ganglions, which Johnstone, Bichat, and Reil entertained, and which others have adopted from them, is by no means exact. They do not interrupt the reciprocal influence of the brain and nerves of the spinal cord, nor of the brain and viscera of the chest and belly, either in the healthy or pathological state of the body. They most certainly do not prevent impressions made on parts supplied with nerves from them, or diseased sensations of the viscera from being felt. On the contrary, the ganglions would appear essen- tial to the structure of the nerves of sensation. They, however, abstract the parts they furnish with nervous energy, from the influence of the will. They also originate nervous fibres; and serve, farther, as points of communication between differ- ent nerves. Lastly, as the existence of a nervous fluid is not impossible, nay, as in all likelihood such a fluid does exist, the ganglions may probably aid in its secretion or evolution, and modify its circula- tion or distribution. 29 Section II. Division of the Masses composing the Nervous System. We have already seen, among the general con- siderations, that the nervous system is not a simple unit, but an aggregation of parts, that originate and may exist separately, but which are in intimate communication, as was required on account of the influence their functions mutually exert. The functions of the abdominal viscera, for instance, act upon those of the brain, and those of the brain in- fluence digestion, circulation, the secretion of bile, &c. The nervous system, therefore, cannot be compared to a net, as is sometimes done, if, by this, similarity of nature in its constituent parts be implied. On the contrary, as differences are evi- dent, divisions become indispensable. But the task of determining the instruments spe- cially dedicated to particular functions, is one of great difficulty. The ordinary division of the ner- vous masses is indisputably defective. A true one can only rest on the nature of the functions per- formed. These functions are naturally separated into two grand classes : vegetative, or organic func- tions, and phrenic, or functions taking place with consciousness. The nervous masses belonging to the first of these are, in common language, the 30 great sympathetic nerve, and the ganglions and plexuses of the thorax and abdomen. Bichat has divided them into a multitude of apparatuses, in- dividually necessary to the offices of the particular viscera. This division is, in my apprehension, founded in nature. I regard the nervous masses of vegetative life as independent of those of phrenic life, in as far as their existence is concerned. I also incline to admit as many different kinds of nerves as different vegetative functions; I farther recognise their communications among themselves, and with the masses of the phrenic functions. The nervous masses of vegetative life are very simple in the lower tribes of being; they are more numerous as we mount in the scale, and as the func- tions of vegetative life become complicated. Their general arrangement, as was to be expected, varies according to the form and disposition of the viscera in species and individuals. If visceral functions, locomotion, and manifestations of sensibility be united, it is also conceivable that to separate the peculiar nerves of these dissimilar operations from each other, would be next to impossible. This consideration shows why some anatomists have compared the nerves of the caterpillar and worm to the great sympathetic; whilst others have liken- ed them to the intervertebral ganglions, or to the spinal cord itself. In the vertebral classes of animals, and especially in the mammalia, the nervous masses are distinctly separated, first, into those of vegetative life, and, second, into those of phrenic functions. Each of 31 these may farther be subdivided into two parts: the first into nerves of viscera, or, as M. de Blain- ville* expresses himself, into a visceral portion, and into nerves of communication ; that is, an appara- tus which establishes sympathy among the visceral nerves, and between these and the nervous masses of the external senses, and of the affective and in- tellectual functions. The second, or nervous masses of phrenic life, again, are commonly divided into the brain, prop- erly so called, the cerebellum, medulla oblongata, and spinal cord, together with the nerves of these four parts. M. de Blainville ranges the nervous masses of phrenic life under two titles: the one he styles Central, the other Ganglionary. ' The first, or central,' says he, | ' is susceptible of three degrees of development; is always situated above the intestinal canal, begins with the oesophagus or pharynx, and is prolonged, more or less backward- ly, so as to correspond to a greater or smaller num- ber of rings of the body when they exist. ' The second, or ganglionary, consists of a very variable number of pairs of ganglions, disposed in a slightly different manner on each side of the first. 1 The central portion is always composed of two similar halves, situated, the one to the right, the other to the left, and more or less intimately united, or drawn together by means of the peculiar ap- * Bulletin des Sciences par la Societe Philomatique de Paris, 1821, Mars et Avril. t Op. cit. p. 44. 32 paratus, styled commissure. It is divided into two rigorously similar portions: a vertebral and a ce- phalic, included, as their titles imply, the one in the vertebral canal, the other in the cranium. Both contain gray and white substance, and longitudinal and transverse commissures. ' The cephalic portion consists, 1st, of an inferior bundle called pyramidal, which passes under the annular protuberance, continues long distinct from the cerebral peduncles, and runs to terminate in the anterior, or olfactory lobe, of the brain. 2d. Of a superior bundle, which continues, in the first place, under the name of prolongation of the quad- rigeminal bodies, to the cerebellum, and, running on the outer sides of the internal geniculated bod- ies, is lost on the hemispheres; and, in the second place, of a deep bundle, which may be followed to the mammillary bodies, and from thence into the optic thalami. The bundles named peduncles of the pineal gland, which are expended on the optic thalamus, are also to be regarded as belonging to this cephalic portion.' The same author subdivides his second or gang- lionary portion into ganglions without, and gang- lions with, external apparatus. Among the for- mer he reckons the olfactory masses, the hemis- pheres properly so called, the quadrigeminal bodies, and the cerebellum. The olfactory nerve, accord- ing to him, is a cerebral mass, in which his inferior central bundle terminates. The hemispheres, he conceives, form another ganglion, having no exter- nal apparatus, but one transverse, (the callous 33 body,) and two longitudinal commissures (the pe- duncles of the brain and the fornix.) It is to this ganglion that the convolutions belong. He farther regards the striated bodies as appertaining to the convolutions, but remarks that, for certain, their fibres do not originate there to go to the hemis- pheres. The quadrigeminal bodies are the third, and the cerebellum the fourth ganglion, without external apparatus. The title, visceral part of the nervous system, ap- pears to me well chosen, but I do not think that the division into central and ganglionary parts, or the other subdivisions proposed, deserve the same ap- proval. In the first place, I cannot see that they are based on anatomical observation: I do not believe, for instance, that M. de Blainville can demonstrate the continuation of the inferior bundle, or pyra- midal body into the olfactory nerve, nor that he can trace what he calls the deep bundle (faisceau profond) of his central portion into the mammillary bodies. Neither do I think that his divisions and subdivisions of the nervous mass pertaining to phrenic life, are consistent with physiological facts; but this in anatomical classification is quite essen- tial ; it is even acknowledged by M. de Blainville himself as its basis. 'We ought,' says he,* ' to consider the nervous system as subdivided into as many parts as there are grand functions performed by the animal body.' * Op. cit. p. 40. 5 34 According to the ideas of M. Desmoulins, the parts which constitute the complete cerebro-spinal system of mammalia, are — 1st, the spinal cord; 2d, the cerebellum, which is itself composed of three parts; 3d, the optic lobes, or quadrigeminal bodies; 4th, the lobes of the brain ; 5th, the olfac- tory lobes. These five parts are admitted by M. Desmoulins not to be always complete in the other classes of vertebral animals, and also to be want- ing, individually, without detriment to the rest. The sturgeon, lamprey, frog, and some other crea- tures, have, he says, no cerebellum; the skate and shark no cerebral lobes; many bony fishes no olfac- tary lobes; in short, he concludes, that generally speaking, the notion concerning the unity of the nervous system is a chimera. In opposition to M. Desmoulins's views, Dr. Bailly has maintained that every vertebra, or every seg- ment, through the whole length of the animal body, contains the same nervous elements; that, belong- ing to each of the vertebrae of the head and spine, there are nerves of sensation, of motion, of diges- tion, and of an intellectual system, charged with the task of appreciating the impressions communi- cated by the others, and of producing determi- nations. In my apprehension, the cerebro-spinal system is not a simple unit, but a compound of many distinct apparatusses, each of which has particular func- tions, which, being taken together, constitute phrenic life. The several instruments are formed after a general plan, but the physical qualities, as 35 the density, form, size, and color, of all, individually, differ not only in the classes and in the species, but also in the members composing each kind; the number of particular organs is likewise greater or smaller in the classes and species, according to the amount of the primary functions or faculties; lastly, each of the apparatuses is simple, or is compound. No one, however, can be said to be perfectly simple; each is, at least, double, or one of a pair. The organs of motion and of touch are evidently multiplied. It is essential in dividing and subdividing the parts of which the cerebro-spinal system is com- posed, to keep in harmony with physiology and pathology, and to distinguish between wThat is common to all, and what is peculiar to each of them. The first grand division must embrace the instruments of motion, and of the five external senses; the second comprise the organs of the af- fective and intellectual functions. I think they are wrong who confound the spinal cord with the cerebral masses, and designate both by a common title, such as encephalon or brain. It is long since Dr. Gall and I pointed out this error, but it is still very generally sanctioned, by the French Academy of Sciences, for instance, so late as 1820, its prize having been accorded to the me- moir of M. Serres under the following title. —' Don- ner une description comparee du cerveau dans les quatre classes des animaux vertebres, et particu- lierement dans ceux des reptiles et des poissons, cherchant a. determiner l'analogie des differentes 36 parties de cet organe, ou marquant soigneusement les changes des formes et des proportions eprouvees, et suivant aussi profondement que possible les ra- tines des nerfs cerebreaux.' Here evidently the brain and its nerves only are spoken of. M. Serres, however, deemed it proper to consider the nervous mass of the spine as well as that of the cranium, and he has designated these two systems by the same name without any objection being made by the Academy to this arrangement. Let us turn to the facts that prove the brain and the spinal cord to be perfectly distinct and inde- pendent of each other. These facts are anatomical, physiological, and pathological. I here assume as an established point, that the functions of the spinal cord differ entirely from those of the brain. The development and demonstration of this truth belong to physiology. I have, accordingly, examined it particularly in my work on Phrenology. Physio- logical experiments and pathological facts tend equally to show that the brain and spinal cord are masses possessed of distinct functions that cannot be confounded. This volume being destined solely to the discussion of anatomical views, I shall, at present, confine myself to such evidence as anatomy affords of the mutual independence of each of the nervous masses mentioned. And, first — all that has been said, in a general way, upon the origin of the nerves, and their exist- ence, independently of the brain, applies particu- larly in the case of the spinal cord. The spinal cord sometimes exists in part, sometimes entirely, 37 when the brain is altogether wanting. If, to this, it be objected that the brain had been removed by absorption, the answer I have given to the same proposition, in reference to the nerves in general, must be repeated here. There is, also, one, and but one, case on record, in which the brain existed, without the spinal cord.* It occurred in a child whose head or heads are preserved in the Hunteri- an Museum, in London; this child was born with one skull placed vertically upon another. Each of these contained a brain invested with its usual membranes ; the dura mater of each brain adhered closely to that of the other, and both were supplied by blood-vessels issuing from common trunks. Parts, consequently, as the brain and spinal cord that exist, or not, independently of each other, cannot constitute one and the same apparatus. 2. The nervous masses of the spinal canal and cranium bear no regular proportion to each other. Man, with his voluminous brain, has a smaller spi- nal cord than the ox or horse, whose brain is so much less. Bartholin in former times, and Scem- merring in our own days, made this remark; nev- ertheless, they both continued to speak of the spinal cord as a process or continuation of the brain. 3. The spinal cord is well known not to decrease in size as it descends in the vertebral canal, and as it sends off nerves. Its volume is even augmented, where its nerves are largest and most numerous; this is obvious, especially towards its sacral end, * See Philosoph. Trans, for 1790, March 25th, and for 1798, Dec. 13th. 38 as may be seen by turning to PI. i. fig. 2, where this part of the spinal cord of a fowl is represented; and yet the spinal cord is commonly considered as a prolongation of the white substance of the brain and cerebellum! 4. The direction of its nerves, especially in the mammalia, proves to a certainty, that the spinal cord is not a continuation of the nervous mass of the cranium. Every pair of spinal nerves is made up by several bundles: (PL i. fig. 3; a part of a spinal cord of a calf;) some of these issue from be- low, and run upward; others come from above, and proceed downward and outward. Now it would be absurd to suppose that these bundles were con- tinued, or derived either from the lower or upper extremity of the spinal cord. They undoubtedly originate at the place whence they issue indi- vidually. In regard to the fifth, sixth, seventh, and other pairs of nerves, styled cerebral (PI. vi. fig. 1;) their direction also shows that they do not come either from the brain or cerebellum. Santorini, when speaking of the course of the fifth pair, remarks, that after descending from the brain, it turns back and runs upwards; and he adds — < if it do not probably come from below, entirely like the acces- sory nerve.' The proofs, confirmatory of the mutual independ- ence of the spinal cord and hemispheres of the brain and cerebellum, serve also to demonstrate the propriety of separating the nerves, styled cere- bral, from the brain itself. The evidence in this, 39 as in the other case, is anatomical, physiological, and pathological. For the reasons already given, I here rest on anatomical testimony alone; for in- formation on the others, I refer to my physiological and pathological treatises* To proceed, then, we see monsters, occasionally, born without any of the proper cerebral masses, but with olfactory, optic, and acoustic nerves, either severally or altogether perfect; and on the contra- ry, these nerves individually, or generally, have been found in a state of atrophy, whilst the brain was sound and well developed in all its parts. There is no proportion, whatever, between the cranial nerves and the true cerebral mass. Many animals have them much larger in proportion to their brain than man. I repeat, therefore, that I divide and subdivide the nervous masses according to their offices: first, into nerves of vegetative functions ; secondly, of ex- ternal sense, and locomotion; lastly, of affective and intellectual operations: these last inhere in parts, which I style truly cerebral, superadded to the nerves of the senses. We shall, afterwards, see that the masses belong- ing to the last named class of functions require sub- dividing into instruments of particular functions, after the manner of the external senses. This principle of the plurality of the nervous ap- paratuses is the basis of these anatomical consid- erations, and is indispensable to the physiological * Phrenology ; or, the Doctrine of the Mind; Lond. 1825. Ob- servations on Insanity; Lond. 1816. 40 researches in which Dr. Gall and I have so long been engaged. It has been, and is still, contested, and at the same time it is brought forward as new. A reference, however, to the date and matter of our publications, will assign it to those who can claim it by right. The general idea of a plurality of organs, indeed, must be allowed to be very an- cient, and not the discovery of any modern author; before Dr. Gall appeared, however, none of the cerebral functions had been specified, and before our combined investigations were made public, the structure of no special apparatus had been demon- strated ; for the parts of the encephalon that bear distinguishing titles, do not, by any means, consti- tute particular organs. The special determination, as well anatomical as physiological, consequently, belongs to Dr. Gall and myself. 41 Section III. Of the Nervous Masses of Voluntary Motion and of the External Senses. These masses include the spinal cord, with its nerves, and the nerves of the head, distinguished by the title, cerebral nerves. They may, therefore, be divided and spoken of as spinal or vertebral, and as cranial. The former, principally contained in the canal of the vertebral column, are more consid- erable in size, and are entirely destined to the functions of feeling and voluntary motion; the lat- ter are less voluminous, but besides nerves of vol- untary motion and of feeling, they include nerves of taste, hearing, sight, and smell. Anatomists have, at all times, separated the nerves of four of the senses, to wit — smell, taste, sight, and hearing, from those appropriated to feel- ing; they have also regarded the third, fourth, and sixth pairs of nerves as especially destined to mo- tion ; but they have confounded all the other trunks of the medulla oblongata and spinal cord, and sup- posed the whole to pertain to feeling. It is long since I maintained the necessity of subdividing these nerves, and of admitting separate fibres for the functions of touch and of motion. My opinion was founded on facts, and anatomical, physiological, and pathological reasonings. It was long ago remarked that feeling and voluntary mo- tion were not alwavs impaired or annihilated 6 42 simultaneously ; sometimes the one, sometimes the other of these functions was seen to be totally lost, whilst the other remained unimpaired. The con- clusion then followed, and it had already been seized by Erasistratus of Alexandria, and that there were nerves of motion, and nerves of feeling. Pathological facts, therefore, first fixed the atten- tion of physicians on this point, as they also gave the first idea of a decussation of nervous fibres in the brain, and of the peculiar structure of the con- volutions. To the above some have replied that the admis- sion of two sorts of nerves to explain the isolated lesion, whether of voluntary motion or of feeling, was unnecessary: the loss of motion, they say, is consequent on a minor degree of cerebral affection, the destruction of feeling on disease of a graver and more extensive description. This objection is purely speculative, and totally unsupported by experience. To make it even plausible, it should surely be capable of standing the test of physiological proofs in the healthy state; that is to say, sensibility and volition should be shown to be mere degrees of the same power, volition being the lower of the two : but observa- tion is very far from confirming such an opinion. Physiology, and analogy, in reference to the oth- er senses, appear to determine feeling as a function entirely distinct from voluntary motion, and to prove each of these functions as manifested by the medium of a special organ or nerve. The func- tions of touch are active and passive, like those of the other external senses. The muscles being 43 called into action, would the internal faculties em- ploy any of the senses to take cognizance of im- pressions from without ? They aid touch in feel- ing, as they assist sight in looking, hearing in listen- ing, and the olfactory nerves in smelling. The nerves of voluntary motion, i. e. of its organs, the muscles, cannot propagate impressions of touch, nor the nerves of the skin those of motion. The im- pressions propagated by the nerves of motion come from within, as those transmitted by the nerves of feeling are derived from without. The muscles, alone, produce sensations of fatigue or weariness, and these have no relation to the nerves of feeling : we may be excessively fatigued, and, at the same time, have the sense of touch extremely;vigorous. Physiological and pathological facts being strik- ingly and universally favorable to the idea of dis- tinct nerves of feeling and motion, and four of the senses being evidently provided with peculiar ner- vous apparatuses, I have always maintained the probability of the same law obtaining, in regard to the fifth, and even invited anatomists to use their endeavors to verify the point, to demonstrate the muscular as distinct from the cuticular nerves. To the above notion, it has been objected, that the nerves of feeling and of motion issue together from the spinal cord, and as a consequence of this, that the nature of every fibre, of the same pair, must be essentially the same. To this I always answered that the objection is neither valid on the strength of the fact, nor on that of the inference from it. For the spinal pairs are composed of two roots, the one dorsal, the other abdominal; and 44 further, each root is, itself, composed of many bun- dles. I also stated that this structure was less con- spicuous in the fifth cranial nerve than in the spinal pairs; but that, nevertheless, the difference of its bundles, and their dissimilar functions, were recog- nized. One, all allow, is appropriated to taste,whilst the rest belong to sensation in general. In conform- ity with the above view, the glossopharyngeal has always appeared to i.me destined to the third sort of the lingual functions, that is, to general feeling.* The above ideas were published in my English work, 'The Physiognomical System,'! in 1815; and in 1818, in my French book, entitled ' Phre- nologie.' J In all the courses of lectures I have delivered since these works issued from the press, I have broached similar ideas, and encouraged those of my auditors, whom opportunity favored, to enter on the inquiry, and to endeavor to trace the nervous fibres from their peripheral expansions to their origin in the spinal cord. The subject has, indeed, been advanced by the labors of Messrs. C. Bell and Magendie. Mr. Bell has recognised a difference between the nerves of sensation in general and those of respiration.^ He admits, in the first instance, that every apparatus performing a single function, is provided with but one nerve; that parts which receive two nerves, * For the more important synonyms of the cerebral nerves so called, see Chapter II. f First and second edition, p. 23. J Op. cit. p. 236. § On the nerves; giving an Account of some Experiments on their Structure and Functions. Read before the Royal Society of London, July 12, 1820. 45 whose origins are different, perform two functions; and that organs whose nerves are derived from sev- eral sources, effect various, and not merely stronger, functions of one specific kind. He regards as res- piratory nerves the pneumogastric, with its vocal branch; the glossopharyngeal; the accessory of Willis; * the facial; the hypoglossal, and the dia- phragmatic (the phrenic!) Mr. Bell has further established a difference in function between the facial nerve, (portio dura of the seventh pair,) and the branches of the fifth pair. He has proved, by his experiments, that the facial gives no impression of pain when it is cut, but that the effect of the operation is to abstract the parts upon which it is ramified from the influence of the respiratory motions. Lesions, on the contrary, of the fifth' pair, produce pain, and the parts it sup- plies become insensible, when its branches are cut across. He still further cites cases, in which sensi- bility remained unimpaired, although the side of the face was completely paralyzed in its movements. Mr. Bell has given his attention more particu- larly to the influence exerted by the facial nerve on respiration, and by the function of respiration on the parts that receive branches from this source. The facial he styles, in particular, nerve of expres- sion. Being a nerve of motion, however, it may, independently of any additional consideration, be * J. F. Meckel, in his Manual of Anatomy, translated by Doane, observes in a note, on the accessory nerve that it is wrongly called the accessory of Willis, as it had previously been figured by Eus- tachius, and described by Coiter. t See note on the page preceding. 46 entitled a nerve of expression. That it may be in- fluenced by respiration, no one will probably deny; but it may also unquestionably act independently of this function. Mr. Bell, indeed, has not in this instance adhered rigorously to his principle recog- nized in theory, and mentioned above, viz. that no nerve performs two functions—the facial, on his own data, can only be a nerve of one kind of function. I chanced very lately to see a case where the voluntary motion of the right side of the face was lost, whilst its sensibility remained; both feeling and motion on the left side were unimpaired; movements of the tongue were also natural; but if the person attempted to spit, the saliva was always thrown to the right by the action of the left and healthy half of the cheek and lips. M. Serres gives the case of a young man, who was constitutionally epileptic, and troubled with a slight ophthalmy in the right eye. The latter affec- tion increased gradually, the cornea became opaque, the sight decayed more and more, and was, in fine, entirely destroyed ; the other organs of sense and motion on the same side were successively para- lyzed; the eye, eye-lid, nostril, and half of the tongue, lost their powers, whilst on the left they remained in a state of perfect integrity. This pa- tient died. On dissection, there was found, first, an organic change of the ganglion of the fifth pair; it was enlarged, of a yellow color, and vascular below; second, a conversion of the nerve itself, at its insertion into the annular protuberance, into a yellow gelatinous-looking matter, similar to the ganglion, and penetrating, in little processes, the 47 substance of the protuberances in the direction of the nervous insertions. The muscular branches of the nerve, however, were found healthy on the otherwise affected side; the process of mastication therefore had never been injured. The difference in the functions of the facial nerve and fifth pair, afford a new proof of the dis- tinction between nerves of feeling and nerves of motion. M. Magendie* found that sensibility was destroy- ed when the dorsal roots of the spinal nerves were cut through, and that motion suffered when the abdominal origins were the subjects of his experi- ments. He also observed that the abdominal side of the spinal cord was less sensible to pricking or cutting than the dorsal; but that the introduction of a probe along the axis of the part did not seem to have influence either upon motion or sensation. The case of a man, who died in his sixty sixth year at Charenton, seems to corroborate these ideas. During the last seven years of his life his organs of motion had been paralyzed, but those of sensation remained uninjured; his intellectual faculties were almost annihilated, and his excretions were all in- voluntary. Thus reduced, he died. On opening the body, the pyramidal and olivary bodies were found pulpy, and of a dirty gray color. The same change was observed along nearly the whole of the anterior surface of the spinal cord, and penetrating through almost the whole thickness of the fibrous bundles that compose it. The abdominal roots of the spinal nerves were still visible, but their con- * Journal de Physiologie Experimental et Pathologique, torn. iii. 48 sistence was much diminished. The dorsal surface of the cord, on the contrary, and its investing mem- branes, were in a healthy condition. I am, how- ever, rather inclined to question the accuracy of the report in reference to the brain and cerebel- lum, when they are stated to have been in a nat- ural state; for I observe that the skull was of an ivory hardness, and three times thicker than com- mon. Such changes of the cranium are, I believe, constantly accompanied by alterations in the ence- phalic masses. M. Magendie also mentions a singular case, ob- served by M. Rullier, of a man who died at the age of forty-four. Up to his last hour, this person possessed great moral energies, strong generative powers, free motion of his lower limbs, and perfect sensation in his upper extremities. The arms, how- ever, were rigid, their muscles being permanently contracted, and often painful. They were rotated inwards, and pressed to the sides of the body, from which they could not be separated, but with some considerable effort. The fore-arms were in a state of uneasy pronation, the hands flexed, and all the fingers bent. On dissecting the body, after death, ' the spinal cord, examined with care, appeared in its natural state, from its upper end as low down as the exit of the fourth pair of cervical nerves. The dorsal surface of its two lower thirds was also healthy; but between the portions named, and through a space corresponding to the branching of eight or nine pairs of nerves, (six or seven inches in extent,) there was a very decided alteration. The spinal cord was there so extremely soft and 49 diffluent, that the sheath formed by the dura mater seemed filled with a true fluid, which, indeed, flowed upwards or downwards, as the body was inclined. A puncture being made through the sheath, a considerable quantity of the fluid in- stantly escaped.' The last passage of this report, in reference to the puncture of the dura mater, does not seem to me very exact; because in a preceding sentence it is stated, that ' below the part of the arachnoid, which adhered to the spinal cord, its proper mem- brane seemed charged with a great number of ves- sels, both arteries and veins, all loaded with blood.' This description leads me to conceive that the dura mater was slit lengthwise, and separated from the arachnoid and proper investing membrane of the spinal cord. If this were not so, the dura mater must then have adhered to the two inner mem- branes, and the puncture, consequently, could not have been confined to the first alone. I am con- firmed in this opinion by what M. Magendie says, when he adds, that the man had probably lost a good third of the nervous matter of the spinal cord, that the communication between its cervical and dorsal parts was, so to say, maintained by the mem- brane only ; for there seemed to remain no more of its entire thickness than a layer of white substance, scarcely two lines broad, and probably altered in its structure. The state of the nervous pairs, corresponding tq the part of the spinal cord that was destroyed, ought also, in my opinion, to have been mentioned. 7 50 Information on the structure of the remaining thin layer of nervous substance would likewise have been interesting: was it composed of longitudinal fibres, serving as a means of communication be- tween the upper and lower portions of the spinal cord ? Is it at all probable, that this office was performed by the sheath, as stated? If it was, the sheath, most certainly, was neither the arach- noid nor the dura mater. The neurilema, that en- velopes the pulpy nervous substance immediately, could alone execute such an office. M. Boulay, jun., veterinary surgeon at Paris, re- lates the case of a horse, whose hind legs were com- pletely paralyzed, whilst their sensibility was ex- treme. On opening his body, the whole of the lower part of the spinal cord was found soft and diffluent. There were no traces of change in the superior portion. The nervous substance of the lumbar and sacral pairs of nerves was of little con- sistence, and their sheaths were red and inflamed. The distinction of the nervous roots into dorsal and abdominal, accords with the two sorts of func- tion performed by the spinal mass. This subject, however, still requires elucidation; for M. Magen- die remarks, that {when the posterior roots, cov- ered even with their sheath, are irritated, signs of extreme suffering are manifested; and, what is particularly deserving of notice, contractions of those muscles that receive nerves from below the place so irritated are excited; these contractions too, only occur on the side of the body the nervous fibres of which are pricked.' According to the ob- servations of the same author, the abdominal sur- 51 face of the spinal cord is not altogether insensible when irritated. The communication between the spinal cord and the nerves of the vegetative func- tions is also known to be established by means of fibres, which communicate directly with the ab- dominal roots of the spinal nerves; nevertheless, the will has no influence over the functions of the viscera. Moreover, the fibres of the dorsal roots are evidently larger than those of the abdominal; both are in proportion to the volume of the parts to which they are distributed, and both send off branches that run into muscles. It is improbable, therefore, that the dorsal roots are solely destined to general sensation. Neither does it seem to me at all likely, that the spinal cord and its nerves are mere conductors of sensation, and of volition in reference to motion. I rather conceive that they aid in maintaining the powers of those parts to which they are distributed; for instance, that the muscles, or instruments of motion, acquire their power, in part, through the influence of their nerves, whilst the will to make the muscles act resides in the brain. Thus, I do not believe that the only office of the spinal cord, with its nervous roots, is to establish a communication between external im- pressions and the brain, and between the brain and the instruments of motion—the muscles. To me it seems probable, that a very small part of the spinal cord suffices for these purposes; the partic- ular portion, or organ, however, cannot, in the present state of our knowledge, be specified. The experiments of M. Magendie prove the abdominal 52 roots to include the conductors of volition; but as each of these is composed of two halves, the one superior, the other inferior, and in man, of two dis- tinct cords, it Avould be interesting to repeat and to extend the experiments., and, by cutting the halves separately, to ascertain whether both prop- agate the dictates of the will; or, if this task is limited to one, to that, namely, which does not communicate with the intercostal nerves. The ganglions of the intercostal nerves, as well as those of the dorsal roots of the spinal cord, may possibly prevent the will influencing the functions of the parts, to which these nerves are distributed. The set of experiments instituted by Dr. Bellin- geri, and detailed in a Memoir read before the Royal Academy of Sciences at Turin, in Febru- ary, 1824, do not tend to throw any new light on this interesting subject. They confirm the general idea upon the presence of separate nerves of sen- sation and of motion in the spinal cord; but they farther accord motion to the nerves that issue from the dorsal roots. Dr. Bellingeri says, his experi- ments prove, 1st, that the posterior roots of the lumbar and sacral nerves produce the motions of extension in the lower extremities; 2d, that the posterior roots alone preside over sensation; 3d, that the anterior roots produce the motions of flexion in the sacral extremities, and do not aid in perceiving external impressions ; 4th, that the pos- terior bundles of the spinal cord preside over the motions of extension of the inferior extremities, and have no connexion with perceptions of touch; 53 5th, that the white substance of the spinal cord, and the nervous fibres that arise from it, are appro- priated to motion; and, 6thly, that the gray sub- stance of the cord and the nervous fibres that spring from it, belong to sensation. I have copied these statements from the ' Revue Encyclopedique,' vol. xxi. p. 723. The ' Bulletin des Sciences Medicales,' for June, 1825, also re- cords these experiments, the inferences from which are in opposition to those deduced by M. Magendie from his own. I do not think, with Dr. Bellingeri, that the ver- tebral nerves can be divided into those which come from the wiiite, and those which issue from the gray substance of the spinal cord; because, on ex- amining the structure of this part and its nerves, I find that the origins of the latter invariably present the same appearance: they are universally im- planted, as it were, into the gray substance of the cord. From all I have said, it must be evident that I do not doubt the presence of two distinct species of nerves among those that issue from the spinal cord in particular, one propagating impressions from without, the other conveying the dictates of voli- tion, in regard to motion, from within. This sub- ject, however, is obviously involved in much ob- scurity, and will require ulterior and farther in- vestigation, to be rendered clear. Let us now examine the structure of the nervous mass of the spine, known under the title of spinal cord, or spinal marrow. 54 CHAPTER I. Of the Nervous Mass of the Vertebral Column. The spinal cord is found in all vertebral animals, whilst in the avertebralia, such as worms, insects, the Crustacea, and moliusca, the nerves form mass- es, which are separate and distinct in the ratio of their functions. The digestive powers of the aver- tebralia commonly predominate over those of gen- eral sensibility; and the same ganglions that sup- ply the viscera with nerves, also supply the mus- cles. In the vertebralia, on the contrary, where sensation and motion play principal parts, there are particular nervous masses destined to each kind of function. The spinal cord having always the same func- tions, may be conveniently compared in the four classes of vertebral animals. In all, its structure is essentially the same; modifications only are to be observed. General Considerations on the Spinal Cord of Man and Animals. The nervous mass of the spine is composed of two similar halves, one on each side of the mesial plane of the body. They are parted to a certain depth by two longitudinal clefts; the one, of course, on the dorsal, (pi. i. fig. 5, 5 — 5) the other on the abdominal surface, (pi. i. fig. 4, 5 — 5) and united 55 between these fissures by a commissure, or appara- tus of union, (pi. i. fig. 6 and 7. a — a.) This com- missure is pierced in its interior by a canal, which is more or less distinct in different animals, (pi. i. fig. 8 and 9,) and is especially visible during the earliest periods of life. The canal of the spinal cord has given rise to much discussion, and nothing certain is even now concluded in regard to it, either as to the space it occupies, or to the mode of its formation. Before explaining my own views upon this point, I shall quote a passage, published in the first volume of our large work on the ' Anatomy and Physiology of the Brain and Nervous System.' 'M. Demangeon and Messrs. Devilliers, uncle and nephew, afforded us the opportunity of examin- ing a case of Spina bifida, conjoined with consid- erable hydrocephalus. From the second to the fourth lumbar vertebra, the spinous processes of which were wanting, there extended a membranous pouch about two inches in diameter. During the eighteen days which the child survived its birth, a great quantity of fluid exuded continually from this pouch. Having cut away some of the other verte- brae, we observed no swelling of the dura materf neither did any fluid escape when we slit open this membrane lengthwise. It was only between the arachnoid and the proper vascular coat, that a small quantity had accumulated, and it communi- cated with the pouch. ' The spinal cord was of its usual form. We shook the head, and turned it in every direction, but could perceive no communication between the 56 water collected in the ventricles of the cerebral hemispheres and the spinal cord or its membranes. ' To learn, positively, whether or not any com- munication did exist between the pouch and inte- rior of the spinal cord, we cut this across in the neck : it appeared in the usual state; on blowing, however, through a pipe upon the transverse cut, each half of the cord presented an opening, about the magnitude of a middle-sized goose-quill. The two canals were separated by the commissure. We could not inflate the spinal cord through its entire length at once. We could only effect this partially. Downwards, the air penetrated freely for three inches, but there the canals were no farther pervious, so that no communication existed between them and the pouch. Neither could we perceive that there was any fluid in the canals. The pouch, itself, was formed partly by the dura mater and tunica arachnoides. Its superior edge touched the lower extremity of the spinal cord. Although we had blown into the two halves of the spinal marrow in so easy and uniform a manner, we still could not determine whether the canals were to be considered as an effect of disease or not. We, therefore, examined the spines of children at the period of birth, of others somewhat older, and of adults; in all we discovered a corresponding or- ganization. We, however, found that the canals did not expand before the blow-pipe so readily in children somewhat advanced, and in adults, as they did in subjects in the earliest infancy; we, therefore, to show the fact, prefer the bodies ol 57 newly-born children. In other respects, precisely similar openings on each side are to be observed in all; the interior surfaces too, being always smooth. If the blowing into the canals be con- tinued from below until as many as six or eight lines are opened, and only five or six lines are cut off in succession, so that the opening may be always maintained, the canals may be followed into the medulla oblongata, the tuber annulare, under the corpora quadrigemina, through the crura cerebri, and even on to the supposed optic thalami and the commencement of the corpora striata. Al- though no opening be observable into the fourth ventricle, nor into the aqueduct of Sylvius, nor into any other of the cerebral cavities, this structure, nevertheless, would incline us to admit the possi- bility of a fluid being secreted by the canals, and producing a true dropsy of one or both cavities of the spinal cord. ' Each half of the spinal cord, then, may be con- sidered as a membrane doubled on itself, along the middle line of which the gray substance, although without apparent division, may, by a slight blast, be separated, so as to form the sides of a canal. ' No violent separation can here be supposed; for, in the first place, it is effected with perfect ease, whilst in other situations, and even where the gray substance is much softer, no disjunction can be effected without the greater difficulty. Sec- ondly, where a separation of parts is the conse- quence of violence, the edges are ragged and un- even ; whilst in the case cited, the surfaces are con- 8 58 tinuous and smooth. Thirdly, the openings in the situations mentioned, are always found in the same places, are perfectly equal, being sometimes quite round, sometimes oval-shaped, sometimes slightly angular, and running now horizontally, then ver- tically, obliquely, or in a semicircular direction, according as the curves, of the medulla oblongata, of the pons Varolii, of the crura cerebri, of the optic thalami, or the parts that surround the canal de- termine. If the air be propelled too forcibly, the canal will burst where the gray substance is the most slightly covered by nerves, as at the places where the nerves issue, although the tearing off of these does not, of itself, give any outlet to the air.' Since the above was written, I have seen reason to change my opinion, and I now consider the canals to be mere effects of the blowing. I have produced them in birds and fishes, as well as in man. They are not, however, I must here observe, to be confounded with the true canal of the spinal cord—the canal that exists in the interior of the commissure or apparatus of union. This is quite uniform, and is occasionally the seat of disease, getting filled with serum. It is more or less dis- tinct in animals of the inferior classes, and in the embryos of those of superior tribes. In the human foetus, it is commonly visible during the first four or five months from conception. After this date it is generally, though not invariably, obliterated. This explains how its existence has been at one time admitted, at another denied, and how, in ex- traordinary cases, it has even been found in ad- 59 vanced old age, as Charles Etienne, Columbus, Morgagni, Senac, Portal, and other authors have observed. The canal of the spinal cord appears to be analogous to the aqueduct of Sylvius. M. Serres has said that the spinal marrow in young embryos consists of two cords,, which unite first in front and compose a gutter, but which coming together, speedily get blended behind. The interior of the spinal marrow is then hollow, and forms a long canal, which is gradually filled up by the deposition of successive layers of gray substance secreted by the pia mater, which has insinuated itself into the canal. This account does not appear to me correct and conformable to nature. I allow that the two cords unite at the bottoms of the clefts; but I likewise maintain that each contains gray matter in its in- terior, in the direction of the two nervous roots that are implanted. But were the spinal mass de- veloped, and its canal obliterated, as M. Serres would have us believe, by the deposition of gray substance in successive layers, this process ought, evidently, to go on in the interior of each of the cords composing it, and not in the canal between the two similar halves. The corresponding parts of each half of the spinal cord are voluminous in the ratio of their implanted nerves. This proposition is strikingly exemplified, by comparing the spinal cord of animals, whose superior are larger than their inferior extremities, with that of others, whose inferior are of greater size than their superior limbs. mm 60 I have already said that there is a mass of gray substance contained in the interior of each half of the spinal cord, proportionate, in quantity, to the volume of the corresponding parts of the same, and to the nervous roots that issue at the same place. The gray substance is disposed in a crescent form on each side of the apparatus of union, towards which its concave aspects are turned. (PI. i. fig. 9 and 10.) The white fibres of the spinal cord, on both the abdominal and dorsal surfaces, follow the gray sub- stance throughout its extent, so that there are two nervous roots in each half: one dorsal, another ab- dominal. (PL i. fig. 9 and 10.) The dorsal roots gen- erally, and the entire dorsal mass (pi. i. fig. 9 and 10,) are more considerable than the abdominal roots and corresponding half. The dorsal roots communicate with the intervertebral ganglions (pi. i. fig. 3,4, a—a,) and the latter are proportionate to the former. No division of the spinal cord into dorsal and ab- dominal portions is effected by two such channels as separate it laterally into halves ; it is, at most, only marked by ligamentum dentatumby which the cord, on each side is attached to the dura mater. (PI. i. fig. 3,4,5, and 6.) Neither is it proper to view the lateral portions of the spinal cord, as composed of three bundles extending from one of its extremi- ties to the other. They are much rather to be con- sidered^as nervous membranes folded on themselves, and forming a tube, containing gray substance. (PI. i. fig. 10.) The fissures on the abdominal and dorsal aspects of the spinal cord, are quite constant. The first is 61 shallower, but more conspicuous, than the other. (PI. i. fig. 10.) The abdominal nervous roots of the spinal cord communicate with the nerves of the thorax and ab- domen ; those of the dorsal surface frequently in- osculate, and filaments from one pair of nerves often run to join the fasciculi composing another. (PI. i. fig. 5.) Among the general considerations on the spinal cord, the question respecting its uses still remains for examination. Anatomists have been in the frequent habit of speaking of a central mass in the nervous system, from which they conceive all its other parts to arise. The brain, cerebellum, and spinal cord, have very commonly been so entitled. M. de Blainville, how- ever, separates the brain and cerebellum from the central mass. These he arranges, along with the external senses and intervertebral ganglions, under the general title, Ganglionary portion of the nervous system; and he confines the central mass exclu- sively to the spinal cord and medulla oblongata. In his view, the ganglionary is only added to, and not derived from, or produced by, the central por- tion. The two nervous roots of the spinal cord are, he says, mere fibres of communication between the central part of the nervous system and the nerves of sensation and voluntary motion which originate in the intervertebral ganglions. To me, anatomical, as well as physiological, facts, seem to militate against this opinion of M. de Blainville. In the first place, the intervertebral 62 ganglions belong to the dorsal roots only of the spinal nerves. Our author, consequently, forgets to mention the origin and use of the abdominal roots entirely. But is it not probable, that both dorsal and abdominal roots arise in the same man- ner ? The filaments of the spinal roots, too, are in proportion to the corresponding masses of the cord whence they issue. Now, as the abdominal roots have no intervertebral ganglions, and as they are evidently detached from the spinal cord, ought we not to conclude that the dorsal roots are so in like manner ? Another proof of the erroneousness of M. de Blainville's opinion exists in the size of the nervous filaments of the spinal cord. The apparatus of com- munication is never so large as the parts that com- municate : what a difference, for example, between the volume of the nervous masses of vegetative and of phrenic life, and their communicating fibres! Neither is M. de Blainville's opinion supported by physiology. The ganglionary portion, he conceives to be sufficient for the performance of its own func- tions ; that is to say, adequate of itself to feel, and to cause the execution of voluntary motion; what then becomes of the central portion ? what is its use? Admitting it to establish communications between the different parts of the nervous system, it cannot, however, exist solely for such a purpose ; its volume is by much too considerable to permit such a supposition to be entertained. Again, it most certainly is not destined to the affective and intellectual operations, for there is no proportion 63 between them and the development of the spinal cord. On the contrary, it is certain that general sensation and voluntary motion are in direct rela- tion to the volume of the nervous masses of the vertebral column. Animals, eminently endowed with these two functions, have always a considera- ble spinal cord, and its parts are proportionate to the organs which receive their nerves from thence. The several parts of the spinal cord are also aug- mented, as the apparatuses of sensation and of mo- tion are increased in number or size. Further, the spinal cord is developed at a much earlier period of life than the cerebral masses: it has acquired solid- ity and firmness, while the brain is still pulpy and devoid of fibres; and, in harmony with this law, children display great muscular activity, — their love of bodily exercise is insatiable, before their mental faculties appear in any degree of vigor. M. Serres, who admits all the organic systems, the nervous in particular, to be developed from the circumference towards the centre, maintains that the nerves are fully developed when the spinal cord and brain are still in a fluid state. In con- formity with this his hypothesis, he denies that the spinal cord gives origin to its nerves. These, ac- cording to him, are only implanted in it. Dr. Bailly conceives, as I have said, the same nervous elements to belong to every particular ver- tebra, i. e. nerves of sensation, of motion, of diges- tion, and of the intellectual functions. He speaks of eight cords that compose the intellectual system of the vertebral column : the superior median cord, 64 continuous with the pyramidal eminence, and ter- minating anteriorly in the cerebral hemisphere; the lateral inferior cord, ending in the internal layer of the corpora quadrigemina; the superior lateral cord, which terminates in the cerebellum; and the inferior median cord, ending in the lateral convo- lutions of the medulla, — convolutions which are most largely developed in cartilaginous fishes, and which correspond to the ribbon of gray substance of mammiferous animals. These eight longitudinal cords, four on each side, according to this author, exercise functions analogous to those of the cerebral hemispheres, the internal slip of the quadrigeminal bodies, the cerebellum, and the lateral convolutions of the fourth ventricle in fishes, or the gray band in the mammalia. The medulla oblongata in the same gentleman's view, presents alternately an intellectual cord and a nervous pair. These physiological suppositions will, I make no doubt, share the fate of so many others, that are now forgotten. In my opinion, the spinal cord is, 1st, the origin of the nerves styled spinal; 2d, an apparatus that contributes to muscular and sensi- tive powers; and 3d, a means of communication between the cerebral operations, the sense of touch, the power of motion in general, and the functions of vegetative life collectively. Particular Considerations in regard to the Spinal Cord. Before the development of the extremities in the embryo, in animals also that naturally have none, 65 and in cases of monstrosity where they are wanting, the spinal cord has no enlargements. These appear with, and are in proportion to, the extremities. In the lower animals the spinal cord extends far- ther into its bony canal than in the human kind. Up to the fourth month of the human embryo's ex- istence, it runs to the extremity of the coccyx; but the peculiar structure, termed cauda equina, which is more remarkable in man than in any other ani- mal, becomes more and more apparent after this period. The caudal appearance is produced by the sacral and lumbar nerves. The spinal cord of the human kind commonly terminates by one or two little knobs, (of which the superior, when there are two, is the larger,) op- posite to the second lumbar vertebra, and it is then attached, by means of a tendinous slip, to the bot- tom of the vertebral canal. The spinal cord is commonly divided into four portions: a cervical, dorsal, lumbar, and sacral. Each of these detaches several pairs of nerves, the number of which varies in different species of animals; in man it amounts to thirty pairs, five be- ing sacral, a like number lumbar, twelve dorsal, and seven cervical. The spinal cord of man, as of animals, is enlarg- ed, and contains a larger quantity of gray sub- stance at those places where the great nerves of the extremities are detached, than at any other. Although many anatomists, M. Serres among the rest, deny that the spinal cord of man and animals is enlarged at, and contracted between, the origin 9 66 of each pair of nerves, I still adhere to the opinion which, in conjunction with Dr. Gall, I published long ago; and I again appeal to nature for confir- mation of its accuracy. These alternate enlarge- ments and contractions are more or less conspicuous, in the ratio of the volume of the nervous pairs; they are, for instance, more apparent in the ox and horse than in man. If, however, the human spinal cord, stripped of its dura mater and arachnoid covering, be held profile-wise against the light, the undulated line of its edges will be abundantly obvious. In the large work on the ' Anatomy of the Brain and Nerv- ous System,' we have given drawings of .this struc- ture from the spinal cord of the calf, and of man. PI. i. fig. 3, is a portion of a spinal cord of a calf. Several anatomists have spoken of numerous transverse folds to be observed, especially on the ab- dominal surface of the spinal cord: these become very distinct when the cord is taken from out its bony canal, and one end is brought towards the other; they disappear entirely, however, when the part is stretched. Such folds, therefore, appear to result from the bending of the cord, and not to be- long to any original peculiarity of organization. On either side, and at some little distance from the great median fissure, two superficial channels may be observed on the dorsal surface of the human spinal cord (pi. i. fig. 5, a—-a.) They extend as far as, or a little way beyond, the first dorsal vertebra. The two bands between the median and these su- perficial fissures, are developed at the earliest peri- od ; their structure is even complete, when the rest 67 of the spinal cord is still a grayish and pulpy mass. The bands on the external edges of the dorsal and abdominal median fissures, appear in general, to attain maturity of structure before any of the lat- eral masses. By opening the mesial abdominal fissure, and stripping off the vascular tissue, the nervous fibres will be seen running lengthwise and parallel to the lateral bands, (pi. i. fig. 6;) but in the dorsal cleft, they will be found descending perpendicularly from its surface towards its bottom. (PI. i. fig. 7.) The structure of the apparatus of union has not the same appearance at the bottom of both fissures. If the edges of the one on the dorsal aspect be gent- ly separated, two white bands will be discovered running lengthwise (pi. i. fig. 7, a—a,) almost as occurs in the middle line of the great cerebral com- missure (raphe of the corpus callosum;) on the bot- tom of the abdominal fissure, on the contrary, fila- ments will be observed running transversely from the sides towards the median line. These bundles do not meet; they rather interlace, each terminat- ing on either side between the two that come from the opposite side. (PI. i. fig. 6, a—a.) Besides the peculiarities of its shortness compar- ed with the vertebra] canal, and of its termination in the cauda equina, the spinal cord of man pre- sents another striking peculiarity in the direction in which its nerves are detached. The direction in which the spinal nerves are sent off, varies wide- ly in different classes of animals. In man, their course is downwards and outwards: this depends 68 evidently on the vertical posture, by which man is distinguished from the other mammalia, and on the shortness of his spinal cord compared with its bony canal. In the human kind, only the first pair of cer- vical nervous roots on the dorsal surface (pi. i. fig. 5,) and the two first pairs upon the abdominal (pi. i. fig. 4,) have one set of bundles coming from above, downwards, and another from below, up- wards ; all the other pairs are detached more and more obliquely downwards, to gain the vertebral holes by which they issue, in proportion as they arise nearer to the sacral extremity of the spinal cord. In other animals, on the contrary, whose spinal nervous masses occupy the entire length of the vertebral canal, the nerves are sent off directly opposite to the intervertebral spaces at which they issue. (PI. i. fig. 3.) I have still to speak of the superior extremity of the spinal cord. By the words spinal marrow or cord, most anatomists understand the nervous mass that extends throughout the spinal canal up to the occipital hole; others, Messrs. Soemmerring and Chaussier among the number, say that it goes as far as the annular protuberance. They conse- quently include the pyramidal and olivary bodies, the accessory, pneumogastric, glossopharyngeal and hypoglossal nerves; all the mass, in short, com- monly named medulla oblongata. They speak of two portions—one cranial, another spinal, of the spinal cord ; the first being, of course, contained in the skull, the second in the canal of the vertebral column. 69 The organization of the spinal nervous mass can- not be said to change entirely when it enters the cranium : several pairs of nerves are still detached precisely in the same manner as they are in the spine: the nerves too, in both cases, perform sim- ilar functions. The proper spinal masses are also intimately connected with those of the medulla ob- longata. In this last part, however, the abdominal fissure is interrupted by the decussating bundles of the pyramidal bodies : the size of the mass is also very sensibly increased here, and there is no regu- lar proportion between the proper spinal cord and the mass that extends from the occipital hole to the annular protuberance. Moreover, the roots of sev- eral cerebral nerves, as they are styled, are found in this last mass, as also the rudimentary parts of the brain and cerebellum. Lastly, the word spinal refers to a particular situation — the spine. These reasons may suffice to make us limit the title spinal cord to the mass included in the vertebral canal, and extending from the occipital hole, or commence- ment of the pyramidal decussation to the cauda equina or horse's tail. 70 CHAPTER II. Of the pretended Cerebral Nerves; or, of the Cranial Nerves of the External Senses and Voluntary Motion. It is usual for anatomists to consider the nervous mass lying between the occipital hole and annular protuberance, as distinct and particular. In former times, this part was taken for a prolongation of the brain and cerebellum, and, therefore, called medulla oblongata. In man it is composed of parts, named severally, pyramidal bodies, which are two in num- ber, anterior (pi. vi. fig. 1., 1—c.) and posterior (pi. xi. in the middle line ;) olivary bodies (pi. vi. fig. 1, a.;) restiform bodies (pi. xi. e. e.;) accessory nerves (pi. i. fig. 3, 4, and 5; pi. vi. fig. 1,2—3;) hypo- glossal nerves (pi. vi. 4;) pneumogastric nerves (ib. 6;) glossopharyngeal nerves (ib. 7 ;) auditory nerves (ib. 9;) facial nerves (ib. 11;) and abductor nerves (ib. 10.) At the conclusion of the last chapter, I remarked that it was a mistake to confound the part called medulla oblongata and the spinal cord together. I here subjoin that the medulla oblongata is not a separate and particular nervous mass : it gives or- igin to the nerves above mentioned, and also to the fifth pair; but there is yet one portion of each of its halves which belongs, decidedly, to the cerebellum, and another which pertains to the brain. 71 The volume of the medulla oblongata varies greatly in the different classes of animals: its size is determined by the nerves that arise from it, and by the bundles that proceed to the cerebellum and brain. Its increase beyond the size of the spinal cord, is more remarkable in reptiles than in fishes, in birds than in reptiles, and is especially consider- able in the mammalia, which, generally, have the medulla oblongata as well as spinal cord propor- tionately larger than man, because the nerves de- tached from these parts are larger in them than in the human kind. At present, I mean only to speak of the supposed cerebral nerves. The pyramidal, olivary, and resti- form bodies will be examined with the cerebellum and brain of which they are parts. The opinions of authors upon the origin of the nerves of the head are very various. In general they are derived from the brain—hence their name cerebral. But some writers limit the title brain to the hemispheres, and join the striated bodies, the optic thalami, the cerebral legs and annular protu- berance to the medulla oblongata; or, otherwise, they look on all these as parts of the spinal cord, and then say that no nerve whatever arises from the brain, that is, the hemispheres, immediately. By thus extending the limits of the medulla oblon- gata and spinal cord, however, parts that belong essentially to the brain are included. The princi- ple we lay down, therefore, that no nerve originates in the brain, and that every nervous part has its own origin, so that the nerves can no more be de- 72 rived from the brain than can the various nervous pairs from each other, cannot possibly be con- founded with any of the foregoing opinions of au- thors. The proofs we adduce in support of our position, as to the independent origin of the various nervous parts, are constantly the same; all, therefore, that has been said to demonstrate the spinal cord to be no continuation of the brain, applies to the nerves of the head, and proves that they do not owe their being to the brain, and that no one pair derives its existence from any other. In the first place, the nerves of the head bear no proportion to the brain in size; and then, these nerves exist in acephalous monsters, whose brain never had being. Moreover, the course taken by the nerves towards the cranial holes through which they pass, proves, in the most positive manner, that they are not continuations of the cerebral fibres. In examining the individual nerves, I shall speak of their several peculiarities, as origin, structure, and connexion are concerned. The nerves of the head, as of the spine, have been long classed into pairs. The number of pairs reck- oned, however, varies considerably; sometimes they are said to be seven, sometimes eight, or nine, or, according to M. Soemmerring's method of counting, as many as twelve. However, the mode of indica- ting the several pairs of nerves numerically, and speaking of the first pair, the second pair, the third pair, and so on, possesses no practical advantages, because, besides the number, the functions of each pair must still be learned. It is better, therefore, 73 to designate each pair of the cranial nerves in suc- cession from its functions or its destination.* Some authors have divided the nerves according to their places of detachment, into nerves of the brain, of the cerebellum, of the annular protuber- ance, and of the medulla oblongata. This attempt at classification is necessarily very deficient, for it is based upon an error in regard to the origins of the nerves; the actual place of origin being con- founded with that at which they issue immediately from the general mass. Of the Accessory Nerve.—Spinal, or Spinal Acces- sory of Willis.—3d Branch of the 8lh pair of J. Bell.—Superior Respiratory of C Bell. The accessory nerve is found in man and the other mammalia. (PL i. fig. 3, 4, 5, and pi. vi. fig. 1, 2—3.) It arises from the cervico-spinal mass and medulla oblongata. Its filaments come from the dorsal surface of these parts, and vary in num- ber, in thickness, and in length, not only in differ- ent individuals of different species, but even on the two sides of the same subject. The first filaments are detached at various heights, sometimes higher, sometimes lower, in the spinal cord, issuing from the level of the seventh cervical pair in one in- * In order to diminish somewhat of the confusion arising from a perusal of the various works on the brain and nervous system, each possessing a nomenclature different from the others, it has been thought proper to add some of their synonyms to each of the cranial nerves. E. 10 74 stance, and from that of the fifth in another. The accessory recedes from the spinal cord and medul- la oblongata, as it approaches the pneumogastric nerve, along with which it escapes from the crani- um. I have already said, that Mr. Charles Bell arranges the accessory among the nerves of respi- ration. In contributing to this function, it produ- ces motion, and is influenced by the will: never- theless, it is detached, as we have seen, from the dorsal surface of the spinal cord. Of the Pneumogastric Nerve.—Par vagum.—Mid- dle Sympathetic.— Vocal.—Pulmonary 2d branch of the 8th pair. The pneumogastric occurs in all vertebral ani- mals. In man (pi. vi. fig. 1, 6,) it issues by numer- ous filaments between the olivary, (ibid, a,) and the restiform (e e) bodies; nearer to the latter, however, than to the former. It unites with a great number of other nerves, a circumstance that has obtained for it the title vagus. Its branches run to the larynx and pharynx, to the thyroid gland, the vessels of the neck, and the great con- duits of the heart, to the lungs, the liver, spleen, pancreas, stomach, and duodenum. As some of its principal branches are distributed to the organs of voice, and as its lesions derange the functions of these parts, it has also been called the vocal nerve. The communications by its means established, and its extensive distribution, explain the sympathies that exist between the throat, lungs, stomach, heart, &c. 75 Of the Glossopharyngeal Nerve. In the mammalia and man, (pi. vi. fig. 1, 7,) this nerve comes off from the medulla oblongata, just before the pneumogastric, by a great many fila- ments, which, speedily uniting into one or more bundles, compose a trunk that runs to supply the pharynx and muscles of the tongue. It appears to be destined to general sensation. Of the Hypoglossal Nerve.—9th pair of Wiilis.— Gustatorius of Winsloiv.—Singualis of Vicq. oV Azyr.—Sublingual. The hypoglossal in man arises partly near the olivary and pyramidal bodies, and partly lower down, by several filaments that are detached, and get united after the manner of the cervical nerves. (PL vi. fig. 1,4.) It supplies the tongue with mo- tive power, and acts in mastication, deglutition, speaking, singing, &c. Of the Abductor Nerve of the Eye.—6th pair of Willis.—External Motor of Meckel. This nerve arises in all mammiferous animals, from the abdominal surface of the medulla oblon- gata. (PL vi. fig. 1, 10.) In some, as the horse, ox, and deer, it mounts all the way along with, and by the side of, the pyramidal bodies, in the form of 76 a band, which, on reaching the annular protuber- ance, divides into two. In the human subject it is generally covered by some transverse fibres of the protuberance. Its distribution, as its name implies, is to the abductor muscle of the eye.* Of the Facial Nerve.—Portio Dura of the 7th pair. —Nervicus communicans faciei of J. Bell. The facial is detached from the spinal cord in the same manner as the nerves I have just discuss- ed. Its true origin is readily seen in the lower an- imals; but in man, (pi. vi. .fig. 1,11,) it (or some of its filaments at least) seems to come from the annular protuberance. This happens in conse- quence of its being covered entirely, or partially, by the transverse fibres of the parts mentioned. The distribution of this nerve is to the muscles of the face; it also communicates freely with all the three branches of the fifth pair. Of the Motor Nerve of the Eye.—3d pair of Willis. —The common Motor of Meckel. The filaments composing this nerve, (pi. vi. and pi. x. fig. 1, 15,) issue from the blackish body of * The origin of this nerve seems not to be confined to any par- ticular spot. The editor has seen it arise at the pyramidal decus^ sation, in the brain of a negro, and pass over and by the sides of the olivary bodies. In another brain, he has observed it arise from the annular protuberance, about a line from the base of the pyramidal bodies. 77 the cerebral legs (pi. x. fig. 1, 30;) theselinite and go to supply the superior, internal, inferior straight, and inferior oblique, muscles of the eye, and the el- evator of the upper eye-lid. It is detached in all vertebral animals, from the cerebral crura behind the tuber cinereum, or ash-colored tubercle, (pL vi. 17.,) which is situated immediately behind the junc- tion of the optic nerves. Of the Nerve of the Superior Oblique Muscle of the Eye. — Athpair or Patheticus of Willis. — Troch- learis. — Internal Motor of Meckel. This nerve, (pi. vi. 13,) springs by several fila- ments behind the posterior pair of quadrigeminal bodies in the mammalia, (pi. xi. fig. 1, o,) and be- hind the bigeminal bodies in the other classes of animals. It has no prerogative over the other nerves of motion in expressing the affections and passions; the name patheticus, or ogling nerve, therefore, which it has obtained, is misapplied. Although a nerve of motion, it arises from the dor- sal surface of the nervous mass. Of the Trigeminal Nerve.—5th pair of Willis.—Tri- facial of Meckel. The trigeminal (pi. vi. fig. 1, 12,) arises from the medulla oblongata. Its place of origin is apparent in those animals, as reptiles, birds, and fishes, which have no annular protuberance, and also in the 78 mammalia which have it of small size; but in man, the monkey, and other tribes, where this part is very large, the trigeminal seems to arise from it; the nerve, however, is, in fact, only covered by some of its transverse fibres, (pi. viii. fig. 1, i—k,) and the origin of the nerve is, in fact, the same in all animals. To show it in the human kind, the fibres of the annular protuberance that cover it, must, of course, be dissected away. The trigeminal nerve is distributed to every part of the face, to the muscles of the forehead, of the eyelids, nose, lips, jaws, and ears; it communicates with the organs of all the five senses, and of volun- tary motion, and brings these and the other parts of the body, the face, neck, trunk, and extremities, into mutual relationship. One branch of the trigeminal is ramified on the tongue, and is regarded as the true gustatory nerve. It is, therefore, destined to take cognizance of an impression of a specific kind, viz. taste. If some feel disposed to regard taste as a sort of touch, they must, at least, admit the impossibility of confound- ing or assimilating this species of perception with sensation in general. Of the Auditory Nerve. —Portio Mollis of the 1th pair. — Acoustic Nerve. The auditory (pi. vi. 9,) as well as the nerve last discussed, must exist in the avertebral tribes, but its origin and peripheral expansion only become 79 distinct and complex as we ascend in the scale of beings. The origin of the auditory nerve is in many fishes confounded with that of the facial and tri- geminal : in some kinds, however, it is more dis- tinct, and is the same as in reptiles and birds. The auditory nerve always arises laterally and posteri- orly to the cerebellum. In the mammalia, it comes from the fourth ventricle, and traverses in its course a band of gray substance, lying between the resti- form body and the cerebellum. In man certain white lines may commonly be seen in the fourth ventricle, (pi. xi. fig. 1. t,) which Piccoluomini was the first to consider as the origins of the auditory nerve. These lines, or streaks, are almost impercep- tible in some subjects, but in others they are very numerous and distinct. They are observed to vary on each side, being occasionally larger and more numerous on one than on the other. Sometimes they occur disposed in rays, sometimes in pencils, and, again, they run parallel, often lying higher on one than on the other side. They at one time ap- pear as little flattened bands, at another as rounded and salient cords. Piccoluomini's opinion of their use seems now to be very generally adopted; it is certain, indeed, that some of them do unite with the auditory nerve, but others run to the anterior cerebellar lobes, and others dip into the middle of the cerebellum. Although many of the mammalia have much larger auditory nerves than man, these white fibres are nevertheless wanting in all. In them, there is a broad band on either side, extend- ing from the one auditory nerve to the other, im- 80 mediately behind the annular protuberance, and above all the ascending bundles, except the pyra- midal bodies. (PL iv. fig. 2, c.) This band appears to be an apparatus of union or a commissure. It does not give origin to the facial nerve, as some anatomists have supposed. Of the Optic Nerve. — 2d pair of Willis. Since the days of Eustachius and Varolius, the majority of anatomists have derived this nerve (pi. vi. fig. 1, 20,) from certain masses, which have, in consequence, been entitled thalami of the optic nerves. (PL x. fig. 1. p.) Others, however, fol- lowed some of its fibres backwards to the anterior of the quadrigeminal bodies. (PL xi. fig. 1, n.) And it is easy in the mammalia to do this. A broad slip issues from the anterior quadrigeminal body of either side, which turns round upon the outer edge of the thalamus, simply superimposed upon the cerebral crura, but attached to the neigh- boring masses along its external edge, as far as its junction with its fellow of the opposite side. (PL viii. fig. 1, w, id, w.) From the tuber cinereum, (pi. vi. fig. 1, 17,) the optic nerve receives many additional fibres which join it in right lines, and without decussating. (PL viii. fig. 2, 20.) It is the opinion of many anatomists, that the op- tic nerves, at their junction, unite intimately with- out decussating; many others, on the contrary, think that either nerve crosses to the side opposite to that on which it had arisen. The numerous 81 cases described by authors, and the facts which Dr. Gall and I have collected, prove that atrophy of one optic nerve is continued on the opposite side after the junction of the two. We, therefore, agree with those who recognise a partial decussation of optic nerves in man and the mammalia. The fibres of the outermost por- tions appear to continue their course onwTards without decussating. It was a great error to consider the eminences called optic thalami, as the sources of the optic nerves. There is, in fact, no proportion whatever between these parts and the nerves of vision. In the horse, ox, sheep, &c, the optic nerves are as large as in man, but the thalami, in the human kind, are much larger than in these animals. A very cursory examination of the structure of the thalami also shows a meres uperficial layer attached to the optic nerve and the whole of the interior fibres proceeding in a divergent manner, backwards, to be distributed to the cerebral convo- lutions. (PL x. fig. I, p. P. P.) When the optic nerve is affected with atrophy, the corresponding thalamus is diminished only in as far as the nerve itself is lessened; the interior of the thalamus suffers no change, but the atrophy of the nerve continues on to the anterior quadrigemi- nal body which belongs to it. I once found, in the brain of a woman who had died insane, the thala- mus of the left sid« half converted into pus; the corpus striatum of the same side was also much shrunk, but the optic nerve was healthy, and 11 82 resembled, in all respects, its fellow of the opposite side, in the vicinity of which no organic change could be detected. The interior pair of quadri- geminal bodies were also in their natural state. The two thalami are rarely of equal size; the one on the left side is commonly the smaller. It, therefore, becomes necessary to guard against at- tributing to atrophy of an optic nerve an appear- ance that depends on natural conformation. Until Dr. Gall and I showed the mistake, the op- tic tubercles of birds and reptiles were confounded with the thalami; these tubercles (pi. xi. fig. 2 and 3, n,) however, correspond to the anterior pair of quadrigeminal bodies. The parts, called thalami (pi. xi. fig. 2, p.,) also exist in the two classes of creatures mentioned, besides the true optic gang- lions, (ib. n.) It is a difficult matter to say whether the long elevated bodies that occur immediately behind the crossing of the optic nerves in fishes (pi. ii. fig. 4, 6, 8, 16,) and correspond to the optic ganglions of birds, that is to say, to the pair of ganglions (pi. iii. fig. 5, 6, 7,10, and 11, n.) which comes immediately after the cerebellum, or whether they ought to be likened to the bodies which are styled mammillary. (PL vi. fig. 1, 16.) The optic nerves of fishes seem to arise from, at all events they communicate with, these bodies. By comparing pi. ii. fig. 4 and 6, 16, with pi. iii. fig. 12, n., the optic ganglions in birds, and especially in mammiferous animals, will be seen to be separated and pushed upwardly and lat- erally by the medulla oblongata, cerebral legs, 83 and annular protuberance. Moreover, the optic nerves in fishes adhere to several other parts, as they do in the higher classes of vertebral animals and in man. It is from this circumstance that a great part of the other masses, especially those that follow the cerebellum, arc called optic ganglions; but these bear no proportion to the optic nerves (pi. ii. fig. 5, 7, and 10, n,) and to me, it seems more reasonable to allow, with Mr. Arsaky,* that the complicated structure of the optic ganglions in fishes, explains the functions of the derived nerves which, in reptiles, birds, and the mammalia, are ev- idently aided in their functions by the other parts with which they communicate. The implantation of the optic nerve of fishes into the inner edge of its ganglion, which causes the greatest part of this mass to appear before and above it, bears a striking analogy to the adhesion of the same nerve to the brain in other classes of animals, and confirms the idea in question. It is rendered still more probable, as the same peculiar- ity of structure is found in connexion with the ol- factory nerves of fishes. And further, the oblong bodies, which, in fishes, lie behind the crossing of the optic nerves, cannot possibly be supposed anal- ogous to the mammillary bodies of mammiferous tribes, because these last belong to the fornix, and this is a part which fishes have not. I may still add that the-mammillary bodies bear no proportion to the optic nerves, and that the oblong bodies of fishes above mentioned do, regularly. * Dissertatio de Piscium Cerebro et Medulla Spinali; Halae. 1813. 84 The oblong bodies of fishes, probably, correspond to the gray tubercle, (tuber cinereum) of the mam- malia. This tubercle, in the higher classes of be- ings, always sends fibres to the optic nerves, which, after this accession, advance of increased size in their course. (PL viii. fig. 2.—17.) The connexion of the optic nerve with such a number of cerebral parts, renders its exact origin uncertain. To appreciate this truth in its full force, it will be necessary to reflect on what I have still to say of the quadrigeminal bodies. Of the Quadrigeminal Bodies. The title, corpora, or tubercula quadrigemina, is applied to two pairs of round elevations, situated behind the legs of the brain. They are only found in the mammalia and in man. (PL xi. fig. 1, n, o.) they are joined together by a transverse band (ib. x,) and they communicate with the part called valve of Vieussens (ib. y,) with the bundles that proceed from the medulla oblongata, and with the pretended optic thalami (ib. p.) I have already spoken of the large band belonging to the optic nerve, that issues from the anterior pair of these bodies. Being in communication with the bundles which come from the posterior part of the medulla oblongata, there can be no doubt but the quadri- geminal bodies have their origin lower down in this nervous mass; but as the optic nerves arise from 85 the anterior pair in the mammalia, and as these nerves issue in birds from a couple of ganglions, separated from the general cerebral mass, (for, in birds, they are only united to the bundles that pro- ceed from the medulla oblongata) the analogy and office of these ganglions cannot be called in ques- tion. The destination of the posterior pair of quad- rigeminal bodies is much less obvious. M. Serres* thinks that the bigeminal tubercles of fishes, reptiles, and birds, and the quadrigeminal bodies of mammiferous animals and man, are the same mass, destined, in all, to originate the optic nerves, and that the division into two pairs hap- pens merely from a transverse furrow, which, as it runs more or less forwards or backwards, equalizes them, or causes in one case the anterior, in another the posterior pair to predominate. The posterior are intimately connected with the anterior tubercles, it is true; but there is a white band which issues from the anterior pair, joins a small collection of gray substance, called corpus geniculatum externum (pi. viii. fig. 1, q.,) is thereby increased in size, and then continues its course into the optic nerve : in like manner there runs a band from the posterior pair, which unites with a mass of gray substance, entitled corpus geniculatum in- ternum (pi. xi. fig. 1, r.,) and gaining, consequently, in size, afterwards dips under the optic nerve, and is continued on towards the middle cerebral lobe. This structure proves, at the least, that the ante- * Anatomie du Cerveau; Preface. Rapport de M. Cuvier. 86 rior and posterior pairs of the quadrigeminal bodies are no parts of one and the same nervous mass. M. Serres also fancies that the corpora quadri- gemina serve as a basis, according to which the other parts of the encephalon are determined (' les tubercles quadrigeminaux servent de base a la deter- mination des autres parties de Vencephale ;') and he investigates their relations with many particular cerebral parts. His assumption seems to me as incorrect and untenable as the one I have just ex- amined, according to which the quadrigeminal bodies are parts of one mass similar to the bigem- inal tubercles of birds and reptiles. ' The quadrigeminal tubercles,' says M. Serres, ' are developed, in all the classes, and in every fam- ily of each class, in a direct ratio, as are the optic nerves and the eyes. Fishes have the largest quadrigeminal tubercles, and the most remarkable eyes and optic nerves. The very considerable size of their quadrigeminal bodies, indeed, has led anat- omists, up to the present time, into the error|of supposing them to be the hemispheres of the brain. After fishes come reptiles, then birds, next, among mammiferous animals, the rodentia, and in succes- sion, the ruminantia, the carnivora, the quadru- mana, and man.' I have already shown, that in fishes generally, the ganglions called optic, are disproportionate in size to the nerves of vision ; and I have said that part of these masses was, probably, destined to oth- er functions. The optic nerve of the carp (pi. ii. 87 fig. 4, 20) is smaller than that of the roach (pi. ii. fig. 11,20;) but the so styled optic ganglions (ib.n.) exist in these fishes in an inverse propor- tion. ' The spinal marrow and the corpora quadri- gemina,' says M. Serres in another place, c are so rigorously developed in the ratio of each other, that the size of the first being given in any class or in any of its families, the volume of the latter may be determined with precision.' But the bigeminal tubercles in the carp (pi. ii. fig. 5, n.) are much larger, in proportion to the spi- nal cord, than in the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1) and the roach (pi. ii. fig. 11.) M. Serres, himself, has giv- en representations of the optic apparatus in the cassowary, ostrich, and other birds, much larger in proportion to the spinal cord, than it is in the many mammiferous animals whose brains he has figured; and the disproportion between the devel- opment of the quadrigeminal bodies and spinal cord, is even greater in the dolphins and porpoises, than in the ox, camel, and horse. The most cursory glance over the physiology of living beings also shows the utter erroneousness of M. Serres's position. Powers of voluntary motion and of touch never bear any direct proportion to the faculty of vision. The mole is, certainly, far more remarkable for its muscular strength than for its eyesight, and the owl for its powers of vision than for its bodily vigor. I shall discuss the several relations which M. Serres believes he has found between the quad- 88 rigeminal bodies and other parts of the nervous system, as I treat of these in succession. M. Bailly, in his Memoir on the Comparative Anatomy of the Nervous System in the four classes of Vertebral Animals, maintains that the corpora quadrigemina anteriora et posteriora, the corpora geniculata externa et interna, are mere parts of the optic lobe of inferior classes. He speaks of the unfolding of the quadrigeminal bodies in fishes, and of two systems of fibres very different from each other; the one exterior, and belonging to the optic nerves; the other interior, and being the expansion of a cord of the medulla oblongata. 'In fishes and reptiles,' says he, ' the internal have a much greater relative development than the external fi- bres ; in mammiferous animals, the external fibres alone remain; the internal exist as mere rudi- ments.' Dr. Gall * also says he is ' convinced that the pos- terior pair of the corpora quadrigemina is a gangli- on, for the purpose of reinforcing or perfecting the optic nerve.' He adds —'The modes in which these two pairs assist the function of vision must differ, for they occur in various proportions to each other, in the different species of animals; in some, indeed, the posterior two are scarcely perceptible, or are even entirely wanting, although vision be perfect, as happens in the case of birds.' This diversity of opinion is to be accounted for, by recalling to mind the delicacy of the cerebral organization, and the intimate connexion of the * Sur les Fonctions du Cerveau.—Ed. in 8vo. Vol. 89 parts with each other. When anatomists observe one or two parts]connected together, they very com- monly conceive the one to be derived from the oth- er. By-and-by I shall treat, in a separate section, of the communications of the nervous masses, and of the importance of this arrangement. Meantime I shall pass my opinions respecting the quadrigem- inal bodies in review before my reader. The mammalia alone have quadrigeminal bodies. Both pairs, however, have not, I conceive, similar offices ; for there is no proportion between them in- discriminately, nor the bundles of fibres which issue in different directions from each. The fibres of the anterior pair, as I have said, join the optic nerves, those of the posterior plunge under the optic nerves, and are lost on the middle lobes of the brain. The optic nerve communicates, by means of superficial bands, with the posterior pair of the quadrigeminal bodies, with the internal and exter- nal geniculated bodies, with the middle and anterior cerebral lobes, and with the mass called tuber cinereum. Reptiles and birds have only one pair of rounded tubercles, before and by the sides of the cerebellum. (PL iii. fig. 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 10 and 11, n.) These are readily seen, by throwing back the cerebellum. On examining their structure, they are found, in the first place, to be hollow (pi. iii. fig. 9, side B. n,) and to detach a superficial layer that communicates with the medulla oblongata behind, and with the optic nerve and base of the brain before; and, in the second place, to be connected by their lower 12 90 parts with the optic nerves and the cerebral crura or legs. This structure of the optic ganglions in birds and reptiles, corresponds to that of the an- terior pair of the quadrigeminal and external gen- iculated bodies in mammiferous animals, as in them the one and superficial layer of the anterior pair is continued into the optic nerve, whilst the other and deeper is connected with the crura of the brain. In fishes, the optic nerves always communicate with the basis of the pair of tubercles that succeeds the cerebellum, and this pair is connected with the medulla oblongata; but it and the optic nerves have not, mutually, any regular or fixed propor- tion, whilst the optic nerves and the rounded tu- bercles that lie immediately behind their crossing, are constantly developed in the ratio of each other. The optic nerve, in fishes, moreover, communicates with the basis of the cerebral masses that come after the cerebellum, precisely as it is connected in the mammalia with the anterior and middle lobes of the brain. In proportion, therefore, as the cerebral masses and fibrous bundles, or their successive additions, diminish in number through the four classes of ver- tebral animals, the primary optic ganglions ap- proach each other, and lie between or among such cerebral parts as still exist; but as the brain gets complicated, and vision exerts an important influ- ence upon its functions, the primary optic ganglions lie backwards, in order that the apparatus of vision 91 may, conveniently, be brought into communication with the cerebral parts whose function it especially aids. Of the Olfactory Nerve,—1st pair of Willis.—Eth- moidal Nerve. Very different opinions have been and are still entertained by anatomists in regard to the origin of the nerve of smell. Some of the moderns have described it as arising from the masses called stri- ated bodies; but there is no proportion, whatever, between them and the nerve; and, further, porpoises and dolphins have the striated bodies, but no olfac- tory nerves. The nerves of smell also exist in many acephalous monsters, whose striated bodies are, of course, wanting. The olfactory nerve, in the human kind, has three roots ; of these, the interior (pi. vi. 21) is the short- est, but broadest, and the exterior the longest (ib. 18,) for it extends to the bottom of the fissure of Sylvius. These different roots are, as it were, im- pacted in the cerebral substance; they approach, by degrees, and having met, advance in the form of a single nervous trunk. In man, the nerve parts from the anterior lobe at the place where the con- volutions commence, and runs along the cleft formed between the innermost of the anterior and inferior of these, accompanied throughout its whole course by a very distinct streak of cineritious sub- stance. Immediately above the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone, it encounters a considerable 92 quantity of very soft gray substance, with which it forms a sort of bulb. (PL vi. fig. 1, 23.) The nerve, here, gains a mighty increase in size, and passing by numerous filaments through the cribri- form plate of the ethmoid bone, it is lost upon the lining membrane of the nose. The olfactory nerve of monkies (pi. v. fig. 3,) and of seals, resembles that of man very nearly. In the class mammalia, generally (pi. iv. fig. 2) a great many nervous fibres may be seen arising from the anterior part of the middle cerebral lobe: these join the fibres that spring from the inferior surface of the anterior convolutions, and compose a broad and rounded band, which, remaining attached to the anterior lobe, runs slightly inwards until it ar- rives at the ethmoid bone, when precisely, as in man, it meets a large mass of gray substance (pi. iv. fig. 2, 3, and 4, 23,) and increases in size so much, that after its exit by the ethmoidal holes, it suffices to cover the entire surfaces of the large superior spongy bones. • The bulb that is formed over the ethmoid bone, if it be incised, or have a piece taken out, will be found to be hollow. Its internal white layer in the lower animals communicates immediately with the anterior cavity of the brain, so that by blowing into the bulb of the olfactory nerve, the air will penetrate and inflate the lateral ventricles Soem- merring says, that the olfactory nerve of the human embryo, at an early period, is also hollow and that air blown into it reaches the cavities of the brain, The same experiment will, occasionally 93 though very rarely, succeed in the adult. When we observe the olfactory nerve so very large in the mammalia, whilst the mass of anterior convolutions is but inconsiderable, and, on the other hand, the nerve in the human kind so small and surrounded by the thick masses of the anterior lobe, we may conceive why the experiment should succeed so readily in animals, and be so rarely practicable in man. The olfactory nerve, it maybe almost unneces- sary to state, is proportionate in size to the ex- tent of external apparatus over which it is dis- tributed. In birds, the nerve of smell is detached from the anterior and inner part of the front lobe, but its fibres are distinct from those of the brain; one of its bands, too, which may be compared with the external root of the same nerve in man, runs to- wards the fissure of Sylvius and the middle lobe. This band, however, is not equally apparent in all birds. It is still less distinct in reptiles. In fishes, the olfactory nerve arises by two very distinct roots; certain fibres of great delicacy bring it into communication with the foremost cerebral ganglions, and others of a firmer texture and whiter color connect it with the longitudinal band, which, in these animals, lies in the middle line between the various ganglions, and communi- cates with the medulla oblongata. (PL ii. fig. 9 and 13.) Some anatomists have considered the entire mass of the anterior cerebral ganglion in the skate (pi. 94 ii. fig. 3, 1, 2, 3,) and all the three pairs of gangli- ons in the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1, 1, 2, 3,) as destined to originate the olfactory nerve. But the error here committed is proclaimed by the fact of the origins being always in proportion to the nerves them- selves, and there being none whatever between the masses mentioned and the olfactory nerves of fish- es. In regard to these ganglions, therefore, that which has been stated respecting those of vision must be repeated; the olfactory nerve is in com- munication with cerebral parts, destined to affec- tive and intellectual functions. 95 Section IV. Oj the best Method of dissecting the Brain. Before Dr. Gall and I began our researches on the structure of the brain, anatomists in their dis- sections and descriptions, had no other object in view than to know the forms of the whole, or of its particular masses, the color, connexion, and consistency, of its individual parts. To attain their end, they were in the habit of cutting down the brain by slices, and examining and noting the ap- pearances presented by each in succession, until they arrived at the base. ' The most accredited method,' says M. Cuvier,* ' of the schools, and usually recommended in books of anatomy, is to take away successive slices of the organ (the brain,) and to remark the appearances offered by each. This is the easiest in practice for the demon- stration, but it is the most difficult for the imagina- tion. The true relations of parts, which are al- ways seen cut across, escape not the pupil alone, but the master himself.' Willis t was the first who objected to the prac- * Rapport des Commissaires de l'Institute de France, on our Memoir entitled ' Recherches sur le Systeme Nerveux en General, et sur le Cerveau en Particulier.' t Cerebri Anatomia. 96 tice of considering, as distinct parts, all the forms accidentally produced by such a mode of dissection. He himself viewed the cerebral parts in their con- nexions. He says, too, that the anatomical demon- stration of the brain should be begun at the basis: by basis, however, let me observe, he understood the striated bodies and thalami. Ascending from these to the superior parts, and returning on the inferior in succession, his attention was confined to the larger masses, which he designates by names that indicate physical qualities only. ' From the striated bodies,' for example, he says, ' the legs of the medulla oblongata are prolonged: remaining apart for a short way, they then approach and get blended into a common stalk, composed, as it were, of two peduncles or stems.' As the Committee of the French Institute have conceived themselves authorized to assimilate the method of dissecting the brain, described in our Memoir, with the plan pursued by Varolius and Vieussens, and have interpreted these authors in a way which their language will not bear, I hope it will not be found amiss, if I extract a few passages from our Memoir, following them by some observa- tions on the report. ' The brain,' say the Committee, ' is attacked from below; the medulla oblongata is pursued across the bridge of Varolius, through the thalami of the optic nerves, and the striated bodies, when its fibres expand and compose the hemispheres. The hemispheres, also, if we choose, may, by tear- ing their lateral attachments to the crura cerebri, 97 be unfolded, the medulla oblongata and cerebellum be split longitudinally, and each half of the former shown as a sort of stem implanted into the hem- isphere of its own side, like the stalk of a mushroom into its cap.' They add, ' It is probable that this method would have had more vogue, were it not expressed in a rude drawing by Varolius, and had not the work of Vieussens remained, it would be difficult to say wherefore, in a sort of discredit, which it by no means deserves.' In reply to this, we cite the following passages from the works of the two authors mentioned above * ' The generality of anatomists,' says Va- rolius, ' think the spinal marrow begins at the oc- cipital hole only: I can bring proof to the contrary. It arises on the one hand below the ventricles of the brain, and on the other from the middle and inferior part of the cerebral basis. In the same manner as the brain, from out its substance, first produces that considerable trunk, the spinal mar- row, from which the ocular nerves soon arise; in the same manner the cerebellum pushes from itself a considerable process, which I call bridge of the cerebellum, out of which the auditory nerves then issue ' t ' That the sense of touch may inhere in all parts, and that all parts may convey images of objects, cognized by touch, to the primary senso- rium, there are four roots issuing from the brain * Constanta Varolii Anatomic*, sive de Resolution Corporis HumanTLibri Quatuor. Frankforti, 1591; and V.eussemus, Neu- rographia Universalis. t Varolius, p. 26. T 13 98 and cerebellum to form a considerable trunk, the spinal marrow, from which nerves are sent off and distributed to every part of the body.' * ' For my part, seeing that there were several cerebral organs situated about the base of the head, and that the brain by its weight (especially in the dead) com- pressed these against the skull, I deemed the ordi- nary mode of dissection liable to many inconve- niences. This is the reason why I am in the habit of commencing the dissection at the opposite part of the head; that is to say, at the basis of the brain, and by so doing, each of its organs is so clearly exhibited, that it seems as if nothing further could be desired. This method, however, which differs from the usual one, is also very difficult.' t Let any one read Varolius's letter to Mercurialis, and he will be convinced, from his entire descrip- tion, that he confined himself to the various forms and appearances visible in the brain and cerebel- lum ; that he did no more than attack the brain mechanically, turning and returning it without order or method, going from the cerebellum to the optic and olfactory nerves, and from the optic and olfactory nerves coming back to the cerebellum. Let me add, that Varolius himself says, that the mass he calls spinal marrow is comprised between the annular protuberance and the cerebral hemis- pheres; and further, that he continually speaks of the spinal cord as a production of the brain What the Committee of the French Institute make Varo- * Varolius, p. 36. f Ibid. p. HO. 99 lius say, consequently, is not to be found in his works. Let us now turn to and review the method of dissection practised by Vieussens, which, accord- ing to the Committee of the French Institute, is ' the same that Varolius employed, but with greater order and detail? In his first thirteen plates he shows nothing but sections of the brain from above downwards. He begins his demonstration with the convex part of the hemispheres, and then passes to the corpus callosum. The better to expose this, he cuts off the entire superimposed hemispheres by a horizontal sweep, and then, by properly trimming the mass that is left, he forms, what he calls, the centrum orale, in which he concentrates all the medullary fibres that, according to him, arise from the cortical substance of the brain, and from which he makes nervous fibres descend to every part of the body. He then passes on to the transparent partition (valvula Vieussenii,) that has since gone by his name, to the fornix, to the choroid plexus, to the nates, to the testes, &c. &c. ' We have,' he says, in one place expressly, ' ex- plained in a clear and complete manner, all that concerns the superior part of the brain and medulla oblongata ; we have also examined the cerebellum externally and internally; we have only then, fol- lowing the order of dissection which we have adopted, to examine what is found without and within the basis of the brain, properly so called, and the spinal marrow.' ' After the exact explanation,' he proceeds,' of 100 all that is to be seen on the upper part of the brain and spinal marrow, or that belongs to the cerebel- lum, in order to find with ease, and to describe with care that which is found at the basis of the brain, properly so called, and of the spinal marrow, we take away the cerebellum by cutting its peduncles transversely, and turn over the brain, freed from its convex parts by partial sections, and then we show the divided trunks of the anterior arteries of the base of the brain — the ten pairs of nerves -^- the infundibulum — the two white prominences sit- uated near the infundibulum — the two processes of the cerebellum towards the medulla oblongata, which run into the major annular protuberance of Willis — the pyramidal bodies — the olivary bodies —and the spinal nerves which join the par vagum.' It was always from his oval centre that Vieus- sens began his sections and descriptions of the brain. His principles, indeed, did not permit him to follow any other method of demonstration. In our answer to the Committee of the Institute, Dr. Gall and I have gone more deeply into details; but the passages cited above will suffice to show that the methods of Varolius and Vieussens are di- rectly opposed to our manner of dissecting and considering the brain and its parts. Vieussens, in deriving all the nervous fibres from his oval centre, proves himself to have had no idea of the successive reinforcement of the cerebral masses. I may, in- deed, say generally, that an examination of all'the anatomical works published before our time, and an inquiry into the various modes in which the 101 brain has been dissected, whether in public or in private schools, will not fail to convince every candid mind that there is not even a hint at the anatomico-physiological views which we have giv- en to the world. These views some modern anat- omists have adopted, but we still advance our claim of right to be considered as the discoverers and introducers of a new method of dissecting the brain — as the first demonstrators of the anatomy of its masses in harmony with their physiology. What, then, is our mode of investigating the structure of the various cerebral masses ? I have already shown (in the preceding section,) that we consider the nerves commonly entitled cerebral, as independent of each other, and that we regard the masses of the cerebellum, and brain, properly so called, as added to the nerves of the five senses and of voluntary motion. This point of doctrine estab- lished, we view the brain not as an unit or single organ, but as an assemblage of particular appara- tuses destined to special and determinate functions, after the manner of the nerves of the external senses. To this it may be said, that several anatomists have spoken of many peculiar parts; that they have even designated these by appropriate names; conse- quently, that our ideas on the plurality of apparatus are not new. There is no doubt whatever, but that all anat- omists have recognised distinct parts in the brain, and given them names according to their physical qualities. They have found hemispheres, convolu- 102 tions, cavities, striated bodies, pea-shaped bodies, stalks or legs of the brain and cerebellum, writing pens, rams'-horns, semicircular tape-worms, pyr- amidal and olive-like bodies, &c. &c. Now we, in showing that the individual masses, so named, do by no means constitute special apparatuses, per- forming peculiar functions, differ from all the anat- omists who have gone before us. We were, also, the first to prove the relative proportions that exist between several of the cerebral masses, and to ex- amine them in their mutual relations. If I continue to make use of the mechanical nomenclature, to speak of parts in particular, which can no longer be considered as special apparatuses, it is only for the sake of being more readily understood. My connected description will show what masses I look upon as peculiar organs. Our physiological views do not, it must be ev- ident, allow us to go on cutting the brain into slices: this procedure, indeed, ought rather to be entitled a destruction, than an anatomical demon- stration of the cerebral structure; it is precisely as though one should pretend to dissect a leg or an arm, by slicing down those members transversely, or to show the structure of the thoracic and ab- dominal viscera, by treating the trunk in a similar manner, and giving names to the appearances ex- posed after each successive slice. We commence our dissection at the place where the proper cere- bral masses are added to the nervous parts already described; we trace them in their continuations, and in their mutual connexions, and in the connex- 103 ions they maintain with the nerves of the five sen- ses and of voluntary motion; in short, we proceed in the dissection of the brain in a manner precisely analogous to that which is followed in the anatom- ical demonstration of the other parts of the body. Besides the above general anatomical principle as regards procedure, it is important to know that on account of their extremely delicate organiza- tion, the structure of several cerebral parts may be more easily and clearly exposed by means of scraping than by cutting. This is the reason why I frequently prefer the handle to the blade of the scalpel, for removing parts that cover those whose course I would show, — for instance, the passage of the pyramidal bodies across the annular pro- tuberance—the continuation of the anterior com- missure through the striated bodies into the middle lobes of the brain, and of the anterior pillars of the fornix, onwards to the mammillary bodies and in- terior of the thalami. The brain should be removed from the cranium, care being taken not to tear the crura at the supe- rior edge of the annular protuberance, (an accident that is very apt to occur,) nor to injure the medulla oblongata at the lower edge of the same part, and to cut the spinal mass so low down as to obtain, besides the entire medulla oblongata, the upper part of the true spinal cord. The brain thus freed from the skull, is then to be put into a plate, with the basis uppermost. The cerebellum and medulla oblongata having lost the support of the bone, now fall backwards. (PL vi. fig. 1.) In this position, 104 all the appearances presented by the base of the brain are visible. Having considered the cranial nerves in the manner described in the preceding section, the structure of the true cerebral masses is to be examined, commencing with that of the cere- bellum. As I treat of the several parts, I shall always indicate the procedure that appears to me the most convenient for exhibiting their anatomy.* * The method of dissecting the brain which M. Laurencet of Lyons proposes, seems founded on imaginary notions, rather than on the observance of nature and fact. According to him, the ner- vous system is like two trees reversed, the branches of the one be- ing continuous with the roots of the other, or after the manner of the sanguiferous system. The spinal cord, he says, consists of four bundles, which, in the medulla oblongata, are the anterior and posterior pyramids. The anterior, after their decussation, he sup- poses to continue across the pons Varolii, the crura cerebri and corpora striata, towards the corpus callosum and the convolutions, from thence to the fornix, to the thalami, corpora quadrigemina, cerebellum and posterior pyramids. The number of nervous fibres is assumed to be every where the same ; there are only bulgings and contractions in succession. M. Laurencet cuts the parts, and then tells us how they are formed. 105 • Section V. Of the Cerebellum. To avoid all risk of confusion, I repeat once more that I separate the nervous mass of the'spine and the cranial nerves from the brain, and confine this last appellation to the entire nervous mass, added to the nerves of the external senses and of volun- tary motion. I also repeat that the first anatom- ical principle of the nervous system, generally, ap- plies to the brain in particular; that is to say, this mass is not a simple unit, but a collection of many peculiar instruments. As this proposition is of great importance, I shall examine it here at some length. It is to be established by anatomy, physiology, and pathology. The physiological and pathological proofs of its truth, are contained in the second sec- tion of my Work on Phrenology, wherein I treat of the plurality of the organs. In this place, conse- quently, I shall confine myself to illustrative ana- tomical considerations. That the cerebral parts are more or less numer- ous in different tribes of animals, is a fact which cannot be gainsayed. Many writers, among others, Dr. Gall,* say that the faculties of animals arc multiplied in proportion as their brains are complka- * Anat. et. Phys. du Cerveau, t. iii. p. 364. 11 106 ted. Were this remark universally correct, it would serve as a positive proof of the brain's being an as- semblage of organs. But, without reckoning the difficulty, not to say the impossibility of determin- ing, anatomically, even in birds and mammiferous animals, the constituent parts of the brain, and ad- mitting that as true which mechanical anatomy demonstrates, viz.—that the brain is made up of a greater or smaller number of bundles, it must still be observed that each particular bundle cannot, legitimately, be assumed as composing a peculiar organ. There are several cerebral masses which, although more or less compound, do not, therefore, cease to be mere units. Take the cerebellum as an example. This is extremely simple in fishes, and very complicated in man; nevertheless, it is but a single instrument in both. The same law applies to several other cerebral parts, which, although exceedingly complex, only compose the instrument of a single function. Thus the first fact showing the structure to be more or less complicated, is no satisfactory or conclusive evidence as to the plural- ity of the cerebral organs; — this induction is still only problematical. Dr. Gall derives another anatomical proof of the principle under discussion, from the analogy that subsists between the organization of the brain and that of the other nervous systems. This analogy, however, is very limited. The spinal cord affords no example of it. Although composed of many parts, or numerous pairs of nerves, its functions are but repetitions of two of different kinds, viz. sen- 107 sation and motion. But the particular organs of the brain must be as distinct as the acoustic, optic, and olfactory nerves. A better anatomical illustration follows from the fact of all parts of the brain not being developed simultaneously, and of their volumes severally bearing no regular proportion to each other. The size of the cerebellum, for instance, is not in any direct ratio to that of the brain, neither are the three lobes of the cerebral hemispheres proportion- ate to each other. The same law applies in regard to all the individual parts of these lobes. These proofs, founded on the non-simultaneous development and disproportionate volume of the in- dividual portions of the encephalon, are strength- ened by facts, that show that the cerebral parts may severally be wanting. M. Jadelot was so kind as to show Dr. Gall and me an hydrocephalic child in the Hopital des Enfants Malades, at Paris, many of the superior convolutions of both hemis- pheres of whose brain were wanting, so that there was a hole communicating on each side with the lateral ventricles. The edges of these holes were smooth, and all the appearances bespoke a congen- ate or primary defect of organization. The brain, of which a drawing is given in pi. v. fig. 5, and 6, belonged to a girl, who died, aged seventeen, and was idiotic from birth. She died in the asylum at Cork. Dr. Abell, of that town, and Dr. Cheyne, of Dublin, had the goodness to send me the natural skull, and casts, in plaster, of the brain and bust. A comparison of this brain with one of a healthy 108 and well-constituted individual (pi. vi. fig. 1 and 2,) will show its anterior lobes to be exceedingly deficient, and the convolutions that commonly exist in the upper region of the forehead to be wanting altogether. It is even less complicated, and more poorly developed, especially anteriorly, than the brain of the ourang-outang, (PL v. fig. 3 and 4) Mr. Stanley, of London, preserves a sim- ilar idiotic brain in spirits. The anatomical evidence, from the want of pro- portion among, and the non-simultaneous develop- ment and entire absence of, the cerebral parts, al- though plausible, is not, however, decisive in prov- ing the plurality of the organs. The testimony these facts supply only becomes conclusive wThen they are united to physiology. Alone, they do not prove that the functions of the parts, whose devel- opment is neither simultaneous nor proportionate, and which, individually, may be wanting, are dis- similar.— The branches of a tree shoot in succes- sion, yet all bear the same fruit, But those cere- bral parts are indubitably the same, however dis- similar in physical appearance, if like functions appear with their presence, increase with their growth, are vigorous in proportion to their masses, and wanting with their absence; Cerebral parts, on the other hand, differ if like functions do not appear with their development, be not manifested in vigor corresponding to their volume, and exist or not independently of their presence or absence. Certain it is, therefore, that how important soever U may be to classify the cerebral organs, anatomy 109 alone would never enable us to attain such a con- summation. The aid of physiology is indispensably requisite. Now, Dr. Gall and I claim the merit of having been the first to compare the relations be- tween the development of different cerebral parts and peculiar functions; and our physiological anat- omy of the brain proves, that the parts indicated in books of descriptive anatomy as distinct masses, such, for instance, as those styled pons or bridge, pyramidal and olive-like bodies, thalami, mammil- lary bodies, callous or hard body, &c. &c, do not constitute particular organs. Some anatomists have expressed doubts as to the possibility of proving the presence or absence of individual parts in the human brain, especially in the hemispheres and their convolutions; because, say they, the physical appearance of these is not invariable. But, provided essentials be not con- founded with modifications, the very reverse of the above assumption may easily be proved. The lobes are always to be distinguished from one another, and certain convolutions from others, with the same certainty as the annular protuberance is to be discriminated from the crura of the brain, the quadrigeminal from the mammillary, and the pyramidal from the restiform or olive-like bodies. The general form and direction of the convolutions, even of the human brain in its complication, are, in fact, remarkably regular. Thus, the transverse convolutions of the superior lateral and middle parts of the hemispheres are never found running in any other direction; never longitudinally, for ex- 110 ample. Those that lie longitudinally again, as they do under the squamous suture, behind the temporal bone, and on either side of the olfactory nerve, are never met with disposed transversely. ' Show Gall,' says Dr. Rudolphi,* ' the organs of theft, of murder, and of the religious sentiment sep- arated from the cerebral mass, and be sure he would not know them.' Dr. Gall, t in his reply to this, contents himself with saying, ' Show M. Rudolphi morsels of the spinal marrow or medulla oblongata, and be sure he would not know them; yet are the spinal marrow and medulla oblongata proved to be aggregates of different nerves.' This answer of Dr. Gall does not satisfy me. In the first place, I have shown the brain to be composed of many parts, whose functions are essentially dif- ferent, whilst all the portions of the spinal cord have similar offices. Dr. Gall's reply, therefore, is simply evasive. But, for my part, I will accept M. Rudolphi's proposition directly; for I maintain that he who has studied the forms of the peripheral expansions of the cerebral organs, will always be able to distinguish, in man, the organ of acquisi- tiveness from that of destructiveness, and that of veneration from either, (the organs of theft, murder, and the sentiment of religion, in M. Rudolphi's nomenclature,) as easily as an ordinary observer will the olfactory from the optic nerve. I am ready at any time, personally, to verify the above statement. * Physiologie, ii. Band, Berlin, 1823. t Sur les Fonctions du Cerveau, torn. vi. p. 138. Ill However, I still admit, that the convolutions forming parts of any particular apparatus, present many modifications in reference to size and number of anfractuosities. Such modifications occur, not only in the brains of different individuals, but even in the two hemispheres of the same brain. Varie- ty, however, need not be confounded with essential configuration. I have remarked that the organs which are best nourished, and most largely de- veloped, have generally the smallest number of an- fractuosities. The importance of classing the cerebral organs is, as I have said, evident. Researches to this end interest the anatomist, the physiologist, and, above all, the practical physician, on account of the brain's influence on the vegetative functions, on the origin, duration, character, and cure of a long list of diseases, especially of such as depend on moral causes. To render such inquiries complete, they must be extended into comparative anatomy, always in the view of proving the brain to be an assemblage of organs, destined to dissimilar func- tions, mutually in relation, severally, liable to dis- ease, and likewise to reciprocal derangement. To compare the spinal cord, and even the cranial nerves, in the different classes of vertebral animals, is by no means a very arduous task. But to do the like in regard to the brain is one of extreme difficulty. Anatomists commonly set out with the human brain in their eye as a type of comparison; this they consider as an unit having two hemis- pheres, a corpus callosum, an anterior, middle, and 112 posterior commissure, an infundibulum, fornix, feet of a hippocampus, mammillary bodies, striated bodies, optic thalami, a semicircular tenia or tape- worm, quadrigeminal bodies, crura or legs, a valve of Vieussens, a pons, bridge, or annular protuber- ance, a cerebellum, having also crura or legs, a medulla oblongata, pyramidal, olivary, and resti- form bodies, four ventricles, an aqueduct of Sylvius, and several other parts; and then they proceed to recognise or deny the existence of parts according to mere physical appearances, especially forms and situations. Such a mode of proceeding, however, is quite in- adequate to establish a system of comparative cere- bral anatomy. The brain, in the first place, is not an unit, but an assemblage of particular appara- tuses, that require severally to be specified; and again, it is certain that the isolated masses, such as they are described in books of anatomy, do not constitute peculiar organs, but that several of them, indeed, sometimes go to the formation of one. The cerebellum is found, either with or without an an- nular protuberance; the anterior pyramidal bodies are not one organ, but consist of parts of many; they are, in fact, the rudiments of the organs of the intellectual faculties. And, further, the physical appearance of apparatuses which are known to have analogous functions, is often exceedingly dif- ferent. Without knowing the functions of the cerebral parts, it is, therefore, impossible to de- monstrate either their identity or their difference. What anatomist, unacquainted with the function 113 of smell, would ever have concluded that the olfac- tory nerves of the skate, flounder, chicken, seal, ox, and man, were analogous masses ? Certainly, sim- ilarity of appearance would never have led to the conclusion ; and differences in appearance among the other cerebral masses, are not less remarkable than in the olfactory nerve throughout the four classes of vertebral animals. Thus, I conceive that the comparative anatomy of the brain can neither be itself advanced, nor afford satisfactory conclu- sions without the aid of physiology. The common and objectionable manner of ex- amining the structure of the encephalon, led anato- mists to conclude thatthey had discovered the same cerebral masses in the mammalia as in man, and, because the forms of the parts were different, to deny their existence in birds and the inferior classes of animals. In all the lower tribes of creation, they supposed another and a different composition of the encephalon. I claim the merit of having been the first to maintain that the analogy or dif- ference of the cerebral parts, in different classes, ought to be determined by the combined aid of anatomy and physiology. I have no hesitation in saying that the relations of the cerebral masses, as they are indicated by M. Serres in his prize memoir, are very far from being satisfactory replies to the question proposed by the Institute of France I also think it may not be out of place to state that Baron Cuvier, perpetual secretary to the physical class of the French Institute, in his analysis of the academy's labors for the year 1820, has mentioned 15 114 the name of Dr. Gall in reference to two points only, on which M. Serres differs with the Doctor, whilst he reports several other important facts and views, discovered by me, and published conjointlv with Dr. Gall, as forming part of the memoir of M. Serres; such, for instance, as the relation between the annular protuberance and the lateral parts of the cerebellum, and that between the corpus cal- losum and the cerebral hemispheres. In admitting as an anatomico-physiologlcal prin- ciple, that the particular organs of the cerebral functions bear no relative proportion, in the same way as the instruments of the external senses are in no direct ratio one to another, I deny generally, as I have already done particularly, in regard to the spinal cord, the proposition of M. Serres, ac- cording to which, ' the tubercula quadrigemina serve as a basis of determination to the other parts of the encephalon? * The quadrigeminal bodies do, in no- wise, bear any direct proportion to the anterior, middle, and posterior lobes of the brain. These masses exist independently of each other, and be- long to apparatuses that are totally distinct. The quadrigeminal tubercles are absolutely smaller in man and the dog, than in the horse and ox; but the brain of the animals last mentioned is larger than that of the dog, and smaller than that of man. Each special apparatus must be determined by it- * Les tubercules quadrigeminaux servent de base de determi- nation aux autres parties de l'Enoephale,' Rapport, &c, p. 67 ; and Serres's Anatomie, &c. vol. 1. Preface. 115 self, and by comparison with its particular func- tion. In many fishes there are portions of gray ner- vous substance to be observed, forming ganglions on the lateral edges of the medulla oblongata. (PL ii. fig. 3, 5, 12, 13, e.) By removing the cerebellum, a ganglion may, in many species, be seen in the middle line (pi. ii. fig. 13, 43;) this ganglion is, some- times, naturally exposed. (PL ii. fig. 5. 43.) All these masses pertain to the nerves that issue from them. The nervous mass immediately succeeding these ganglia, is the first cerebral apparatus; it is distin- guished by the name of cerebellum, and exists in all vertebral animals lying on the dorsal surface of the medulla oblongata. Its name is derived from the anatomy of man, in whom the corresponding mass is smaller than the brain, properly so called. This example, among many others that might be cited, proves how defective that nomenclature of the encephalon is, which is founded on the physical qualities of the human brain. Is it not astonishing that anatomists, who deny to fishes a cereberum or brain, should still acknowledge them possessed of a cerebellum or little brain? Let me, therefore, repeat, that a good nomenclature should have for its basis the nature of the functions performed by the particular instruments. My only reasons, as I have said already, for continuing to use the old and faulty names, is to make myself understood more readily. The cerebellum or organ of amativeness, is com- 116 plicated in different degrees in the four classes of vertebral animals; its form and its volume vary exceedingly, but it regularly communicates poste- riorly with the nervous mass of the spine on either side, and anteriorly with the bigeminal or quadri- geminal bodies. * To acquire accurate notions of the cerebellum in the superior classes,' says M. Cuvier, ' according to M. Serres, they must, in the first instance, be taken from fishes. In these animals, this organ is formed of two very distinct parts — a middle lobule deriving its roots from the ventricle of the quadri- geminal tubercles, and two lateral portions coming from the restiform bodies; the two parts are insu- lated and disjunct in the whole of the class pisces, a circumstance that has occasioned them to be mistaken.' * Now this fact is not, by any means, so general as M. Serres would have us suppose. What are his reasons, let me ask, in the first place, for class- ing the ganglion I have just pointed out as situated in the fourth ventricle (pi. ii. fig. 5 and 13, 43,) along with the cerebellum ? — He can have none. And again, in the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1.)—in the cod * 'Pour avoir des notions exactes sur les cervelets des clashes su- peneures d'apres le Memoire de M. Serres, il faut d'abord les em- prunterauxpoissons.-Chez les poissons cet organe est forme de deux pames tres distinctes: d'un lobule median, prenant ses racines dans le ventncule des tubercules quadrigeminaux, et de feuillets aterauxprovenantdu corps restiforme: les deux parties sont Iso- ldes, disjomtes dans toute la classe des poissons ce qui les avait fait mtconnzitTe'-Eappon des Travaux de la Classe Physique pendant VAnnie 1820, p. 67. *y**?we 117 (pi. ii. fig. 2) — in the flounder (pi. ii. fig. 7) — in the roach (ib. fig. 11,) and many others, the surface of the cerebellum is quite smooth. In the skate, on the contrary, it is evidently composed of several ramifications. (PL ii. fig. 3.) In fishes, generally, there is a bundle which, with its fellow of the opposite side, forms a hollow mass stretched above the fourth ventricle, and covered on its peripheral surface with gray substance. It is, for the most part, loose at one extremity only; it has either a pointed, a rounded, or a flattened form, and is most commonly turned towards the back. In the skate, however, it has two loose extremities, the one pointing forwards, the other backwards. Reptiles, in general, have a cerebellum, composed of two parts, which are hollow, superficially smooth, and in communication with each side of the medulla oblongata. In some of the class, it is extremely small, as in the toad and frog (pi. iii. fig. 2,) which, in this respect, resemble the sturgeon among fishes: in other reptiles, however, and of the number is the crocodile, the cerebellum, as in the skate and shark, is furrowed on the surface, and bears marks of a more complicated structure. The ramified and lamellar structure of the cere- bellum, (pi. iii. fig. 5, 6V 7, 9, 10, and 11,) and its division into median and lateral portions, become very evident and regular in birds and mammiferous animals. In birds, (pi. iii. fig. 5, 7, 10, and 11,) the middle or primary portion is large, compared with the lateral parts. These, indeed, are scarcely 118 evolved in the class aves; they, however, become ever more remarkable in size and number of ram- ifications, in proportion as we mount from the low- est up to the highest of the mammiferous tribes, and reach man. In the foetus, the cerebellum, even of those animals in which it afterwards presents nu- merous convolutions, is always quite smooth. The primary portion of the cerebellum, in all birds, is divided into several branches ;- the middle and up- per one of these is the most remarkable. Below and in front of the fourth ventricle, in the same class, there occurs a little lobule, the volume of which, like that of the lateral parts, is much aug- mented in the mammalia. These peculiarities of structure prove that oviparous animals, in general, cannot be said to have a simple cerebellum. The cerebellum of fishes, of reptiles, and of birds (pi. xi. fig. 3, 62,) is constantly hollow. Its cavity communicates with the fourth ventricle (ib. m,) or space between the cerebellum and medulla oblon- gata. The cerebellum of the mammalia is, on the other hand, invariably solid; the fourth ventricle is the only cavity in its vicinity. (PL xi. fig. 1, m.) The anatomical principle laid down in regard to the regularity of proportion, between the cineri- tious and white substances, and to the occurrence of cineritious matter at the origins of nervous mass- es, is confirmed by the structure of the cerebellum. At the place of this organ's attachment to the medulla oblongata, there is always an accumula- tion of pulpy substance to a greater or smaller amount; that is to say, the quantity is great, or 119 small, relatively to the volume of the entire cere- bellum. In man it composes an irregularly-shaped mass, toothed or serrated around the edges. This collection of cineritious intermixed with white matter, is described in books of anatomy under va- rious names, such as corpus rhomboideum, corpus dentatum zig-zag, and kernel of the cerebellum. I speak of it under the title of ganglion of the cerebellum. Vicq d'Azyr believed that the ganglion of the cerebellum was only to be found in the human kind. But in 1808, conjointly with Dr. Gall, I showed that it exists in the mammalia generally; * and I now add, that it is very distinct in birds. Whenever the cerebellum is somewhat considera- ble, it may always be demonstrated. Its small size and pale color were probably the causes of its existence being overlooked. But it is matter of prime importance not to confound the forms as- sumed, and the lighter or deeper shades of color possessed by the pulpy substance, with its necessa- ry existence. The quantity of cineritious substance at the ori- gin of the cerebellum is in the direct ratio of the entire mass, and not merely of the lateral parts of that organ. It is very conspicuous in birds, although the lateral parts of their cerebella be but rudi- mentary. In man, too, a portion of the ganglion may easily be demonstrated running towards the vermiform process (the primary part) of the cere- bellum. * Vid. Memoire sur l'Anatomie du Cerveau, &c. 120 From what I have said, it follows, that this gang- lion is not situated in the middle of the cerebellum, but commences where this mass is connected with the medulla oblongata. Vicq d'Azyr commits a capital error in his plate xxxi. fig. 20, in represent- ing the ganglion of the cerebellum as placed so much externally, so near to the anterior edge of the organ to which it belongs, and so far distant from the medulla oblongata. In forming the cerebellum, nature has, in all an- imals, pursued the same plan. Two bundles con- stantly bring it into connexion with the two sides of the medulla oblongata: these are of variable size; they meet a greater or smaller quantity of gray substance, and proceed strengthened in pro- portion to the quantity of this substance encounter- ed ; they then regularly compose a primary portion, which in the lowest tribes is smooth superficially, but which, as the scale is mounted in, appears fur- rowed transversely, or divided into lamella?, and becomes complicated by the addition of lateral masses, laminated in like manner. This lamellar structure is exposed by a vertical cut through the cerebellum; it bears the name of arbor vita?, on account of its supposed resemblance to the foliage of the thuya, or tree of life. (PL viii. and ix. fig. 1 and 2.) Although the human cerebellum be very com- plicated, the elements of a precisely similar forma- tion to that of the lower animals, may without diffi- culty be traced. One slip from the ganglion forms, with its fellow of the opposite side, the vermiform 121 process, or primary portion, which, by a cut carried through the micldle line, may be seen afterwards dividing commonly into seven principal parts. (PL vii. fig. 2, and pi. x. fig. 1, and pi. xi. fig. 1, A.) These divide into branches, and these again into leafets, differing in each case in number, length, and volume generally. The other slips or bundles that issue from the ganglion proceed backwards, upwards, downwards, and outwards, and expand into layers, which are disposed horizontally. Those that come from the middle of the ganglion are the longest; the others are successively shorter, as they issue nearer to the commencement of the ganglion. (PL viii. fig. 1, pi. ix. 1 and 2, and pi. xi. fig. 1, B.) The peripheral extremities of the bundles thus disposed in lamella?, are, as well as when they ex- pand arid form an uninterruptedly smooth surface, covered with a layer of cineritious substance. By a section carried vertically through the mid- dle of the ganglion in the direction of its bundles, eleven principal trunks or branches are commonly exposed. The nearer the cut runs to the middle, or posterior edges, of the cerebellar hemispheres, the greater will be the number of branches discovered, as their quantity will be fewer in proportion as it passes nearer to the mesial line of the body. Two branches may frequently be seen intimately united near the base of the ganglion (pi. ix. fig. 2, and pi. xi.fig. 1, B.,) and forming, for some little way, a common stalk; but in other cases they proceed quite distinctly from the very beginning. Still 122 greater varieties are to be detected in the interior divisions and subdivisions, which are unimportant, and require no particular mention. The ganglion of the cerebellum varies consider- ably in form, and this, not only in different species, but even in different individuals of the same kind of animals. Its appearance also changes according to the mode in which it is cut; horizontally incised, it has the figure expressed in pi. viii. fig. 2, B. s.; obliquely, the form represented in pi. xi. fig. 1, B. s. ; and vertically, the shape given in pi. viii. fig. 1, and pi. ix. fig. 1 and 2 s. A horizontal cut shows it in its greatest extent. Although the portions or ramifications of the cerebellum be divided into parallel lamella?, they do not ultimately lie parallel with each other; each portion thus considered is placed obliquely. Nei- ther are the furrows on the outer surface parallel. (PL vi. fig. 1 and 2. PL viii. fig. 1,B.) Scemmerring has remarked, and exposed the error generally committed by anatomists in this matter in their drawings. It is also a mistake to suppose that the furrows of the cerebellum penetrate deeply into its substance. They only do so in the clefts that mark its principal divisions. In the mammiferous class of animals the cerebel- lum is augmented by the addition of a mass, known variously by the names pons Varolii, tuber annu- lare, or mesolobe. The transverse fibres of this part evidently belong to the hemispheres or lateral portions of the cerebellum, to which they bear a marked and regular proportion. (PL viii. fig. 2.) 123 No one can deny to Dr. Gall and me the merit of having first detected the relations that exist be- tween the annular protuberance and cerebellar hemispheres. M. Tiedemann^ says, that we err in deriving the transverse fibres of the annular protu- berance from the cineritious substance covering the leafets of the cerebellum, because these transverse fibres are visible before any division into lamella? can be recognised in the cerebellum. This reason- ing of M. Tiedemann, however, does not seem very conclusive, since the annular protuberance and cerebellum are developed at the same time. It is, on the other hand, at least certain that M. Tiede- mann is himself mistaken, in supposing the trans- verse fibres of the annular protuberance to come in part from the corpus dentatum (ganglion of the cerebellum.)! The annular protuberance lies most evidently anteriorly and quite externally to the ganglion of the cerebellum; its fibres, moreover, may easily be demonstrated, as commencing di- rectly from the lamella? of the hemispheres. (PL viii. fig. 2.) I, for my part, insist rather on the progressive aug- mentation of the cerebellum in the different classes of animals, than on the successive increase of its individual constituent parts. It appears certain, that the several parts of the cerebellum are not sim- ultaneously developed; that some of these are vis- ible at an earlier period than others. But it still remains a point of great difficulty to determine the * Anatomie du Cerveau dans le Foetus Humain, Nurnberg, 1816. t Op. cit. p. 107. 124 part which serves as the centre of departure or originator of the others. Are the various portions developed after the manner of the graft of a fruit- tree, or, are there branches thrown off in succes- sion, or, are the rudiments of the whole called into being at once, and maturity of growth then ac- quired by each in succession ? The first supposi- tion, I confess, appears the most probable to myself. It is not unreasonable also to presume, that the fibres of communication, and the fibres of the commis- sures, or apparatuses of union, appear together, whilst the peculiar apparatus which is to perform a special function acquires the form, volume, and organic condition necessary to this duty by degrees. Although the cerebellum be ever more complica- ted as we ascend in the scale of beings, it is a fact, that animals do not exhibit faculties in proportion as this mass presents a larger number of lamella?, or ramifications. Neither is it found that the intel- lectual and other general mental powers of individ- uals of the human kind increase, or are active in the ratio of the cerebellar lamella?, as was once maintained by Malacarne. Many authors have said, that the cerebellum con- tained, in proportion to its bulk, more cineritious substance than the brain; and, as a consequence of this notion, it came to be regarded as softer than the brain. Both of these assumptions are incorrect. The primary mistake was probably committed from vertical sections having always been employed to expose the internal structure, i. e., the arbor vita? of the cerebellum, whilst horizontal slicing was the 125 method followed in regard to the brain. But let an opposite procedure be adopted; let the cerebel- lum be divided horizontally, and the brain verti- cally, and more cineritious substance certainly will then appear in the brain than in the cerebellum. The cerebral convolutions again, it is natural to think, will, on account of their mere thickness, seem firmer than the cerebellar leafets; but careful examination proves that in one case the brain, and in another the cerebellum, possesses the greatest degree of consistency. Age and fortuitous circum- stances, as disease, &c, exert a particular influ- ence in this respect. From what has been said when discussing the nerves of the external senses and voluntary motion, it may almost be unnecessary here to state, that none of them arises from the cerebellum. All the parts of the cerebellum are double, or exist in pairs: its primary portion or vermiform process, is no exception to the law; all its parts, however, are not necessarily symmetrical; the ele- ments composing each side are alike, but the size of the essential bundles, the length and thickness of their branches, the number of subdivisions of these, and the form and position of the lamella? vary extremely in different individuals; and often on both sides of the same cerebellum, the entire mass of one side is not unfrequently more largely developed than that of the other. In their descriptions of the cerebellum, anato- mists still make mention of two soft and thin lay- ers, the one of which unites the upper part of the 126 posterior pyramidal bodies with the lower portion of the vermiform process, (the primary or essential mass,) whilst the other brings the cerebellum into connexion with the quadrigeminal tubercles. The latter is commonly known by the name of the valve of Vieussens, or by the singular title of pro- cessus a cerebello ad testes. (PL xi. fig. 1 and 3. y.) Reil styles them the superior and inferior medulla- ry veils. The valve of Vieussens, or layer of communica- tion between the primary portion of the cerebellum and quadrigeminal tubercles, may be distinctly traced through all the four classes of vertebral an- imals. It increases in size in the ratio of the dis- tance and nervous masses, between the cerebellum and quadrigeminal tubercles. The inferior veil, on the contrary, is but just apparent in birds: in the mammalia, however, like the superior, it is conspic- uous, although varying in size in the different spe- cies composing the class. The idea of a determinate proportion between the brain and cerebellum is one still very generally entertained. Authors on the comparative anatomy of the brain have even drawn up tables of this sup- posed proportion. 'It is easy,' says M. Cuvier,* ' to ascertain the proportionate weight of the brain and cerebellum, because no variation in health, and no change in the bodily condition, as to obesi- ty or leanness, exert any influence on them.' But it is long since Dr. Gall and I showed, that the * Legon's d'Anatomie Comparee, vol. ii. p. 152. 127 cerebellum has no regular proportion to the brain. In the adult, a small cerebellum is often met con- joined with a very large brain ; and in other cases, the cerebellum occurs of great size, while the brain is particularly small. M. Chaussier has also ob- served, that the cerebellum composed at one time the sixth, at another the seventh, and at another, and more rarely, the tenth of the weight of the en- cephalon. In newly-born infants, he found that it was the thirteenth, the fourteenth, the seventeenth, and, in one instance, no more than the thirtieth part, by weight, of the encephalon. Whoever, in- deed, will be at the pains to compare the enceph- alon of children of two, four, six, and ten, and of young people up to their sixteenth year, will be convinced, that relatively to the brain the cerebel- lum is at these periods smaller than in adult age. If any exception to the rule be found, it must be regarded as an individual peculiarity of organiza- tion. Moreover, the cerebellum is generally larger in men than in women, and in males than in females of the same kind of animal. Let us now examine some points of M. Serres's Prize Memoir, in relation to the cerebellum. He says,* ' it may appear singular, that the cere- bellum" should not be formed till after the quadri- geminal tubercles; nevertheless, there is no ex- ception to this fact in any class.' Now, this fact will not appear singular, but seem perfectly natural to those who are acquainted with the functions of the apparatus that arises from the * Rapport de l'Institut, 1820, p. 68. 128 quadrigeminal bodies, and with the office of the cerebellum. As the optic nerve has to act long before the cerebellum, its organization requires to be complete long before that of the cerebellum needs to be perfect. Further, the brain is also de- veloped at a later period than the quadrigeminal bodies; the cerebellum consequently presents noth- ing whatever that is singular in this respect. ' The vermiform process,' says M. Serres in an- other place,* ' comes from the quadrigeminal tuber- cles, whilst the other part, issuing from the resti- form bodies, constitutes the hemispheres of the cerebellum. The two elements of the cerebellum, (the vermiform process and lateral parts,) also, though united, are still entirely independent of each other.' These propositions do not to me seem founded in nature. Were we to admit the bigeminal bodies of fishes and reptiles, and the quadrigeminal tuber- cles of the mammalia, as the source of the vermi- form process, or primary portion of the cerebellum, we should then expect some regular proportion in the development of these parts to each other. But M. Serres himself excepts reptiles. ' In all the classes, except that of reptiles,' he says,! £ the me- dian lobe of the cerebellum (processus vermiculaire superieur,) is developed in the direct ratio of the quadrigeminal bodies.' Now, this exception is of itself sufficient to refute M. Serres's entire assump- tion. However, let the cerebellum be compared with the optic tubercles in the sturgeon, in the eel * Rapport de l'Institut, 1820, p. 68. f Ibid. p. 68. 129 (pi. ii. fig. 1,) flounder (pi. ii. fig. 4,) skate (pi. ii. fig. 3,) barbel (pi. ii. fig. 12,) pigeon, turkey, dog, and other animals, to procure data, by which to esti- mate the utter erroneousness of M. Serres's opin- ions. On dissecting the cerebellum of mammiferous animals, the vermiform process, as well as the other ramifications, generally, will be seen communicat- ing distinctly with the dentated body, or ganglion of this organ. Lastly, and still further, to expose this error relative to the origin of the primary por- tion of the cerebellum, let us recur to pathological anatomy, as well to the cases produced accident- ally or by disease, as to those created intentionally or by mutilations. Atrophy of the optic nerves, caused in any way, is well known to extend to the anterior pair of quadrigeminal bodies; no one, how- ever, has yet imagined that it was continued on to the median lobe, or primary portion of the cerebel- lum. The optic tubercles and the vermiform pro- cess, therefore, exist independently of each other; they are connected, only that they may exert a mutual influence. It is not the same in regard to the median lobe and the lateral parts of the cerebellum. True it is, that these two masses are not necessarily propor- tionate to each other; this fact, however, does not prove each entirely independent of the other, as M Serres has presumed.* I take his own manner of viewing the matter in illustration; he regards the bio-eminal tubercles of birds, reptiles, and fishes, and the quadrigeminal bodies of the mammalia in * Rapport, &c. p. 66. 17 130 man, as essentially analogous masses; the quadri- geminal appearance in mammiferous tribes, he con- ceives to arise from a transverse furrow, which, in the human kind, commonly passes across the mid- dle of the mass, which, in the carnivora, runs more anteriorly, and in the ruminatia and rodentia, passes more posteriorly, and thus makes the two pairs of tubercles appear of nearly equal size, or causes the front or back pair to predominate.* The two pairs of tubercles, therefore, vary in pro- portionate size in the different tribes of mammife- rous animals, precisely as do the median and lat- eral lobes of the cerebellum. Now, if, according to M. Serres, the dissimilar development of the dif- ferent pairs of the quadrigeminal bodies does not prove their independence, I do not see that it is reasonable to regard diversity of development of the median and lateral lobes of the cerebellum as any proof of their independence. M. Serres thinks that the spinal cord, the median lobe of the cerebellum, and the quadrigeminal tubercles are developed in the direct ratio of each other, and the inverse ratio of the cerebellar hem- ispheres and annular protuberance. I have said that M. Serres himself excepts the class of reptiles from this law, and I have added oth- er facts which refute his opinion regarding the de- pendence of the median cerebellar lobe on the quad- rigeminal bodies. I have also shown above, that the direct ratio of development which M. Serres recognises between the spinal cord, and the quad- rigeminal tubercles has no foundation in nature. * Rapport, &c. p. 68. 131 An appeal to the same authority also proves that M. Serres deceives himself when he fancies the spi- nal cord, the median cerebellar lobe, and the quad- rigeminal tubercles to be developed inversely as the lateral parts of the cerebellum and annular protu- berance. For in fishes, reptiles, and birds, there are no cerebellar hemispheres, and no annular pro- tuberance. The lateral parts of the cerebellum and annular protuberance are, in fact, developed in a greater or less degree in different tribes, but never inversely as the optic tubercles and spinal cord are concerned. The flounder (pi. ii. fig. 7,) and carp (pi. ii. fig. 5,) have optic tubercles of greater size than the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1,) which of the three kinds, however, has the most voluminous spinal cord. M. Serres also notices several relations between the annular protuberance and different other cere- bral parts. Dr. Gall and I were the first who showed the development of the annular protuber- ance in the direct ratio of the cerebellar hemis- pheres. No other one of the relations, mentioned by M. Serres, is constant; and, consequently, no other can be laid down as a law. The annular protuber- ance, for instance, is not regularly developed in the inverse ratio of the cerebellar median lobe, of the quadrigeminal tubercles, and spinal cord, as M. Serres pretends* The annular protuberance in general is relatively less considerable in women than in men ; but the spinal cord is not developed in an inverse ratio in the two sexes. 1 shall expose another of M. Serres's errors in supposing the an- nular protuberance to bear a directly proportionate * Rapport, &c. p. 69. 132 development to the corpus callosum,* when I come to speak of the last-named cerebral mass. The advantage, it seems to me, would be little commensurate with the labor of ascertaining all the modifications presented by the cerebella of different animals, and giving titles to every lobule superadded to the primary median portion. Such inquiries ap- pear to me rather superfluous, as I view the entire cerebellum in the light of a single organ or appara- tus, and as performing only one species of function. I have still to add, that the cerebellum, like every other organ, is not only more or less complex in the different kinds of animals, but also that its several constituent parts are modified in the various individ- uals of the same species. The organ, too, I may here observe, is developed at rather a late period of life, a fact which I have already had occasion to cite, in showing that there was no irregularity of proportion between the brain and cerebellum: I, however, call my readers' attention to the phenom- enon, at present, in order that he may understand the tardy appearance of the function of the part (the sexual appetite.) For the sake of physiology, I also adduce in this place, the fact of the larger size, generally, of the cerebellum in men and males, than in women and females of the several kinds. The cerebellum, then, is an apparatus of a struc- ture more or less complicated in different species of animals, having a greater or smaller development in the two sexes, and in different individuals of the same kind, and being in a direct communication * Rapport, &c. p. 72. 133 with the medulla oblongata and quadrigeminal tubercles. To obtain as clear and comprehensive ideas of the position and external and internal structure of the cerebellum, as possible, I request my reader to turn to, and peruse the plates in the order as follows:— For differences of form, of volume, and of super- ficial structure, see pi. ii. figs, 1, 2, 5, 7, 10, 11, 12; pi. iii. figs. 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 10, and 11; pi. iv. figs. 1, 3, 5, and 6; pi. v. figs. 3, 4, 5, and 6; pi. vi. figs. 1 and 2. For the appearances and connexions in the median line, examine pi. viii. fig. 2. In oviparous animals, the cerebellum is hollow, as is seen in pi. xi. fig. 3, 62. In the mammalia and man it is solid; in them, there is nothing that can be likened to a cavity, except the separation between its primary portion and the medulla ob- longata, or rather the posterior part of the annular protuberance; see pi. vii. fig.2, m; and pi. xi. fig.l,m. The disposition of the cerebellar ramifications, from birds up to man, is lamellar, as it is presented in pi. iii. fig. 1; pi. viii. fig. 1 and 2; pi. ix. fig. 1 and 2; and pi. xi. figs. 1, 3. For the appearance and structure of the cerebel- lar commissure (annular protuberance,) inspect pi. iv. fig. 2; pi. v. figs. 3 and 5; pi. vi. fig. 11, 2; and pi. viii. figs. 1 and 2. The ganglion of the cerebellum (dentated body) is represented in pi. viii. figs. 1, 2; and pi. xi. fig. 1, B. s. To demonstrate the connexion of the cerebellum with a bundle of the restiform body, the medulla 134 oblongata must be pushed to one side, and the auditory nerve and a thin layer interposed between the medulla and the cerebellum, scraped off with the handle of the scalpel. The second bundle of the restiform body, reckoning from the posterior pyramid, will then be seen to plunge into the cere- bellum. By entering the point of the knife at the insertion of this bundle, and cutting the cerebellum vertically, so that about two thirds of its substance may be left externally, and the other third remain internally, the communication of the cerebellum with the medulla oblongata, its ganglion from the entrance of the connecting bundle of the restiform body to about its middle, the ramifications of the white substance, and the peripheral extremities of the various branches universally covered with cin- eritious matter, constituting the appearance denom- inated arbor vita?, and all will be exposed. These peculiarities are represented in pi. viii. fig. 1. To see these uniting fibres of the cerebellum, composing the annular protuberance or commis- sure, are distinct from those of the bundle that connects it with the medulla oblongata, the last named part must be turned aside, and the vocal, glosso-pharyngeal, facial, and auditory nerves re- moved with the handle of the scalpel; the fibres of union will now be seen gathering themselves from the peripheral parts, and lying over the bundle that springs from the medulla oblongata, and plunges into the cerebellar ganglion. Finally, eve- ry cut in the direction of the cerebellar lamella? exhibits a white surface, as is pictured in pi. viii. % 2. 135 Section VI. Of the Brain. In the preceding section, I have shown anatomy, physiology, and pathology, concurring to prove the cerebellum a single and peculiar instrument. In this respect, it is the opposite of the mass properly styled brain. I have, already, exposed the anatom- ical proofs of the brain's complexness, and for the evidence which physiology and pathology afford, referred my reader to the respective treatises upon these subjects.* Conviction of the brain, properly so called, being an assemblage of instruments, is readily obtained; but to specify the limits of these instruments, indi- vidually in the different species of animals, is a matter of extreme difficulty, if it be not, perhaps, impossible. Let us follow the procedure of nature in this part of our inquiry, and commencing with the most sim- ple brains, pursue them in their increasing com- plexness as we mount in the scale of beings, until we arrive at that of man, the most complicated of all. They who have gone into the comparative anatomy of the nervous masses, have constantly assumed the human brain as their type of compari- * Phrenology, or the Doctrine of the Mind; Lond. 1825, and Ob- servations on Insanity; Lond. 1816. 136 son. This they have always viewed as a simple mass; and then the hemispheres, the three lobes, the callous body, the anterior, middle and posteri- or commissures, the infundibulum, the mamillary bodies, the fornix, the septum lucidum, the hyppo- campus's foot, the striated bodies, the optic thala- mi, the semicircular ta?nia, the quadrigeminal tuber- cles, the aqueduct of Sylvius, the valve of Vieussens, peduncles of the brain, the annular protuberance, the medulla oblongata with its pyramidal, olivary, and restiform eminences, and lastly, the ventricles and their communications, have been their grand objects of comparison. The existence of parts, as this specimen of the nomenclature, in use, suffi- ciently proves, appears, therefore, to be admitted, or denied according to mere physical indications, the form and situation of the masses being especial guides to the conclusions formed. But this mode of studying the comparative anat- omy of the brain is insufficient. In the first place, it is certain that the mass, styled brain, consists of a multiplicity of instruments performing particular functions; again, it is undeniable that the cerebral parts, as they are spoken of, in systems of descrip- tive anatomy, do not constitute entire and special organs; that several of them, indeed, occasionally go to compose a single apparatus, a circumstance which we have observed in reference to the cere- bellum, when treating of that part. Further, it is no less obvious that the identity of the individual organs in the various classes of animals, ought to be determined by means of physiology, seeing that 137 the forms of those which are known to be analo- gous often vary immensely. I have already said, that no anatomist, who, in ignorance of the sense of smell, should examine the mere configuration of the encephalic masses, would venture to maintain that the olfactory nerves of the skate, flounder, fowl, seal, and ox, were masses performing analo- gous functions. And the outward appearances of the other cerebral instruments are not less diversi- fied than are those of the organ of smell. It is, consequently, in vain attempting to advance the comparative anatomy of the brain, without a knowledge of the affective and intellectual facul- ties of animals and of the functions performed by the different cerebral organs. The brain being made up of very many parts, whose functions are entirely dissimilar, the connex- ion of these with the nervous masses of voluntary motion, and of the five external senses, with the spi- nal cord in particular, requires to be examined. The cerebral organs have several primary roots, or bundles in the medulla oblongata; these bundles have been particularized by the names anterior and posterior pyramidal, and olivary, and restiform bod- ies. From what has been already said, however, it is evident that the last-mentioned masses do not be- long exclusively to the brain; we have seen that they include the roots of several nerves, and the primary bundles of the cerebellum. In the human kind all the three portions of the medulla oblongata are very distinct; but they severally become less and less evident as we descend in the scale of beings, 18 138 and when we arrive at fishes we are almost tempted to doubt their existence altogether. Some, indeed, go so far as to deny these animals a brain. To me, how- ever, it appears that every vertebral animal has a brain, in the strict and proper sense of the wTord; all have a nervous mass superadded to the nerves of the external senses and voluntary motion; and fishes, which, besides a cerebellum, seem to have ganglions of nerves of sense only, may be proved to possess a true brain in addition. The proof lies in this, that ganglions in general are proportionate to the nerves arising from them; but the ganglions of the skate, and the three pairs of ganglions which, in the eel, are regarded as the origins of the olfactory nerves, and the optic ganglions of the carp, pike, and many others of the class, are much too large in proportion to the nerves which issue from them. I, therefore, contend, that the olfactory and optic nerves of fish- es communicate with certain cerebral parts, destin- ed to peculiar functions, precisely as the same nerves do in birds and the mammalia, with this difference only, that the masses they communicate with, are of different sizes in these different species of animals. I repeat, then, that with M. Arsacky,* I conceive the complex structure of the optic ganglions in fishes to explain the functions which, in reptiles, birds, and mammiferous animals, are obviously performed by certain parts with which the optic nerves communi- cate. I apply the same idea to the ganglions of the olfactory nerves. In the eel, for example, I only give the anterior pair to these nerves; and in the de Piscium Cerebro et Medulla Spinali, Halte, 1833. 139 skate, no more than the outer parts of the ganglions with which they communicate. M. Carus, in his work on the brain,* broaches an idea that deserves a passing notice: it is to deter- mine whether, when the cerebral parts are sep- arated from each other at their peripheral extrem- ities, this is to be regarded as a sign of perfection or of imperfection. M. Carus himself considers the brain to be perfect in proportion as its masses ap- proach unity. The brain of the eel is, according to him, extremely imperfect, on account of the sep- aration of its constituent parts. This opinion seems to me too general, and but little satisfactory. We know that the brain and the cerebellum are always separate from each other. In proportion too as the last is perfected in the four classes of vertebral animals, its different parts be- come more numerous, more distinct, and lie farther from each other, especially towards their peripheral extremities. The brains of reptiles, of birds, and of several mammiferous animals, consist of two smooth hemispheres : in proportion, however, as the brain in superior classes of beings has more numerous of- fices to perform, that is, as it increases in perfection, the division into lobes becomes distinct, and convo- lutions appear. The cerebral hemispheres of the human foetus are at first smooth, but as the enceph- alon is developed, the separation of its peripheral parts grows ever more conspicuous. I divide the functions of the brain into two class- es : viz., affective and intellectual; and, in harmony * Carus, Darstellung des Gehirns, Leipzig, 1814. 140 with this physiological division, I recognise two kinds of cerebral parts. The anterior pyramidal bodies I consider the rudiments of such as belong to the intellectual operations; and the other bundles of the medulla oblongata (in man they consist of the olivary and part of the restiform bodies) which run across the annular protuberance to communicate with many of the cerebral masses, as the roots of those that pertain to the affective manifestations. This separation into two systems of parts is very evident from the medulla oblongata upwards, as far as the pretended optic thalami and striated bodies in man and the mammalia. Let us then examine them, one after another, through their entire course, com- mencing with the bundles of the intellectual faculties. The pyramidal bodies are scarcely to be de- monstrated in birds, and still less are they to be seen in reptiles and fishes; in the lowest mammif- erous tribes, however, they are abundantly evident. They differ in general as to their size and length, not only in the various species of animals, but also in individuals of the same, especially of the human kind. In man they usually commence about twelve or fifteen lines below the annular protuberance. As they approach this mass, they increase grad- ually in size, and it is in consequence of this struc- ture that they have obtained their name. There is a striking peculiarity in the mode of ori- gin of these bundles: the primary fibres of each do not issue from the same side as that on which they lie, but uniting, in the first instance, into two, three, or as many as five little cords, they cross the mesial 141 line of the body one above another, from below up- wards : the bundles of the right pyramidal body, therefore, come from the left side, and those of the left pyramidal body from the right side of the spinal cord. The structure just described is termed the decussation of the pyramidal bodies. It is a con- stant peculiarity; but it is modified as the number of decussating cords is concerned. When they are numerous,.the appearance that results very much resembles plaited straw. In some very rare in- stances, the two pyramidal bodies cross, as entire and undivided masses, from one side to the other. The decussation of the pyramidal bodies is a point of much importance in a physiological and patho- logical point of view. It is very long since cases of disease impressed the idea of a nervous decus- sation on the minds of medical men. Lesions on one side of the head were often observed to occasion so unfortunate a symptom as palsy on the opposite side of the body. Hippocrates himself mentions the circumstance; but Areta?us was the first who attempted to explain it, by supposing a decussation of the nerves at their origin in the brain. Dion Cassius is the next who speaks of a decussation of the cerebral nerves and spinal cord; but with him the subject dropt, and the attention of the medical world was only recalled to the fact in 1581, by Fa- bricius Hildanus. The true decussation of the pyra- midal bodies, however, was first described by Mis- tichelli, in 1709: it was noticed by Petit in the year following, and at later periods by Lieutaud, Santorini, and Winslow. The same authors also 142 speak of other decussations, but probably on mere supposition. Modern anatomists, before Dr. Gall and myself, were divided in opinion upon the subject of decus- sation. Many admitted the fact, but no one pointed out the place of its existence. Vicq d'Azyr, for ex- ample, confounds the simple transverse fibres be- tween the two halves of the spinal cord with the true decussation of the pyramidal bodies. Many others, among the number Prochaska, Barthez, Sa- batier, Boyer, Dumas, Bichat, and Chaussier, have, in the most positive terms, denied the decussation of the pyramidal bundles altogether, as we have shown in our reply to the report of the Committee of the French Institute upon our Anatomical Memoir. To demonstrate the decussation of the pyramidal bundles, we do not require any such maceration as Santorini believed necessary. It is sufficient to strip off the pia mater to show the structure. Let a slight cut be made through the membrane in the me- dian line, without implicating the cords beneath, the edges then separated gently, and the decussation will appear. It will now be easy to reckon the number of bundles, and to take them away in succession with the handle of the scalpel. (PL viii. fig. 1—1.) The decussation at this place is incontestable; but whether there be any similar peculiarity in other situations, and whether that of the pyramidal bodies suffices to explain all pathological phenom- ena, are points that still remain undetermined. Some authors believe that the bad symptoms which attend lesions of the encephalon are always 143 manifested on the side of the body opposite to that on which the injury occurs. Others again cite cases where injuries of one cerebral hemisphere have caus- ed pathological symptoms on the same side of the body. ' Although the palsy of the body,' says Haller,* ' produced by injury done to the brain, be commonly manifested on the opposite side, it often enough hap- pens that derangements of the brain and cerebellum affect the same side (as that on which they hap- pen.') He quotes De Haen, Schlichting, Morgagni, and others, in illustration of the fact. Prochaska believed that when the opposite side of the body suffers from cerebral affections, it is principally when the striated bodies are the seats of disease. Anatomy has as yet demonstrated no other decus- sation in the medulla oblongata than that of the pyramidal bundles. No crossing fibres has ever been found of the primary bundles of the cerebellum nor of the posterior cerebral lobes, although physi- ological experiments and pathological facts tend alike to prove that the influence of the cerebellar hemispheres is propagated to the opposite side of the body. May not the cerebellum possibly act only through the medium of the brain? To such an hypothesis the inability to perceive any proper lesion of the brain cannot be well opposed as an objection; because it is not at all times possible to determine precisely whether the cerebral fibres are healthy or diseased. The lesions maybe percepti- ble in one and not in another part. I conceive that * Physiologia, t. iv. p. 333. 144 the cerebellum may be evidently disordered, and that the brain may suffer, in consequence, without our being able to detect any traces of disease in its organization, whilst the opposite side of the body exhibits pathological symptoms. The connexions of the various parts, and the resulting influence of each on the others reciprocally, render observations on these matters difficult, and the conclusions de- duced more or less suspicious. Farther researches on the decussation of the nervous parts, not imme- diately connected with the masses of the anterior pyramidal bundles, are still wanted. The optic nerves decussate partially, and this is the cause why the eye is frequently deranged on the same side as that on which the brain is diseased. Let us now follow the pyramidal bundles in their course towards the annular protuberance ; and first, let us remark the fibres which are detached from the pyramidal, towards and around the olivary bod- ies, the media of communication, probably, between these different bundles of the medulla oblongata. (PL viii. fig. 2, 64.) May not these connecting fibres explain the influence of the lateral cerebral parts upon the opposite sides of the body ? The pyramidal bodies, just as they enter the an- nular protuberance, are somewhat contracted in their thickness (ib. c.) but they are, by no means, interrupted in their course. Immediately after hav- ing plunged into that mass, they separate into sev- eral bundles, and are mingled with cineritious sub- stance (ib. /.) Here many new fibres arise and join the others; all advance, some of them disposed 145 in layers, and some intersecting the bundles of the annular protuberance. The pyramidal bodies are so much increased in their passage, that on emerg- ing from the annular protuberance, they compose the anterior and outer two-third parts of the cere- bral crura or legs (ib. g.) To see the structure of the annular protuberance distinctly, that is to say, to see the transverse uniting fibres of the cerebellar hemispheres, the longitudinal fibres communicating with the pyramidal bodies, and the crura of the brain and the cineritious sub- stance intermingled with each, an incision of about a line in depth must be made across the transverse fibres : if the cut be made deeper than this, it must not be carried in a straight line, but in the slightly- curved direction of the longitudinal bundles; the transverse must now be separated from the longi- tudinal layers with the scalpel, by pushing those on the outside, towards the hemispheres of the cerebellum, and those on the inside, towards the mesial line of the annular protuberance. By this means, the mode in which the longitudi- nal, or bundles of the pyramidal bodies are aug- mented in the annular protuberance, as it were in a true ganglion, is made evident. (PL vii. fig. 2, 88; and pi. ix. fig. 2,/.) If these longitudinal bundles only are to be ex- amined, it is sufficient, by pushing the handle of the scalpel upwards from its inferior edge, to re- move the transverse layer of the annular protu- berance that covers them. Certain it is, therefore, that great errors are com- 19 146 mitted, when the two crura of the brain are de- scribed as blended together, and the annular pro- tuberance is styled a compound of the medullary or white substances of the brain and cerebellum. The great bundles called crura of the brain, thus appear to be, in part, at least, a continuation of the pyramidal bodies increased in size and in perfection. These crura, as they advance, also contain cineri- tious matter in their interior, from which, addi- tional fibres are continually sent off, to join and strengthen those that have come from below. In mammiferous tribes, the cerebral crura are very evidently divided into two parts, viz., an interior and external, and a posterior and internal mass. Two superficial furrows mark their limits respectively. They bear no regular proportion to each other. In the human kind, the anterior and external portion composes, as I have already said, two-thirds at least, of the entire crura; but in the lower animals, the posterior is, by far, the more considerable por- tion of the two. Before advancing further in our examination of the longitudinal bundles which we have followed from the pyramidal bodies across the annular pro- tuberance, let us first consider the origin and pro- gress of the bundles that compose the posterior and inner portion of the cerebral crura. I have already had occasion to say that anato- mists, besides the pyramidal, speak of the olivary and restiform bodies of the medulla oblongata of man. I have also shown that the restiform bodies contain the origins of the primary bundles of the cerebellum, and of the vocal, glossopharyngeal, facial, and trige- 147 minal nerves. The remaining fibres of these, and the fasciculi of the olivary bodies, mount behind the ganglion of the pyramidal bundles in the annu- lar protuberance, and, joining themselves with, aid the completion of the cerebral crura. In their course, they gain some increase in size, which, how- ever, is inconsiderable, compared with that of the pyramidal bundles. (PL vii. fig. 2, 87, 90 ; and pi. ix. fig. 2, a 70, 70.) The olivary bodies are, themselves, true gangli- ons, and present the general forms (pi. ix. fig. 2. a,) and modifications observable in the ganglion or dentated body of the cerebellum. (PL ix. fig. 2, s.) Their size varies greatly in different individuals. The cineritious and white substances are observed to be variously distributed throughout them. The modified appearances of the interior of the olivary bodies depend, as in other cases, on the mode in which they are incised for examination. The second, or posterior and inner portion of the cerebral crura, is intimately connected with quad- rigeminal bodies. In the mammalia, it is much more voluminous than the anterior mass; and as we descend in the scale of beings, its relative pro- portion increases continually. These two portions of the cerebral crura contain, so to say, the roots or primary bundles of the hem- ispheres of the brain, properly so called. They, however, it is evident, must be immensely increased in volume before they can form such a mass as the brain. It is as the upper extremity of the anterior portion, where the optic nerve winds over, and is attached to it by a pulpy layer; that is to say, at 148 the outer part of the striated bodies, that the great augmentation takes place. After this, the fibres advance of unequal lengths, and, expanding into layers covered on their peripheral extremities with cineritious substance, ultimately form the inferior, anterior, and external convolutions of the front and middle cerebral lobes. To show that the lower and inner convolutions of the middle lobe are formed by the anterior and out- er crural bundles, the middle lobe must be removed. This is easily effected, as it is separated from the anterior lobe by the fissure of Sylvius. Besides the depth and extent of the Sylvian fissure by this means exposed, certain short convolutions which do not reach the surface, will also be brought into view. These convolutions lie hidden between the middle lobe and the superior cerebral parts. The bundles which issue from under the optic nerve will also now be seen to belong to the middle lobe, and to the anterior part of the posterior lobe. (PL viii. fig. 1, w, w.) If the entire outer part of the striated bodies be removed, the manner in which the convolutions, situated along the middle region of the hemispheres, on a level with the temples, arise from the bundles in continuation with the pyramidal bodies, will be made apparent. (PL ix. fig. 1.) The pyramidal bodies, their ganglions in the annular protuberance, the an- terior and outer portions of the cerebral crura, and the convolutions in which their bundles terminate, are always developed in the direct ratio of each other. Let us now trace the posterior and inner bundles of the cerebral crura to their termination. These 149 plunge into a thick, massy, and firm ganglion, flat- tened in the middle, and unequal above, and poste- riorly ; this is generally known under the name of optic thalamus, it having been long regarded as the origin of the visual nerve. The error, here com- mitted, I have exposed in speaking of the apparatus of vision. These ganglions, commonly called thalami, are developed in the direct ratio of the cerebral convolu- tions dependent on them. The posterior and inner portions of the cerebral crura being larger in the lower animals than the anterior and outer masses of the same, it follows that the convolutions of the upper and posterior parts of the hemispheres must be more considerable than those of the anterior and middle lobes. The most internal part of the thalamus is the largest in animals, consequently, so is the mass of convolutions that belongs to it. In the interior of the cerebral ganglion we are now discussing (pi. ix. fig. 2, p,) there are a great number of very fine nervous filaments ; these unite at its superior edge into bundles, which then diverge towards the convolutions in the manner of rays. The two portions of the ascending masses, called cerebral crura, which I have just described, may be separated from each other either by the blow- pipe, or a stream of water. At the place, however, where they issue from the thalami to enter the striated bodies, their fibres are all so intimately united by a transverse tissue, that any farther parti- tion of the two portions becomes impossible. The anterior bundles of the thalamus traverse the striated bodies. These are so named, because, when 150 cut in the usual mode, anatomists fancied they be- held alternate streaks of white, and of cineritious substance. But the gray matter is not disposed in bundles; it is a mere mass traversed by white di- verging fibres. By scraping away to about the middle, or to the place where the large white bun- dles pass, the cineritious substance will be seen disposed in the form of streaks between them; a closer inspection, however, proves that it only lies in the intervals between the fibrous bundles. The masses styled optic thalami and striated bodies, therefore, are true ganglions, in which the primary bundles of the brain are increased in their progress to completion in the convolutions of the brain; for the radiated diverging fasciculi expand into layers, and, being covered with cineritious sub- stance on their extremities, compose the convolu- tions. The faultiness of the ordinary method of examin- ing the structure of the brain will now be, in some degree, appreciated. Instead of tracing the masses from their rudimentary state upwards to comple- tion, anatomists have been in the habit of commenc- ing the dissection by mutilating the parts when already arrived at perfection. By scraping the parts, Vieussens followed, and has given a rude drawing of the nervous bundles in connexion with the pyramidal bodies. These fasci- culi, however, he derives from his oval centre: he was altogether ignorant of their destination — the formation of the convolutions. He conceived them all to unite in the pyramidal bodies; he, therefore, 151 had no idea of the successive additions to, or of the augmentations of, the primary bundles. Vicq d'Azyr has attempted to imitate the prepa- ration of Vieussens, in his twenty-second and twen- ty-third plates ; but the mass of gray substance, and the nervous bundles which traverse, and partly arise from it lying obliquely, he could not possibly succeed by his horizontal incisions. In the striated bodies, he only saw alternate streaks of white and gray substance; these he also regarded as coming from above downwards, to pass united in a single bundle across the annular protuberance. He has, moreover, neglected several rudimentary bundles entirely ; he regarded the streaks of the superior as shorter than those of the inferior parts, in conse- quence of having cut them first, and he was quite ignorant of their prolongation into the convolutions. He only makes them proceed forwards, overlooking their sideward and backward directions altogether. Vicq d'Azyr, therefore, has done nothing more than picture, and that in a faulty manner too, mutilated pieces of the encephalon. I conceive it of importance further to remark, that the nervous fasciculi are less numerous but larger in the posterior and middle than in the anterior region. In the latter they are very numerous, but also very small. (PL ix. fig. 2; and pi. x. fig. 1, A. P. P.) This anatomical fact corresponds with what we know of the physiology of the brain, and explains the reason why the organs situated in the forehead are more numerous but smaller than those which lie in the occipital region. Let us now glance over the comparative anatomy 152 of the cerebral parts, the structure of which we have examined particularly in man. The anterior pyramidal bodies are generally dis- tinct in mammiferous tribes, but they are compara- tively smaller than in the human kind. The lateral parts of the medulla oblongata are not formed like the olivary and restiform bodies with which they correspond in man; their fibres, however, are cer- tainly prolonged beneath and across the transverse uniting fibres of the cerebellum. At their exit from this mass, the anterior and external parts of the cerebral crura are observed to be proportionately smaller than the interior and posterior portions. The relative size of the two portions of the crura, indeed, are found to vary exceedingly in the differ- ent tribes of the mammalia. The anterior and outer bundles, or continuations of the pyramidal bodies, extend under the optic nerves (which by their external edges communicate with the cerebral masses precisely as in man,) and on the external parts of the striated bodies gain an increase of size in the direct ratio of the anterior convolutions of the middle lobes, of the outer convolutions of the front lobes, and of those around the fissure of Syl- vius. The description of this external portion of the striated bodies is entirely omitted in the work of Tiedemann. The posterior and internal parts of the cerebral crura are intimately connected with the quadrigeminal bodies; they then plunge into the pretended optic thalami, and join themselves to the striated bodies; their structure, in short, is analogous to that of the same parts in man. In birds and the inferior classes, all traces of pyr- 153 amidal bodies, visible in the mammiferous tribes, disappear; and all analogy, as regards form, with the medulla oblongata of man is lost. The medulla oblongata, however, of all vertebral animals is in- variably augmented downwardly and outwardly in proportion as nerves of greater or smaller size are detached, or as the rudimentary bundles of the proper cerebral masses are thence derived. The mere outward form, here as elsewhere, is not the essential consideration. It is quite certain that several parts of the medulla oblongata proceed for- wards, and pass under the optic nerve; and that in birds and reptiles there lies a mass below the posterior cerebral lobes, and on the inner side of the crura, which resembles in every thing the sup- posed optic thalamus; and, finally, that in the two classes mentioned, the advancing bundles encounter true striated bodies. (PL xi. fig. 2 and 3, I. I.) The pretended optic thalami and striated bodies, or the two principal ganglions of the cerebral hem- ispheres, consequently, exist in birds and reptiles, precisely as they do in man and the mammalia. M. Serres must therefore be mistaken when he denies the existence of striated bodies in the brains of these animals.* Dr. Gall and I were unquestionably the first who distinguished the true optic ganglions from the sup- posed optic thalami, which are masses belonging to the brain, and who demonstrated the relations be- tween the two cerebral ganglions and the hemis- pheres. * Rapport, &c. p. 70. 20 154 Among fishes, in fine, certain cerebral masses in the form of ganglions, and covered externally with cineritious substance, are always to be found. When treating of the optic and olfactory nerves. I proved that the entire encephalic mass could not be destined to originate them. But it is a difficult task to compare analogous cerebral parts in different tribes and classes of animals. In fishes, the me- dulla oblongata is evidently continued forwards, and from it are detached, in succession, the primary bundles of the cerebellum, and of the cerebral nerves; but M. Serres promulgates an error when he says, that the cerebral hemispheres of fishes are simple rounded bulbs, lying before the quadrigemi- nal tubercles, in which the crura expand. Two longitudinal bands we can observe continued on- wards to the olfactory nerves, with which a larger or smaller number of ganglions communicate; but whilst the functions of these shall remain undis- covered, they can only be designated according to their numerical number. (PL ii. fig. 1 and 13; 1, 2, 3, 4.) The anatomists also certainly err, who speak of the restiform bodies as simple bundles. The olivary bodies, as they are not to be demon- strated in the greater number of animals, cannot belong to the quadrigeminal tubercles, as M. Tied- emann supposes. In reptiles, birds, and the mammalia, the prolon- gation of the two great cerebral ganglions compose the hemispheres, as they are styled, of the brain. The volume of these varies extremely in different kinds, and even in different individuals of the same 155 species of animals, and this not only in their total- ity, but also as their various portions are concerned. The external surface of the cerebral hemispheres in reptiles, birds, and several mammalia is smooth. The division into three lobes, however, is always marked by slight furrows, and the individual parts are dissimilarly developed in different situations. In the greater number of the mammalia, as in man, the hemispheres are convoluted, and furrowed to a greater or less depth. Of the Structure of the Convolutions, and of the possibility of unfolding them. The unfolding of the cerebral convolutions has sometimes been considered as the essential point in the anatomical discoveries of Dr. Gall and myself. This is the reason why some have taken particular pains in combating the possibility of this operation. The fact is in itself of great importance; it affords several explanations that particularly interest both physiology and pathology. Many cases of disease would be quite inexplicable without a knowledge of the structure of the cerebral convolutions; yet not this, but the demonstration of the plurality of the nervous instruments, their independent exist- ence, and their connexions with each other, consti- tute the essentials of our anatomical inquiries. It has often been, and is still said, that the vascu- lar membrane, by plunging at intervals among the white substance, to convey the blood-vessels neces- sary for its nourishment, is the cause of the cerebral convolutions and anfractuosities; but no part of 156 the structure is thusmechanical. The convolutions internally consist of white fibres, which are covered on their extremities with cineritious substance. These fibres, which terminate the nervous bundles of the cerebral crura, are not all of the same length. Many, especially of those which are situated on the outer sides of the convolutions, terminate immedi- ately beyond the exterior walls of the cavities; the others extend to distances progressively greater as they run more centrally, those of the interior ex- tending the farthest of all. It is in consequence of this peculiar structure that prolongations and de- pressions are formed on the surface of the hemis- pheres. The cineritious substance follows all the forms composed by the white fibres, and covers every elevation and depression with a layer. The fibres of analogous bundles are not prolonged in every individual to the same distance, not even in the two hemispheres of the same brain. I find that the parts which are most largely developed have the fewest inequalities on the surface of the convo- lutions, the fewest depressions, and even the smallest number of anfractuosities. They are simply volu- minous, and their peripheries are regular and smooth. The convolutions are for the most part inclined slightly to the roof of the ventricles; they rarely stand up vertically. Their peripheral edge is fre- quently depressed, and this gives them an appear- ance similar to that which a fold of paper takes when its edge is pressed lightly inwards. (PL ix. fig. 1 and 2.) When a convolution is cut across vertically, the white substance will be observed of greater thick- 157 ness at its bottom than at its top. This happens from the nervous fibres losing themselves successive- ly on either side in the cineritious enveloping layer, whilst those of the centre only run to the extremity. A clean cut only shows the white substance of the convolutions as a simple mass. No line of sep- aration can be perceived in any direction. Never- theless, it may be demonstrated to consist of two layers covered externally by cineritious matter. These layers meet in the middle line of the convo- lutions, and are slightly agglutinated by means of a very delicate neurilema. On this structure is founded the possibility of separating, without injuring, the fibrous layers, and thus of extending or unfolding each convolution into a simple sheet. Before citing any anatomical evidence in support of the above fact, I shall give, as briefly as possible, the history of its discovery. Dr. Gall having had several opportunities of dissecting hydrocephalic heads, found, as Morgagni had before him, the brain distended like a large bladder, several convolutions having entirely, and others in part, disappeared. The internal surface of the enlarged cavities was uniformly white; and, generally, the nervous fibres and the blood-vessels that accompanied them were distinctly perceptible. He also met with a female, fifty-four years of age, whose head was much en- larged, without doubt in consequence of considerable dropsy of the brain. In person she was thin, but she was as active and intelligent as women usually are in her sphere of life. Dr. Gall being convinced that the brain is indispensable to the intellectual 158 manifestations, drew, as I have said in the preface, the same inference as Tulpius had done before, viz. that the encephalic mass cannot be disorganized or destroyed in those persons, who, being affected with hydrocephalus, still preserve their understanding entire; and, farther, that the structure of the brain must be different from what is commonly supposed. The female, whose case is quoted above, happen- ing to die of an inflammation of the bowels, Dr. Gall found that the cerebral cavities contained about four pounds of pure and limpid water. The convolutions of the upper part of the forehead, and of the superior region of the head, had disappeared entirely; lower down, however, they were distinct in different de- grees. In the interior of the great cavities, the fibrous structure and the blood-vessels appeared very conspicuous. I saw this head dissected at Dr. Gall's house, but neither of us at that time had any definite idea of the mode in which the cavities had been enlarged, the convolutions obliterated, and the cineritious substance made to appear spread over the entire surface, like an envelope of nearly equal thickness. This case, however, gave a new impulse to our anatomical inquiries on the encephalon. We began by attempting to imitate artificially the state of the brain in hydrocephalus. Having stripped off the pia mater, we insinuated our fingers into the great cavities; and by pressing, more particularly against their posterior walls, we found, with pleasure, that after overcoming a slight resistance at the com- mencement, the convolutions separated along their interior, with all readiness, into two parts. Even 159 this rude experiment made us conceive the possibil- ity of the gradually increased and unremitting pres- sure of the water, as it accumulated in hydrocepha- lus, unfolding the hemispheres into a membranous sheet, without destroying any of the nervous fibres. By degrees our anatomical discoveries were in- creased, and our ideas of the change effected on the cerebral masses in hydrocephalus became exact. The imagination of Dr. Gall's auditors was some- how more struck by the experiment above related, than by the whole of our cerebral anatomy besides. Our opponents, by the same reason, have conceived themselves particularly called upon to disprove its practicability. In their zeal, they have sometimes put sentences into our mouths which we never ut- tered : we have been made to say, for instance, that the entire brain could be unfolded into a membrane without any laceration of fibres: our idea has also been travestied, by reporting that we regard each separate convolution as a sort of little purse. The question concerns the structure of the convo- lutions only, and the possibility of proving them composed of two distinct layers, separable from each other. Let us, therefore,proceed to the inquiry. If a convolution be cut vertically across to its base, a very gentle pressure with the finger on the cut surface will suffice to separate its two fibrous layers. The surfaces by which these were aggluti- nated, will, even after this rude procedure, remain perfectly smooth and equal. At the base of the con- volutions there is a mass which prevents any further disjunction of their component layers. Of the struc- 160 ture of this mass I shall speak, when I treat of the commissures, or uniting fibres of the cerebral parts. If a portion of the hemisphere, lying above the ventricles, be taken in one hand, and the ventricular surface be lightly pressed on by the other, the mass at the base of the convolutions, which has just been mentioned, will be torn, and the two layers of the several convolutions then yield readily, and may be forced apart by the fingers. Whilstthis is doing, we may always observe a slight furrow along the line of every separation, and the blood-vessels coursing along the same. Now, this could not happen, were not the convolutions composed of two fibrous layers, not united by transverse fibres, but simply agglutina- ted by means of a fine and yielding cellular tissue. If the convolutions be cut off externally to the cavities, they may be readily unfolded, and without any tearing of parts. The convolutions, hardened in alcohol, in diluted nitric or muriatic acid, or in oil by simple boiling, may also be unfolded with perfect ease in the mid- dle line, and only in the middle line of their agglu- tination. No vestige of laceration will there be perceived, but the fibrous expansions on either side will become exceedingly distinct. It is objected that the fibres may be separated at every point, and by the side of the middle line of the convolutions. Now, this is as it ought to be, be- cause each layer is made up of many fibres, running in one and the same direction. The fibres of every ligament and muscle may be parted in like manner ', but this fact does not do away with the existence of entire ligaments and muscles, distinct and easily 161 separable from each other. Moreover, the nervous fibres on the outer surfaces of the layers, as they are continually and successively dipping into the cineritious substance, may the more readily be sep- arated from each other, the more the bundles lie externally on which the attempt is made. To prove further that the convolutions consist of two fibrous layers but slightly agglutinated, the following experiment may be tried: — Let a convolution be cut transversely, and blown upon at random through a pipe; both the white and the gray substance may, although with some diffi- culty, be destroyed; but no disjunction of fibres nor of the two substances will be effected. But let the stream of air be directed on its middle line, and it will instantly be split from the apex to the base. (PL ix. fig. 1, 1—2.) If the same experiment be made on a convolution that is slightly depressed at its summit, it will open at the base by a simple cleft, but at the upper part this will branch off to- wards the two corners. (PL ix. fig. 1, 1—2.) When, instead of air, water is thrown with a sy- ringe on a convolution cut transversely, the separa- tion is effected in a similar manner; this fluid too, may be pushed for three or four inches along the middle line, between the layers; it will even follow all the windings, and fill all the subdivisions of the convolutions that occur in this space. If after their injection the superior edge of the convolutions be cut away longitudinally, to the depth of about two lines, they will be found divided into two equal parts; and the manner in which the fibres penetrate 21 162 and lose themselves in the cineritious substance on either side will also be distinctly seen. Water thrown with violence upon the outer sur- face of a convolution will wash away the gray and one half of the white substance; as soon, however, as it reaches the middle line, it will penetrate to the right and left, and separate the two layers to the extent of an inch or two each way, as in the former experiment. Here also, as in the former case, the direction and distribution of the nervous fibres will be rendered very apparent. All these demonstrations prove incontestably that the convolutions consist of two fibrous layers agglu- tinated together, and surrounded by cineritious sub- stance. Nevertheless, authors still continue to speak of the cerebral pulp, or of the medullary and pulpy substance of the convolutions. But the idea of a mere pulpy matter is in contradiction to all known anatomical and physiological phenomena. Were the white substance pulpy, it would be de- stroyed or carried away by a stream of air or water directed upon it. It would be impossible, on the supposition of a pulp, to account for the similar re- sult of the various experiments related, viz. the regular separation of the convolutions into two layers along the course of their middle line. In connexion with the idea of a cerebral pulp, that of hydrocephalus, to any considerable extent, becomes an impossibility, because the slightest dis- tention of any one portion of the brain would tear the pulp, and the disease could no longer proceed. But many cerebral parts are often seen completely unfolded, and still resisting the contained fluid, 163 although theybestretchedintoa membrane scarce- ly two lines in thickness. Similar changes, too, are frequently undergone by the majority of the con- volutions without any laceration or destruction of parts. Some anatomists appear to have felt this difficul- ty, and, to escape, have said that the cineritious substance was very tenacious, even more so than the white, and this quality enabled it to resist lac- eration, and to suffer the expansion peculiar to con- siderable hydrocephalus. But all good anatomists know full well that the cineritious is much softer than the white substance; that the first is never seen disposed in layers, except where the last occurs in distinct bundles; indeed, that in the very places where the cineritious sub- stance appears in layers, it falls into pulp the mo- ment the white fibres are taken away. In hydrocephalus, some anatomists have pretend- ed to see nothing more, no other change than a mere extension of the cerebral cavities. But on this supposition, how may the entire disappearance of the convolutions be explained ? Were the cav- ities merely distended, the convolutions would only be separated more widely from each other; they would never be unfolded; their component layers would not, from a naturally vertical, assume a hor- izontal position. The structure of the convolutions once familiar, however, we readily conceive a capacity in the hemispheres to undergo great changes without the occurrence of any actual disorganization. We even perceive that when the cerebral fibres chance to be 164 elongated, this does not necessarily imply derange- ment of intimate structure: vision is well known often to remain perfect when the optic nerve is very much lengthened. Anatomy, consequently, shows how it comes that individuals affected with dropsy of the brain may manifest all their intellectual and affective facul- ties. We cannot now say with Walter, Ackermann, and so many others, that in hydrocephalus there is destruction or absorption of the organization, and that the mental functions are all necessarily anni- hilated. The physiology of the brain being now established, the existence of this mass of organs is seen clearly to be indispensable, and its disorgani- zation impossible in those hydrocephalic persons who exhibit affective and intellectual faculties. All this is made evident by our discoveries in anat- omy and physiology. It still remains for me to make some inquiries into the comparative anatomy of the cerebral hem- ispheres. To show the presence of the brain in the lower animals is not enough; the particular organs that compose the hemispheres must be de- termined in addition ; the resemblances to the three cerebral lobes of the human kind, and to the indi- vidual portions of each being also demonstrated. Anatomists are not even agreed as to the existence of the posterior lobes in all mammiferous tribes. Messrs. Cuvier, Tiedemann, Serres, and others, maintain that the posterior lobes of the brain are only found in man and the quadrumana. They rest their opinion on the fact of the cerebellum, in all the other classes, being uncovered by the brain. 165 The conclusion here, however, is faulty, and the generally horizontal posture of animals explains, in the most satisfactory manner, why the cerebel- lum, instead of lying under, is situated behind the posterior lobes. Let me observe, once for all, that no part whatsoever can be denied on account of mere difference of configuration. Were it thus, the existence of the front and middle lobes, in all the lower animals, might also be disputed with perfect propriety. The bulb of the olfactory nerve is cov- ered by certain cerebral parts, in man. monkeys, and the phoca?; but in the generality of mammifer- ous animals and birds, it lies, altogether, anterior to the cerebral hemispheres ; nevertheless, no one concludes that they, therefore, want the front lobes. On the other hand, it is a great error to assume with M. Serres,* as an axiom, that —' The enceph- alon of all vertebral animals is constructed after one uniform type, and with the same elements,' or with M. Cuvier,! to say that —' The brains of the mammalia have the same parts as that of man.' In principle, I maintain that the cerebral hemis- pheres of animals are composed of a greater or smaller number of parts, as different from each oth- er as the optic from the auditory nerves. The ex- istence of the posterior lobes of animals is not to be doubted, seeing that the ganglion, out of which they proceed (thalamus,) is found in them, as well as in man. Moreover, the functions of the human posterior cerebral lobes are also manifested by an- * Anatomie du Cerveau. t Anat. Compare, t. ii. 166 imals. To the above, it is still necessary to add, that variety in peripheral expansions of individual organs, indicates neither the absence nor presence of any one in particular: one hemisphere that is entirely smooth, and another that is furrowed in all directions, may both of them contain the same ele- mentary parts. This consideration it was which induced me to say, that the comparative anatomy of the brain could not be advanced without the assistance of physiology. With the aid of physiol- ogy, however, discoveries of great interest might, undoubtedly, be made. The analogy that exists between the brains of the cat family (pi. iv. fig. 5,) of the dog tribes (pi. iv. fig. 6,) of the sheep, mon- key, and other kinds, is as striking as the general similarity of disposition and character of each. The same law holds in the case of man. Individual or- gans, however, are more or less developed in every species of the same genus. In pi. v. fig. 1 and 2, I have given representations of the brains of two spe- cies of monkey. They are, evidently, much alike, considered in general; but still, the anterior lobes in fig. 1, are smaller, comparatively, than in fig. 2. The brain of the ourang-outang is figured in the same plate, fig. 3, and 4, and the brain of an idiot girl, fig. 5 and 6. There is a striking general re- semblance between the two, yet the front lobes of the ourang-outang are more considerable than those of the idiot. In a precisely similar manner are the different constituent parts of the healthy human brain dissimilarly developed, and their specification is matter of especial interest in the study of man. 167 Section VII. Of the Commissures or Fibres of Union. All the proper cerebral organs, like the other instruments of phrenic life, occur in pairs, or are double from the medulla oblongata up to their ex- pansion in the convolutions. This, probably, hap- pens because of their importance, and to the end, that the congenerate parts may supply each other's places, should either chance to be injured. But, in addition to the parts already described, there are others still that contribute to the formation of the brain. These have been long known by the name of commissures. Anatomists speak of the great commissure or cor- pus callosum, and of the anterior, middle, and pos- terior commissures of the brain. Until Dr. Gall and I published, it was the custom to take merely mechanical views of these, without attempting to discover their relations with the other cerebral parts, their derivations, or the causes of their dis- similarity in different animals; and this too, al- though Vicq d'Azyr had said * that ' the commis- sures seemed to exist for the purpose of establish- ing sympathetic communications between the dif- ferent parts of the brain.' The successive additions made to the diverging bundles, in their course from the medulla oblongata to the convolutions, is a point in anatomy that is * Memoires de l'Acad. des Sciences de Paris, 1781. 168 now generally admitted. I conceive that I have discovered another or second order of fibres, which, with Dr. Gall, I distinguish by the title, converging fibres, or apparatus of union. This discovery, how- ever, is disputed. M. Tiedemann calls it a chime- ra * Dr. Gall, in the sixth volume of his work, in 8 vols., on the Functions of the Brain, replies at great length to M. Tiedemann, and to those who dispute the converging fibres. For my part, I here address the same reproach to M. Tiedemann, which I have laid against him in another place, and in a general manner. He has admitted laws from re- searches made on the brain of the embryo, which are in contradiction with the structure demonstra- ble in the adult. Nothing can be easier than, by dissection, to prove the two orders of cerebral fibres: the diverg- ing and the converging, and to show that the mass or bundle called corpus callosum belongs to the converging order. The corpus callosum extends anteriorly and pos- teriorly beyond the striated bodies. (PL x. fig. 1.) Its thickness, at either extremity, is greater than at its middle. (PL vii. fig. 2, x, ^.) The fibres which compose the folds of the corpus callosum, proceed evidently from the convolutions which form the most anterior and posterior parts of the hemis- pheres, and by no means from the thalami and striated bodies. (PL x. fig. 1, 39 and 40.) Their direction, consequently, is entirely different from that of the bundles constituting either of the two * Bildungs-geschichte des Gehirns, Nurnberg, 1816, p. 156. 169 great cerebral ganglions. If the fibres of the sup- posed thalami be followed towards the posterior lobes, by scraping the parts, the converging fibres may readily be observed passing out betwixt them. (PL x. fig. 1, P.) Moreover, the two folds (ib. 39 and 40) of the corpus callosum are always propor- tionate in size to the fissures that part the hemis- pheres before and behind, or otherwise, to the con- volutions on either side of the fissures, but by no means to the diverging fibres. The corpus callo- sum is softer and much larger than the bundles of the striated bodies; the fibres of either of these masses, consequently, cannot be mere continuations of the other. But, in fact, the entire difference of the two or- ders of fibres cannot possibly be called in question, saving at the middle of the corpus callosum, or place opposite the striated bodies ; and even here, the analogy that exists between the middle and the extremities of the mass, may be assumed a suffi- cient index of an analogous formation. The scalpel, however, sets the matter completely at rest. The converging mass, I must not forget to state, does in- deed communicate with the great cerebral ganglions by means of a superficial band or layer, which has obtained the title of semicircular tape-worm. On examining the middle part of the corpus callosum at- tentively, its edge, opposite the great cerebral gang- lions, will be seen to consist of this semicircular band alone. It is composed of its own fibres, so to say, and increases in thickness as the middle line is approached; for the fibres become gradually thick- 22 170 er till they meet, in the same way as the folds in- crease, as they advance on each side to their junc- tion. In attempting to imitate the naturally unfolded state of the brain in hydrocephalus, by pressing upon the roof of the cavities with the fingers, a certain quantity of resistance is always experienc- ed at the bottoms of the convolutions. This pro- ceeds from a tissue formed by the interlacement of the two orders of cerebral fibres. This tissue once torn, the line between the layers of the convolu- tions is entered immediately, and they are then unfolded with perfect ease. Were the diverging fibres or bundles of the great cerebral ganglions prolonged directly into the cor- pus callosum, it would be extremely difficult to un- derstand how they could be elongated to the de- gree occasionally observed in hydrocephalus. The disappearance of the convolutions too, would be al- together incomprehensible; the water would act against the corpus callosum and the walls of the cavities, but there would be no reason for the un- folding of the convolutions. With the true struc- ture in view, however, all that happens in hydro- cephalus is easily explained; there is neither ab- sorption nor increase of the cerebral parts: the converging and diverging fibres have but to change positions; to separate at the place of their inter- section, at the bottoms of the convolutions; the convolutions to split in the line of their component layers, and the change is completed. I, for my own part, feel inclined rather to recognise increase than diminution of the cerebral mass in hydrocephalus. 171 Why, indeed, should not that happen in regard to the brain which so plainly occurs as the investing membranes are concerned ? How greatly is not the arachnoid coat, the dura mater, and even the osseous covering of the whole augmented? All concurs, then, to prove the presence of the two or- ders of fibres in the brain as well as in the cere- bellum. And now a question of much interest presents it- self; it is this: are the bundles styled corpus cal- losum an annular protuberance, true commissures, or organs of union ? It is quite certain that the annular protuberance belongs to the lateral parts of the cerebellum, and that it appears with, and is proportionate to, these. It is also undeniable, that the corpus callosum is in relation to the cerebral hemispheres, precisely as is the annular protuber- ance to the lateral parts of the cerebellum. We have seen, too, that both the brain and the cerebel- lum are composed of two orders of fibres. On the other hand, the nervous apparatus of animal life is universally double, and united in the middle line by means of commissures. I believe, indeed, that the law of commissures is quite general; but I doubt of each congenerate pair of fibres having its own commissure, or bundle, uniting in the middle line. This, in the first place, cannot possibly be the case in the spinal cord and medulla oblongata, because the apparatus of union is there much less consider- able than the several halves of these masses. The same thing may be said in regard to the annular protuberance and corpus callosum. These bundles, compared with the white masses of the cerebellar 172 and cerebral hemispheres, are much too inconsider- able. It would seem, therefore, that a small mass is sufficient to unite the two congenerate halves. Still, the mass that unites, and the mass that is unit- eel, not being of equal dimensions, does not gainsay the possibility of the commissures being proportion- ate to the congenerate parts that are joined. M. Serres, maintains,* that the corpus callosum is proportionate to the annular protuberance, and that the hemispheres of the brain are developed in the direct ratio of those of the cerebellum. It is easy to demonstrate this error. The masses men- tioned vary extremely, and are never developed di- rectly in the ratio of each other, neither in different kinds, nor in different individuals of the same kind of animals. To be convinced of this truth, it is enough to glance over the tables of the compara- tive sizes of the brain and cerebellum, which vari- ous authors (among the number, M. Cuvier) have drawn up. The proportion between the brain and cerebellum even varies in the same individual at different periods of life. The cerebellum attains its complete growth later than the cerebral parts generally. As to the proportion between the annular protu- berance and cerebellar hemispheres, and that be- tween the corpus callosum and hemispheres of the brain, recognised by M. Serres, these are facts announced by Dr. Gall and me, long before this gentleman's publication appeared. It still .remains to be seen, whether or not the * Rapport, &c, p. 72. 173 annular protuberance and corpus callosum are true commissures. These parts are only found in man and the mammalia: in birds, reptiles, and fishes, they are wanting. Birds and reptiles, however, have undoubtedly cerebral hemispheres, with two great ganglions; and, as in them, the other com- missures, entitled anterior, middle, and posterior, are of a size proportionate to the lateral masses, and are situated as in man and other superior tribes, they cannot be assumed to supply the place of the corpus callosum. It follows, therefore, ei- ther that the union of the cerebral hemispheres of birds is established according to another law than that of the mammalia, or that the annular protuber- ance and corpus callosum are not true commis- sures, in which event they would have to be con- sidered as mere constituent parts of the cerebellar and cerebral apparatus. Dr. Gall and I commit- ted an error in the first volume of our large work, when we treated of a part in birds, as analogous to the corpus callosum of quadrupeds. In the succeed- ing section of this book, I shall show that the mass in question corresponds, not to the corpus callosum, but to the fornix of mammiferous animals. There, too, I shall prove that we did wrong, in classing the entire fornix among the commissures. My doubts of the propriety of regarding the an- nular protuberance and corpus callosum as com- missures, increase in consequence of the existence of two longitudinal bands, running along the middle line of the latter, forming what is called its raphe, (pi. x. fig. 1,) and of the layer of fibres between the raphe, and between the two halves of the annular 174 protuberance, similar to that which occurs between the peduncles of the brain. (PL vii. fig. 2. x, /*, x.) Lastly, Reil relates a remarkable case* of a wo- man, above thirty years of age, whose intellectual faculties were very limited, and whose corpus cal- losum was found split in the middle line through its entire length. Last year, (1825,) through the kindness of Messrs. Morgan and Keys, surgeons to Guy's Hospital, Lon- don, I had an opportunity of seeing a similar case. The name of the man in whom it occurred was James Cardinal, a portrait of whom I gave, in 1815, in my work, entitled ' The Physiognomical System,' which Dr. Gall copied in his continuation of our large work, and which I myself re-published last year in my book on Phrenology. I do not think that any case more remarkable than James Cardi- nal's has ever been the subject of observation. He had hydrocephalus to an enormous amount, and manifested the affective and intellectual faculties. The greater part of the water was accumulated between the brain and dura mater, but the lateral cavities were at the same time distended by about a pint of fluid, which communicated freely with the liquid collected without the brain, as the corpus callosum, with its extreme folds, was split through its entire length along the median line. Its masses on either side, together with the lateral convolu- tions, were quite distinct. The union of the corpus callosum from this would not appear essentially ne- cessary to the unity of function of the two crebral * Archiv fur die Physiologie, B. ii. S. 341. 175 hemispheres. Nevertheless, I regard the corpus callosum, as well as the convolutions necessary to produce an organization fitted for its peculiar func- tions. This structure, perhaps, has some advan- tages, as the vegetative functions of the parts are concerned: it may possibly have the same end as the cavities and anfractuosities, of which I shall speak more particularly by-and-by. With the exception of regarding the corpus cal- losum as an apparatus of union, I see no reason to alter any of the other ideas relative to this mass, published by Dr. Gall and me, in our Memoir to the French Institute in 1808, and in the first volume of our large work : I still maintain the proportions be- tween its different parts and diverse cerebral mas- ses ; the dissimilar course of the diverging and con- verging fibres ; the accumulation of fibres at its two extremities, in consequence of the anterior and posterior fissures of the hemispheres. The direc- tion of the fibres forming the raphe, will, perhaps, be better explained in the succeeding section, which treats of the communication of the nervous apparatuses. The structure of the raphe appears to be more analogous to that of the fimbriated body, or its slip communicating with the fornix, and of the semicircular tape-worm of Haller (tania semicircu- lar is.) The idea formed of the folds of the corpus cal- losum, depends on the view that is taken of the mass lengthwise and from above. Anatomists, con- templating it in this way, have imagined that it was folded down upon itself before and behind, in con- sequence of the prolongation of the cavities into 176 the anterior and posterior lobes between the supe- rior and inferior masses of convolutions. The error of classing the fimbriated body and the entire fornix among the apparatuses of union is evident. To be convinced of the mistake, it is enough to remember the direction of the fibres composing these parts. The bundles of neither ever cross the median line; they always run into lateral masses, and are undoubtedly instruments of communication. In the mammalia there lies a band upon the me- dulla oblongata behind the annular protuberance, which Dr. Gall and I have regarded as a commissure of the auditory nerves. Our view, in this particu- lar, may be disputed, and I leave the point still un- determined. The band, however, is certainly in connexion with the auditory nerves. Let us now treat of the undoubted fibres of union, or of the true commissures. And, first, the nervous masses of the vertebral column are connected in the median line by a peculiar order of fibres. (PL i. fig. 6. a.) In the fourth ventricle of fishes, especially of those in which the lateral edges of this cavity re- semble ganglions, certain transverse fibres may be distinctly seen behind and before the median gang- lion, and uniting the two cerebral halves. (PL ii. fig. 14, e, e.) I am much inclined to think that the nervous in- struments are principally connected at their origins. The two halves of the cerebellum of all vertebral animals, for instance, are united by transverse fibres running between the two primary bundles that 177 come from the sides of the medulla oblongata. In the sturgeon, frog, (pi. iii. fig. 2,) and other fishes, and reptiles, where the cerebellum is so small, there is a thin white slip which joins the two sides to- gether. In other fishes (pi. ii. fig. 13,) and reptiles, whose cerebellum is larger, as also in birds, we may perceive a white layer at the bottom of the cere- bellar ventricle, terminated in birds at its anterior edge by a small tubercle. (PL iii. fig. 8, 43.) A similar apparatus is found in man, and in all mam- miferous animals. The cerebellum is at a greater or smaller dis- tance from the bigeminal or quadrigeminal bodies. This accounts for the different lengths of the part styled valve of Vieussens, which consists of a thin slip of fibres running transversely between the cer- ebellum, the edges of the primary bundles that ex- pand into the organs of the affective faculties, and the bigeminal or quadrigeminal bodies. (PL xi. fig-1, y) To show the interior of the fourth ventricle, the union of the two cerebellar hemispheres, and the Vieussenian valve in man and the mammalia, the brain must be laid on its superior surface (pi. vi. fig. 1,) the annular protuberance and medulla oblon- gata separated, and the two halves pushed aside. In fishes there is scarcely a part analogous to the valve of Vieussens. This happens from the vicinity of the cerebellum and the other ganglions. In birds, however, the part is very distinct. (PL iii. fig. 6, and pi. xi. fig. 3, y.) Behind the bigeminal bodies in birds (pi. xi. fig. 23 178 3,) and the quadrigeminal bodies in the mammalia and man (pi. xi. fig. 1,) at the point where the nerve of the superior oblique eye-muscles goes off (ibid. 13,) there is a transverse uniting band. The bigeminal or quadrigeminal bodies are al- ways united by commissures or transverse bands. (PL iii. fig. 6, and pi. xi. fig. 1, 2, 3, x.) The body called pineal gland is always found in animals pos- sessed of the great inferior ganglion of the brain (thalamus.) I do not believe that it occurs among fishes. M. Tiedemann entertains the same opinion; but M. Serres* declares, that the pineal gland is to be demonstrated in all the four classes of vertebral animals. According to him, the body has two sets of peduncles, one issuing from the optic thalamus, another from the quadrigeminal tubercles. He, however, is in evident contradiction with himself when he adds, that the thalamus does not exist in fishes. Anatomists now-a-days are agreed as to the part in question being no gland, but a nervous mass composed of white and cineritious substance. Its form and size vary extremely in different classes of animals. It always lies between the true optic and the great inferior ganglions of the brain. It has four connecting slips, two on each side. The pos- terior pair running backwards and downwards, and adhere to the proper ganglions of the optic nerves. (PL vn. fig. 2, and pi. x. fig. 1, B. e.) It is at the fore-part of the pineal gland that the * Rapport, &c. p. 69, 70. 179 granular concretions which have attracted so much notice are principally found. Malacarne* says, that he has seen them in the pineal gland of a goat, and Soemmering,! that he has detected them in that of a fallow deer. Des Cartes is generally known to have considered the pineal gland as the seat of the soul. At the present day, it were useless to repeat the arguments that StenoJ has employed against the Cartesians in refutation of their error. I only speak of the pineal gland in this place for the sake of connexion, for it appears to belong, at least, as much to the instruments of communication as to those of union. The part which, by anatomists, is entitled poste- rior commissure, is found lying anteriorly to the proper optic ganglion, and at the commencement of the great inferior ganglion of the brain, into which it plunges immediately. (PL iii. fig. 9, pi. vii. fig. 2. and pi. xi. fig. 1, v.) The transverse fibres, usually termed the soft or middle commissure, may be readily seen extending between the great inferior cerebral ganglions in the mammalia. (PL xi. fig. 1, 46.) Many authors have doubted the existence of this part in man. It may unquestionably be demonstrated in the human brain; but its very delicate fibres are easily torn, and care is required in the attempt to point it out. * Encephalotomia di alcuni Q,uadrupedi, Mautua, 1795. | Vom Hirn und Ruckenmark, Maynz, 1788. X Discourse on the Anatomy of the Brain, inserted in Winslow's Anatomy. 180 But it may be most readily shown from below by separating the crura, the annular protuberance, and the medulla oblongata. I now come to a commissure that is quite con- stant in all vertebral animals. In man, it is called the anterior commissure. Its regular occurrence, like that of the posterior commissure and uniting fibres of the quadrigeminal or bigeminal bodies, is an index of its importance. In the eel, (pi. ii. fig. 1,) and barbel, (ib. figs. 12 and 13,) it lies between the second pair of ganglions, beginning the reckon- ing from before (ib. fig. 13, 61;) in the flounder, (ib. fig. 4,) the roach, (ib. fig. 11,) and others, it is sit- uated between the olfactory ganglions. In all the animals possessed of striated bodies, viz. reptiles, birds, and the mammalia, it always lies anteriorly to the junction of the optic nerves. It traverses the striated bodies, and is continued onwards be- tween the anterior and middle lobes. (PL iii. figs. 6 and 9; pi. viii. figs. 1 and 2, and pi. xi. figs. 1 and 2,61.) In all the mammalia, the anterior commissure is composed of two parts : the one of these communi- cates with the olfactory nerve, the other, as in man, is prolonged towards the fissure of Sylvius between the anterior and middle lobes. This dis- covery I published in the spring of 1821, in a thesis, entitled Encephalotomie* The portion running to the olfactory nerve forms an arc, the convexity of which is turned backwards, the concavity forwards. (PL iv. fig. 4, 31.) * Vide p. 23. 181 In man, the anterior commissure traverses the exterior half of the striated bodies: it communi- cates first with the innermost convolutions of the anterior lobes; then with those situated at the bot- tom of the Sylvian fissure; and lastly, with those in the fore part of the middle lobe. It thus forms an arc, whose longest and concave edge is directed backwards. (PL viii. fig. 2, 61.) M. Chaussier * and M. Tiedemann,! regard the anterior commissure as a continuation of the cere- bral crura. Anatomy exposes this error. Having laid the brain on its superior surface, (pi. vi. fig. 1, and pi. viii. fig. 2,) turn back the optic nerves and separate the hemispheres slightly, a white cord about the thickness of a writing-pen will be brought into view. Remove the superimposed parts with- out implicating the anterior commissure itself, and its passage across the striated bodies will be dis- tinctly perceived. Arrived at the anterior extrem- ity of the middle lobes, the anterior commissure divides into a greater number of filaments that communicate with the convolutions. I shall bring this section to a close, by proposing the following question : — Is the anterior commissure proportion- ate to the cerebral masses destined to the intellectu- al faculties ? * Anatomie du Cerveau, p. 71. f Op. cit, p. 138. 182 Section VIII. Of the Communication of the Nervous Apparatuses. Thus far advanced, we have seen that the ner- vous system is an aggregation of organs performing special functions; that the instruments of phrenic life exist in pairs, and that there is a particular structure, by which congenerate parts are united. Another essential consideration refers to the com- munication with each other of the peculiar nervous apparatuses, an arrangement which is indispensable to account for the mutual influence, or, as it is termed, the sympathies of the functions. The functions of the thoracic and abdominal vis- cera are evidently mutually influential, and the nerves of vegetative life, generally, communicate together. Again, vegetative and phrenic life are mutually related ; imperfect digestion, for example, disturbs the intellectual energies, and excessive mental application, or moral sadness, interrupts the process of digestion. The communications between the nerves of the thorax and abdomen, and the an- terior roots of the spinal and cranial nerves, espe- cially the pneumogastric, the hypoglossal, the ab- ductor, the facial, and the trigeminal, are well known anatomical facts. Further, the spinal nerves are intimately con- nected with each other: there are longitudinal bands that run along the edges of the two fissures of the spinal cord; these are, especially, evident on the 183 dorsal aspect of the cervical portion (pi. i. fig. 5,) and on each side, at the bottom of the dorsal fissure. (PL i. fig. 7.) The nerves of voluntary motion and of sensation are also most intimately connected. Moreover, the nerves of motion, of general sensa- tion, and of the various special sensations, taste, smell, hearing, and sight, communicate with each other. The mutual influence of the organs of motion, of the external senses, and of the affective and intellec- tual faculties, equally require an organic communi- cation. The physiological experiments or mutila- tions which have been made in reference to this subject, are not, by any means, satisfactory. The cerebellum, which, according to M. Rolando, gives the locomotive power, and to M. Flourens, regulates the motions, is in more intimate connexion with the dorsal than with the abdominal roots of the spinal nerves ; yet M. Magendie has been led to conclude that the dorsal roots of these nerves preside over sensation, whilst the abdominal are the special or- gans of motion. It is, however, certain that the external senses and the organs of motion, aid, to a greater or less amount, the cerebral functions, prop- erly so called. It is to this end, consequently, that the nervous masses of motion, and those of general and special sensation, communicate with the organs of the affective and intellectual faculties. This communication is established by means of the me- dulla oblongata, the decussation of some of whose component bundles explains the effects of cerebral injuries being frequently manifested on the side of the body opposite to that on which they occur. 184 The sense of taste is subservient to the functions of digestion and nutrition ; and its instrument is a branch of the trigeminal, a nerve of general sensa- tion, supplying the organs of mastication, and arising very nearly from the same place as the glossopha- ryngeal, the pneumogastric, and the facial. The auditory nerve embraces the primary bun- dles of the organs of the affective powers, and these it assists more generally, especially among animals, than those of the intellectual faculties: males and females call on each other as the season of love draws near; the cries of the young bring the mother to their aid; the barking of the dog, the grunting of the hog, the crying of the monkey, collect other individuals of their several kinds to render assist- ance ; all the animals that plant sentinels, know the sounds of alarm; and the courage,* secretive- ness, or circumspection ! of many species is excited by peculiar tones, appreciated by the sense of hear- ing. The cock taunts his adversary by crowing; the sparrow, turkey, common fowl, and many other kinds of birds advise their young of an enemy's ap- proach, and by their cries admonish them to keep quiet, or to seek security by flight. The optic nerve aids almost all the affective and intellectual faculties. Instead of running directly from its origin to the eye, it therefore turns out- wardly, and, in its course, communicates with all the parts adjacent, from its ganglion, onwards, to the tuber cinereum ; so that in order to turn it back in man, it must be torn away from a thin communi- * Combativeness, of the new nomenclature. f Caution. 185 eating layer all the way along the outer edge as far as the external geniculated body. Phrenology shows that vision assists more immediately those organs situated backwardly, laterally, and anteri- orly, than those in the middle line of the head, such as veneration, (reverence,) firmness, hope, justice, conscientiousness, &c. In all animals, the optic nerve is evidently connected with a great number of cerebral masses. The nerve of smell always communicates with the anterior cerebral lobes, and in animals this sense aids in a very particular manner the faculties that take cognizance of external objects, their qual- ities, their relations, and the phenomena they pre- sent. Animals examine their food, their friends, their enemies, their dwellings, &c. &c. by means of smell. The olfactory nerve also communicates with the anterior convolutions of the middle lobes, especially with the part that corresponds to the hippocampus's foot, or the amnions' horn. If there be a cerebral part presiding over the functions of hunger and thirst, it would appear to be situated in this neigh- borhood. I am confirmed in this supposition by the vicinity of the organ of destructiveness. To conclude, — nature has taken great pains to establish an intimate connexion between the differ- ent cerebral parts themselves. Two longitudinal bands, running at the bottom of the dorsal fissure of the spinal cord, are continued into the fourth ventricle. In fishes they run as far as the olfactory ganglions. (PL ii. fig. 13.) 24 186 The cerebellum, as I have already shown, always communicates with the medulla oblongata, and the bigeminal or quadrigeminal bodies. The parts successively added to form the cerebral hemispheres are connected together by transverse bands or interlacing fibres. A band of this nature is sometimes seen below the olivary bodies. (PL viii. fig. 2, 64.) Different other transverse inter- lacements are visible in every human brain that is moderately consistent, 1st, at the superior edge of the annular protuberance (pi. viii. fig. 1, 33;) 2dly, in the middle of the cerebral crura (pi. vi. fig. 1, and pi. viii. fig. 1, 34;) 3dly, below the optic nerve (ib. fig. 1, 35;) 4thly, at the place where the fibres issue to enter the superior convolutions of the middle lobe (pi. viii. fig. 1, 36;) 5thly, between the two great cerebral ganglions (ibid. 37;) 6thly, at the external edge of the striated bodies (pi. viii. fig. 1, and pi. ix. fig. 1, 38.) These interlacing bundles do not occur on the external surfaces only, they also penetrate the interior of the masses to which they belong. The greater part of the fornix, that is to say, the fimbriated bodies running from the hippocampus's foot, the posterior and anterior pillars, and the lon- gitudinal fibres between them, as also the mammil- lary bodies and their prolongations, are evidently apparatuses of communication. (PL x. fig. 1 B. 57 — 62.) The anterior pillars of the fornix trav- erse the tuber cinereum, and plunge into the mam- millary bodies, from whence another bundle issues, to be lost in the great ganglion of the organs of the 18? affective faculties (thalami.) (PL x. fig. 1, 16.) In the mammalia the fornix further communicates with the two longitudinal bands of the corpus cal- losum (raphe,) and with the septum lucidum, which last in its turn communicates with the raphe, and with a band lying before the optic nerves. (Ibid. 57, 58, 59.) The raphe itself, and also the semi- circular tape-worm of Haller (pi. xi. fig. 1, A. 32,) appear to be instruments of communication, the first between the converging fibres themselves, the other between the great cerebral ganglions, and the con- verging fibres. These parts are strikingly analogous in all the mammalia. In birds and reptiles there are fibres, which, from the middle lobes, the circumference of the posterior lobes, and from the median line, come together towards the base of the brain, and form a bundle that communicates with the cerebral crura behind the optic nerves. This fibrous layer, or bundle of birds, consequently, resembles the fornix and septum lucidum of mammalia. (PL iii. fig. 6; and pi. xi. fig. 3, B. 60.) In fishes, besides the two longitudinal bands that run the whole length of the encephalic masses, each ganglion sends a slip of communication to its neighbor, the cerebellum to the optic ganglion, this to the succeeding one, and so on successively, whilst the most anterior of all sends fibres to join the com- mon longitudinal band of the olfactory nerves. (PL ii. fig. 13.) The mass entitled infundibulum lies behind the junction of the optic nerves. It is evidently trav- 188 ersed by white filaments, and communicates with the tuber cinereum. (PL ii. fig. 4, 6, 8; pi. iii. fig. 12; and pi. vi. fig. 1, 17.) The influence of the anterior pyramidal bodies, on the propagation of the will towards the instru- ments of motion, deserves some attention. Accord- ing to phrenological observations, the anterior lobes of the brain contain the organs of the perceptive and reflective intellectual faculties, without which there would be no will.* Now these lobes are in- timately connected with the pyramidal bodies, and these last communicate directly with the abdominal half of the spinal cord, whence, as it appears, the nerves especially appropriated to propagate the will are detached. Lastly, in the animals whose brains are smooth on the surface, the peripheral connexion of the in- dividual organs is manifest; in the other kinds, whose brains are convoluted, it is extremely inter- esting to trace the connexion of the different cere- bral masses composing special instruments. These connexions explain the mutual influence of the fac- ulties. The organs of analogous powers are reg- ularly in each other's vicinity; the convolutions that compose them even run into each other. The organ of philoprogenitiveness communicates with that of inhabitiveness, and with that of courage (combativeness;) the organ of courage also com- municates with that of attachment (adhesiveness;) and with that of destructiveness ; the organ of se- cretiveness communicates immediately with that of * See Philosophical Principles of Phrenology, Lond. 1825, p. 32 189 destructiveness, and with that of circumspection (caution ;) the organ of benevolence communicates with the organ of veneration (reverence;) the organ of firmness is in communication with those of all the faculties around it — veneration, justice, and self- esteem ; the organ of justice runs into that of the love of acquiring (acquisitiveness) — this is connect- ed with that of construction (constructiveness;) the organsof the perceptive faculties are all linked to- gether, as are those of the reflective powers in like manner; the organ of artificial language is placed across the organs of the intellectual faculties gener- ally. Thus, the especial pains which nature has taken to establish communications between the ce- rebral parts cannot be overlooked; and, as I have already said, it is this arrangement that enables us to understand the mutual influence of their respec- tive functions. 190 Section IX. Of the Anatomico-Physiological Relations of the Nervous Apparatuses. In Phrenology it is an admitted axiom, that struc- ture does not reveal function; still, it is certain that there is a relation between the organic structure of an apparatus and its functions. Many consid- erations bearing upon this point are contained in the works which Dr. Gall and I have published conjointly as well as severally. In this place it will be enough merely to refresh the reader's memory. No affective or intellectual function is ever mani- fested without a brain; and these functions appear simultaneously with the brain ; this organ is in the first instance pulpy; it increases in growth, becomes fibrous, and advances gradually to its maturity; but, after the meridian of life has been passed, it shrinks again, and the convolutions become less plump, and seem less firmly packed than in youth and man- hood's prime. The nerves also decrease in old age. The cerebral fibres in declining years acquire den- sity and firmness, but they lose in specific gravity. In conformity with these facts, the mental faculties of newly-born children are confined to voluntary motion, to the sensation of hunger, and to some ob- scure feelings of pain; by slow degrees the functions of the external senses become perfect; the child begins to attend to outward objects, to act upon 191 these, to manifest determinate desires and clear no- tions : it becomes, in succession, boy, youth, and man, when the faculties show themselves in their ut- most energy : little by little they now begin to lose strength, and old age is at length marked by blunt- ed sensibilities, and enfeebled intellectual powers. A defective development of the brain prevents the exhibition of affective and intellectual faculties, and causes idiotism. Men of great and general abilities have always a voluminous brain. Different cere- bral parts are differently developed in the two sexes, in different nations, and in different individuals of the same people. The cerebral organization is oftentimes alike in several members of a family. In all these cases it is easy to prove a relation be- tween the condition of the brain and the affective and intellectual manifestations. The brains of different species of animals vary in density, in texture, and probably in their general organic constitution. This is the reason why func- tions essentially similar offer so many modifications, and never correspond in degree of energy with that of the development of their instruments: that is to say, the affective and intellectual aptitudes cannot be measured by the absolute size of their organic apparatuses. Size, however, is one condition essen- tial to energy; and, other conditions being equal, the largest organs will show the greatest vigor. It would appear that, in conformity with the law established in regard to electrical phenomena, the energy of nervous apparatuses depends in a great measure on their quantity of surface ; more on this, 192 perhaps, than on their component quantity of ner- vous matter. M. Desmoulins, in his Memoir to the Academy of Sciences of Paris, attempts to prove, 1st, that integrity of surface is the only constant condition for the production of nervous actions; 2d, that the intensity and perfection of nervous actions depend on the proportionate extent of nervous sur- faces ; and, 3d, that the nervous actions are per- formed and transmitted by the surface. I do not mean to say that the quantity of a ner- vous apparatus has no influence on the energy of its functions; but the influence of the peripheral expansion is certainly great. May it be, on this account, that the cerebral apparatuses are hollow or lamellated? In the inferior classes of animals, the nervous masses are very commonly hollow. Cavities and anfractuosities unquestionably in- crease the surface of organs. Have the annular pro- tuberance and corpus callosum of man and the mammalia a like destination ? The cavities and convolutions also facilitate the circulation of the blood, and thus appear to produce twofold good effects. Let us turn, and then examine the cerebral cavi- ties and their communications. The common num- ber of cavities reckoned is four. The fourth is formed by the separation between the cerebellum and the medulla oblongata. (PL ii. fig. 13; pi. vii. fig. 2; pi. x. fig. 1; pi. xi. figs. 1 and 3, m.) The di- mensions and development of the fourth ventricle, says M. Desmoulins, coincides with the development of the eighth pair of nerves. Now, to me the fourth 193 ventricle seems nothing in itself, and not at all pro- portionate either to the cerebellum, or the eighth pair of nerves. It, in fact, depends entirely for its size on the united breadth of the medulla oblongata and development of the cerebellum. Is the cer- ebellum broad and the medulla oblongata narrow, the fourth ventricle will be narrower than it would have been, were the medulla and cerebellum both broad- In the sturgeon, frog, toad, &c, the fourth ventricle is of considerable magnitude, and the cer- ebellum very small. The fourth ventricle runs between the>nnular protuberance and valve of Vieussens on to the aque- duct of Sylvius, or canal between the crura of the brain and the commissure of the quadrigeminal bodies. (PL vii. fig. 2,
•) This
canal (iter a quarto ad tertium ventriculum) then
opens into the third cerebral ventricle, or separation
between the great ganglions of the affective facul-
ties. (PL xi. fig. 1, M.)
The third ventricle communicates on both sides
of the median line by the cleft between the fornix
and the great ganglions of the affective powers (pi.
x. fig. 1,) with the lateral cavities which extend into
the anterior, posterior, and middle lobes.
The communication between the lateral cavities
of the brain and the third ventricle is thus estab-
lished, and not by a natural opening, styled foramen
Monroi, nor by any supposed rupture of the septum
lucidum.
Having placed the brain on its superior surface,
and cut through the optic nerves, the crura, the an-
25
194
nular protuberance, and medulla oblongata at their
junction in the median line, and separated the two
halves, an opening will be seen between the anterior
pillar of the fornix and the great inferior ganglion of
the brain. This opening, just described, was called
a foramen, by Dr. Monro of Edinburgh, and anato-
mists have entitled it foramen Monroianum. It is, as
we have seen, no true foramen or hole, but a mere
part of the great cleft that brings the third ventricle
into communication with the great lateral cavities.
From this peculiar structure of the cerebral cavi-
ties, it must appear evident that a great accumula-
tion of water can never be found in one hemisphere
only. A small quantity may undoubtedly be col-
lected in the lateral and posterior parts of one of
the great ventricles; but, whenever it becomes
considerable, it raises the fornix, flows into the third
ventricle, and from this immediately gains the cav-
ity of the opposite side. All the observations which
have been made are consistent in this particular.
The large hydrocephalic crania preserved in muse-
ums prove universally that both hemispheres have
been distended.
Some anatomists speak of a fifth ventricle be-
tween the two layers of which the septum lucidum
consists; but in this sense, every anfractuosity may
be called ventricle.
M. Serres* is much mistaken when he maintains
that in fishes, reptiles and birds, there is no ventri-
cle, and that the brain is a solid mass. In fishes
each ganglion is hollow; and in reptiles and birds
there is a cavity between the striated bodies (the
* Rapport, p. 77.
195
great cerebral ganglions) and the fibrous band, pre-
cisely as there is one in the mammalia between
the two cerebral ganglions, the fornix and the sep-
tum lucidum. (PL xi. figs. 2 and 3.)
Here it will not be out of place to take a general
and cursory view of what are entitled physiological
experiments; the means which, in the eyes of experi-
menters, ought to establish and confirm the physi-
ology of the nervous system in general, and of the
brain and its parts in particular. Dr. Gall and I
have always declared against such violent means
of obtaining conclusions. We prefer observing the
relations between the development of different
parts and the exhibition of particular functions, and
investigating pathological facts. However, I do
not incline to neglect any means, whatever its na-
ture ; I conceive that it is a great mistake to con-
fine inquiries within any one particular channel.
Mr. Charles Bell regards anatomical research as
the means the most favorable to the progress of
physiology; Messrs. Magendie, Flourens, and oth-
ers, on the contrary, prefer physiological experi-
ments, in other words, mutilations; whilst M. Lel-
lemand, again, seems to give the preference to pa-
thological observations. Pathological cases appear
to have been more fertile hitherto in results both
anatomically and physiologically, than mutilations.
Observing, for example, that the effects of the lesion
of one side of the brain were manifested on the op-
posite side of the body, the decussation of the ante-
rior pyramidal bodies was sought for and discover-
ed. Observing that great distension of the cerebral
hemispheres did not impede the affective and intel-
196
lectual manifestations, the structure of the convolu-
tions was inquired into and demonstrated. Remark-
ing that sensibility may be lost without the powers
of motion suffering, and that the power of locomo-
tion may be lost without that of sensation being
disturbed, researches on the nerves of these two
sorts of functions have been instituted, and so on.
Pathological facts, however, cannot be interpreted
in opposition to physiological observations. Will
anyone say with M.Foville, that the superficial
substance of the brain presides over the intellectual
functions, and the white and deep-seated masses of
gray substance over locomotion, because he has
observed that derangements of these two orders of
function accompanied organic alterations of the su-
perficies, or internal part of the brain ? Will any
one say again, with Messrs. Foville and Serres,
that the great ganglion of the affective faculties
(thalamus) and its white fibres are in relation to
the motion of the arm and the ganglion of the in-
tellectual faculties (corpus striatum) and its radia-
ting fibres to the motion of the leg ? Is it not neces-
sary before such a proposition be received as cor-
rect, to prove physiological observations in harmony
with these pathological cases ? This, however, has
not and cannot be done; hence there cannot possi-
bly be any truth in the above surmises,
Physiological experiments would tend to the es-
tablishment of yet more extraordinary ideas. M,
Magendie pretends, that without the trigeminal
nerve (fifth pair,) we could neither hear, nor see, nor
smell; that the cerebellum is necessary to precision
m all movements forward, as every severe wound of
197
the cerebellum renders motion in this direction im-
possible ; that the crura of the brain, the quadri-
geminal tubercles,.the pretended thalami, and the
striated bodies, have all functions relative to motion;
lastly, that the nerves arising from the posterior
roots of the spinal cord belong to sensation, and those
of the anterior roots to motion, whilst M. Bellin-
geri * says, that his experiments lead him to conclude
that the function of the posterior roots is motion,
especially motion of extension, and not sensation.
He thought he could confidently advance, that when
feeling only was paralyzed, the seat of the disorder
was in the cineritious substance; that in palsy of
the locomotive powers without loss of feeling, the
disease was exclusively in the white substance; and
that in palsy of both functions, the two substances
were altered at the place where the nerves distrib-
uted to the affected parts arise. In fine, if the loss
of motion be a more frequent symptom than the
loss of feeling, this, according to our author, hap-
pens from the white substance being situated on
the outside of the spinal cord, and consequently, in
his estimation, being more exposed than the gray
to the action of external agents.
The cerebellum, by the experiments of M. Ro-
lando, is made out an electrometer, or sort of vol-
taic pile, and the sole source of a fluid that excites
motion; whilst M. Flourens, by his experiments,
discovers that the cerebellum is the balancer, or
governor, or regulater of voluntary motion.
* Mem. dell' Academia delle Scienze di Torini, torn. 30. The
reader may also find an account of these experiments in the Lancet.
198
In the opinion of some experimenters, the quad-
rigeminal tubercles are the seat of vision; but M.
Flourens is led to consider them as the simple con-
ductors of vision, which is not changed into percep-
tion, but in the brain itself, because he found that
on cutting away a lobe, he produced blindness of
the opposite eye.
The medulla oblongata, according to M. Flour-
ens, is the central magazine, the vital knot of the
nervous system, to which all impressions must
come, to be appreciated; from which the commands
of the will must emanate, to be executed ; to which
it is sufficient for parts to be attached, in order to
live, and from which to be detached, is sufficient
cause of death. But M. Fodera, from his experi-
ments, believes that the medulla oblongata is the
seat of dozing and of sleep; he even accords to
this part of the encephalon, some influence on the
production of moral phenomena.
Lastly, according to M. Flourens, the brain in-
cludes sensibility and will: without brain there is
no sensation, nor will. M. Rolando observed that
dozing and stupidity supervened each time he cut
away the cerebral lobes. He conceives the brain to
be, at once, the seat of the sensitive and intellectual
faculties, and the agent of the regulating and direct-
ing power of the cerebellum over the motions; an
agent, however, which cannot influence the motions
without the cerebellum, their immediate cause.
The reader must have felt the jarring and con-
tradictory nature, and insufficiency of the preceding
opinions. They do not appear to me worthy of any
199
detailed refutation. Their contradictions bespeak
clearly enough their falsity. I do not deny the
facts, as they are reported by the experimenters, but
I think these gentlemen over-eager and too ready to
draw conclusions: I see them unapt to distinguish
between facts and their causes, between primary
and secondary causes, and to separate powers and
conditions of activity from actions. I also think
that they neglect the mutual influence of the several
instruments too much; and that they frequently at-
tribute to one condition, that which belongs to sev-
eral. The trigeminal nerve aids the nerve of vision,
but the optic nerve is the instrument of sight; the
olfactory nerve is assisted in its office by the nerves
of inspiration, of the nostrils, but it alone possesses
the special sensation we entitle smell.
These physiological experiments or mutilations,
would reduce the functions of the spino-cerebral
system to a very small compass; and it is but natu-
ral that I should declare for the physiological obser-
vations made in the state of health, by considering
the relation between the development of special
organs, and the energy of peculiar manifestations.
These observations are collected, and their results
published in the volume entitled Phrenology.
I conclude this treatise by repeating, that the
anatomical knowledge of the nervous system in
general, and of the brain in particular, must be in
harmony with physiology and pathology; and that
any one of these three departments must be modi-
fied whenever there is contradiction in reference to
any of the others.
EXPLANATION
OF
THE PLATES AND FIGURES.
Plate I.
Fig. 1. Four ganglions, with their branches of commu-
nication, of the caudal extremity of a caterpillar. This ar-
rangement of the nervous system reigns through the entire
length of the body of caterpillars and worms.
Fig. 2. The caudal extremity of the spinal marrow of a
fowl, with the origin of the three last pairs of dorsal nerves.
Fig. 3. The three superior cervical pairs of nerves in a
calf, seen on the abdominal surface. The dura mater and
arachnoid are slit longitudinally and turned aside, so as to ex-
pose the mode in which the nervous filaments come from the
common mass, their different directions, and the passage of
the bundles through the dura mater. The commencement
of the intervertebral ganglions (a) is covered by the reflected
membranes. Between the different pairs of nerves, the teeth
of the ligamentum dentatum (6) are seen on each side of the
nervous/;mass ; these ligaments separate the abdominal from
the dorsal roots, and are attached to the dura mater by
means of slips, naturally different in size and position. At
the origin of each pair of nerves, a swelling is perceivable,
varying in magnitude, in proportion to the volume of the is-
suing nerve. The direction of the nervous fibres from be-
fore backwards, and from behind forwards, is evident.
J
201
2, 3. The accessory nerve.
5. Median abdominal fissure.
Fig. 4. The four superior cervical pairs of nerves in
man, seen from before.
Fig. 5. The five superior cervical pairs of nerves in
man, seen from behind.
The membranes, ligamentum dentatum, accessory nerve,
intervertebral ganglions, bulgings and contractions of the
nervous mass, are, absolutely, the same as in Fig. 3. On the
dorsal surface there are, frequently, fibres of communication
from one pair of nerves to the other; for instance, between
the fourth and fifth pairs.
Fig. 4. 5—5. Median abdominal fissure.
Fig. 5. 5 — 5. Median dorsal fissure.
a—a. Lateral dorsal channels.
Fig. 6. The edges of the two abdominal halves separa-
ted, permitting a view of the structure at the bottom of the
abdominal fissure.
/3 — (3. Lateral cords.
a__a. Apparatus of union of the two halves situated at
the bottom of the abdominal fissure.
Fig. 7. The edges of the two dorsal halves, separated,
permitting a view of the structure at the bottom of the dor-
sal fissure.
j3 — /3. Lateral edges.
a— a. Apparatus of union of the two halves situated at
the bottom of the dorsal fissure.
Fig. 8. Transverse section of the cervical nervous mass
of a carp.
Fig. 9. Transverse section of the cervical nervous mass
of a fowl.
Fig. 10. Transverse section of the cervical nervous mass
of a man.
In the three last figures the general type is the same :
two halves of the spinal cord; two median fissures, an ante-
rior and a posterior.
26
202
<*. Exit of the abdominal roots of the nerves-,
rj. Exit of the dorsal roots of the nerves.
Plate II.—Brains of Fishes.
Fig. t. The brain of an eel seen from above.
Fig. 2. The brain of a cod seen from above.
Fig. 3. The brain of a skate seen from above.
Fig. 4. The brain of a carp seen from below.
Fig. 5. The brain of a carp seen from above.
Fig. 6. The brain of a flounder seen from below.
Fig. 7. The brain of a flounder seen from above.
Fig. 8, The brain of a cod seen from below.
Fig. 9. The optic nerves in the cod are thrown back, to
permit a view of the two roots of the olfactory nerves.
Fig. 10, The brain of a pike, with the cavity of the optic
ganglion exposed.
Fig. 11. The brain of a roach.
Fig. 12, The brain of a barbel seen from above.
Fig. 13. The interior of the brain of a barbel, prepared
to permit a view of the ganglions, their communications
and junctions.
Plate III. — Brains of Reptiles and of Birds,
Fig. 1. The spino-cerebral system of a frog seen from
below.
Fig. 2. The brain of a frog seen from above.
Fig. 3. From M. Carus's work. — The brain of a lizzard
(Iacerta iguana) seen from above.
Fig. 4. From M. Carus's work. — The brain of a young
crocodile seen from above.
Fig. 5. The brain of a common fowl seen from above.
Fig. 6. The brain of a fowl. — The two hemispheres
separated to show the cerebellum, the optic ganglions, their
commissure, the commencement of the supposed optic thala-
mi, and the part analogous to the fornix of the mammalia.
203
Fig. 7. The brain of a turkey seen from above.
Fig. 8. The brain of a turkey with the cerebellum cleft
posteriorly, to show its lamellar structure.
Fig. 9. The brain of a common fowl, prepared to show
the cavity of the optic ganglion, the posterior commissure,
v.; the beginning and continuation of the great cerebral
ganglions.
Fig. 10. The brain of a duck seen from above.
Fig. 11. The brain of a goose seen from above.
Fig. 12. The brain of a goose seen from below.
Plate IV. — Brains of Mammiferous Animals.
Fig. 1. The brain of myrmesophaga didactyla seen from
above, copied from the work of Tiedemann.
Fig. 2. The brain of a hare seen from below.
Fig. 3. The brain of a hare seen from above.
Fig. 4. The brain of a hare placed on its upper surface,
like Fig. 2; the mass below the crura cerebri is cut off; the
crura are separated and pushed sidewards, to see the under
surface of the posterior lobes ; the fornix, 60; the posterior
fold of the corpus callosum, 40; the anterior pillar of the
fornix y ; the anterior commissure, 61, and its two portions.
Fig. 5. The brain of a cat seen from above.
Fig. 6. The brain of a dog seen from above. All parts
are reduced to the fourth of their natural size.
Plate V.
[The brains and their parts, represented in this plate, and all preparations
of the human brain, in the following plates, are only a fourth of their natu-
ral size.]
Fig. 1. The brain of the monkey, simia sabcea, seen from
above.
Fig. 2. The brain of the monkey, simia capucina, seen
from above.
Fig. 3. The brain of the ourang-outang seen from below.
204
Fig. 4. The brain of the ourang-outang seen from the
right side.
Fig. 5. The brain of an idiotic girl seen from below.
Fig. 6. The same brain of the idiotic girl seen from the
right side,
Plate VI.
Fig. 1. The basis of the human brain. — All the parts are
in pairs, but not quite symmetrical. The brain is taken
from the skull, and turned on its upper surface ; the cerebel-
lum and medulla oblongata have fallen backwards ; the dif-
ferent investing membranes are removed; the cerebral and
nervous masses alone are visible.
To take the human brain from the skull, I make an incision
from one ear to the other, turn the integuments backwards and
forwards, and detach the temporal muscles from the bone. If
it is requisite to preserve the skull, it must be sawed about
three-quarters of an inch above the superciliary ridge, round
on each side to the middle of the occipital bone ; if it be no
object to keep the skull entire. Bichat's plan, which is much
easier and more speedily accomplished, may be followed; it
consists in using the sharp edge of a hammer along the course
indicated, and breaking instead of sawing the skull. In this
way infinitely less risk is run of injuring the membranes and
cerebral convolutions than when the saw is employed. There
then usually issues a greater quantity of fluid from between the
membranes, and from out of the vessels of the neck when they
are divided, in consequence of the shaking the parts have un-
dergone ; the cerebral mass also sinks to a greater extent, and
the dura mater does not look so tightly stretched over the
convolutions, but the internal organization suffers no change,
When the skull-cap is removed, 1 cut the dura mater on
each side of the longitudinal sinus before and behind, and
transversely between the ears ; I then turn down the flaps,
detaching the falciform process of the dura mater in the fron-
tal region, and turn it backwards, I now bring back the head,
205
so that the base becomes the superior part; I support it with
the left hand in the occipital region, and the brain then lies on
the palm of the hand. Their own weight almost always suf-
fices to detach the anterior and middle lobes from their places,
—at the most, the slightest assistance from the fingers accom-
pli hes this. The bulb of the olfactory nerve generally sep-
arates from the ethmoid bone of itself, or the handle of the
scalpel detaches it with ease. The optic nerves, the infundi-
bulum, the oculo-motors, the abductor nerves of the eye, the
nerves of the superior oblique, and the trigeminal pair, must
be cut in succession : I now depress the head upon the hand,
first on one, then on the other side, pushing at each time the
hemispheres from the tentorium, in order to cut this part
across. After this, I detach the nervous pairs and blood-
vessels situated near the medulla oblongata ; and lastly, I cut
the cervical nervous mass below the occipital hole, as low
down as possible, not to damage the decussating fibres. I
raise the cerebellum with the fingers of the right hand, and
lift the entire cerebral mass from the cranium. There is one
precaution that is very necessary to be taken ; it is, to sup-
port the hemispheres with the flat of the hand properly, to
prevent the crura from being torn before the tentorium is cut,
and to guard against the same accident, in regard to the
medulla oblongata below the annular protuberance.
A hammer or a saw, and two scalpels, the one to separate
the integuments and the muscles, the other to cut the mem-
branes and the nervous parts, suffice for this operation. It is
well to have the second scalpel long in the handle, to secure
the cervical nervous mass being easily divided low enough
down.
Fig. 2. A skull sawed vertically through the middle of the
forehead, the vertex and the occiput, to expose the exterior
and lateral surface of the brain, cerebellum,annular protuber-
ance, and medulla oblongata in their natural situations. The
bone here supports the cerebellum and medulla oblongata,
whilst in Fig. 1 they had sunk downwards and backwards.
206
Plate VII.
Fig. 1. A skull sawed horizontally in a line from above the
eye-brows, by the middle of the temples, and the upper part
of the occipital bone. The membranes and blood-vessels
are removed, and the two hemispheres are seen from above.
Fig. 2. The skull, brain, and cerebellum cut vertically
through the median line, and in their natural situations. In
this preparation the various parts are retained in their proper
places, not without considerable difficulty.
Plate VIII.
Fig. 1. A preparation exhibiting various parts about the
base of the brain.
Side B. The hemisphere of the cerebellum entire; its pri-
mary bundle is seen to plunge between the facial nerve, 11,
and the auditory nerve, 9. The trigeminal nerve, 12, is en-
tirely covered by the transverse fibres of the cerebellum ; the
olivary body, a, plunges through the transverse fibres of the
cerebellum ; a portion of the transverse mass is removed to
exhibit the course of the pyramidal bundle, 1 — c, which
begins to diverge and to be augmented. The optic nerve is
in its natural position; on its outer edge the expansion of
the nervous bundles, w, w, in the inferior convolutions of the
middle lobe is seen.
Side A. A vertical section of the cerebellum guided through
the entrance of its primary bundle, and the middle of its gang-
lion, s, showing the augmentation of the primary bundle in
the ganglion, and the ramifications and subdivisions of the
nervous cords. All the transverse fibres of the annular pro-
tuberance, which coverthe trigeminal nerve, and the prolonga-
tion of the pyramidal bundle,arecut away. The continuation
of the olivary body, a, is still covered by the transverse bun-
dles. The optic nerve is raised from the crura, g, and cut
207
across at q. The pyramidal bundle is thus exposed from the
decussation, 1, to the transverse interlacement beneath the
optic nerve. The mass of gray substance seen on the oppo-
site side has been scraped off, in order to expose the two cords
of the mamillary bodies, 16, the one near the transverse in-
terlacement, 35, the other towards the fornix. The nervous
fibres that expand in the convolutions, and contribute to their
formation, are cut at h, h, between 35 and 37, on a level with
the anterior commissure, and the middle lobe is removed
altogether. The mass of gray substance of the great superior
cerebral ganglion (striated body,) and a part of the convolu-
tion slying at the bottom of the great fissure, D, between the
anterior and middle lobes, are incised in the same direction.
The way in which this collection of gray substance is divided
by the nervous bundes, P, at its internal, /, and its external,
L, parts, the mode in which fine filaments traverse the exter-
nal part of the gray mass, the manner in which the convolu-
tions, 44—45, are formed by the posterior cords of the cere-
bral crura placed before q, and the length and depth of the
great fissure, between the anterior and the middle lobes, are
all exposed in this preparation. By the removal of the mid-
dle lobe, the side of the great lateral ventricle, N, has become
visible. This ventricle is continued backwards, inwards,
and forwards, below the crura of the brain, g. Only a small
portion of the anterior lobe is cut off.
Fig. 2. The great commissure of the cerebellum (annular
protuberance,) b ; the anterior commissure, and the commis-
sure of the anterior lobe (the anterior fold of the corpus callo-
sum,) 39, are here represented.
Side A. The anterior edge of the cerebellum is removed by
a vertical cut from within outwards, to expose the convey-
ance of the uniting fibres towards the median line ; the mid-
dle and anterior lobes of the brain are entire; the optic nerve
is turned back, to exhibit the augmentation it has received
from the gray mass situated at its junction. The communi-
cation of the white fibres, 63, before the optic nerve, and in
208
front of the anterior commissure, 61, together with the sep-
tum lucidum, and the connexion of the olfactory nerve with
the inferior convolutions of the anterior lobe are also to be seen.
Side B. The lower portion of the cerebellum is removed
by a horizontal incision, 65, which passes by the deep chan-
nel seen on side A.
To show the anterior commissure, I cut the optic nerves at
their junction, reflect their two extremities, and from the
middle of the commissure I begin with the handle of the
knife to remove all the parts that cover it, taking care to in-
jure none of the fibres of the commissure itself. One part
of the convolutions, the interior of which is called foot of
the hippocampus, 29, and the anterior internal convolutions
of the middle lobes are cut off.
The brain has been left in the cranium, but as this has been
cut away above the cerebellum, and a part of the posterior
cerebral lobes, these masses have fallen out of their natural
situations ; and on the side B, the convolutions of the mid-
dle lobe have been pushed outwards, because the commis-
sure was prepared from within outwards.
Plate IX.
Fig. 1. The cranium sawed on the right side; the cut
passes vertically by the middle of the cerebral and cerebellar
hemispheres, and by the right orbit. The great union of
the cerebellum b, is cut transversely behind the exit of the
trigeminal nerve, and the external part of the cerebellum is
removed. The cerebral parts which, in pi. viii. fig. 1, were
situated between the transverse interlacement, 35,and the ner-
vous bundles, P, are taken away, so as to permit a view of
the divergence of the fibres in all directions.
By comparing this figure with the first figure of pi. viii. the
different appearances of the same parts prepared in their situ-
ations, with the support of the cranium, and deprived of
this by being taken out of the skull, will be appreciated.
209
Fig. 2! The cranium sawed perpendicularly in the middle
line of the head. All the parts which are seen in pi. vii. fig.
2, and even to the convolutions indicated by Roman letters,
are here removed by cutting or scraping, in order to show
the passage of the pyramidal bodies, 1 — c, across the annu-
lar protuberance,/, the augmentation of the diverging bun-
dles in the great inferior ganglion (optic thalamus) p, and
the course of the diverging bundles which issue from them.
The course of the bundles which communicate directly
with the anterior pyramids, is principally shown in fig. 1;
whilst the manner in which the internal and posterior con-
volutions of the hemispheres are formed by the nervous fibres
of the great inferior ganglion of the brain, (thalamus opticus,)
is seen in fig. 2.
Plate X.
Fig. 1. The brain laid on its superior surface. The side
B, exhibits the entire nervous mass of the brain and cerebel-
lum, cleft in the median line from the medulla oblongata to
the fornix and laid on the side. The gray mass behind the
junction of the optic nerves, and a small part of the pretended
thalami have been scraped away, to expose the two internal
cords of the mammillary bodies, 16, the one running towards
the transparent septum, 57, 58, 59, the other plunging into
the interior of the great inferior ganglion, p, p. Side A. The
medulla oblongata and cerebellum are cut near the annular
protuberance. At 30, a part of the median layer is removed.
to expose the continuation of the oculo-motor nerve into the
black substance. The posterior part of the great inferior
ganglion, the half of the pineal gland with two cords, and the
quadrigeminal bodies, are as on the opposite side B ; the an-
terior p°art of the great inferior ganglion is cut on to show the
prolongation of the internal posterior cord of the mammillary
bodies,* with the transverse interlacement, 36, pi. viii.; the
septum lucidum, the fornix, the internal portion of gray sub-
27
210
stance of the corpus striatum, and several internal convolu-
tions of the posterior and middle lobes, are removed to show
the great diverging bundles P, P, and the masses of union
known by the name of corpus callosum, in the great cerebral
cavities N, N. The anterior, 39, and the posterior, 40, fold ;
and the raphe of this mass are perceived. The direction of
the converging fibres from behind is forwards and inwards,
from before backwards and inwards, and from the middle
directly inwards. Lastly, the interlacing of the diverging
bundles with the fibres of union is presented.
Fig. 2. The brain of a goose laid on its upper surface.
Side B. The cerebellum and the hemisphere of the brain
entire ; the medulla oblongata cleft along the median line,
and one of its halves put aside.
Side A. The half of the medulla oblongata, its adherence
to the cerebellum, and its continuation towards the brain at
the crus are divided, to expose the communication between
the cerebellum and the optic ganglion, which is hollow, and
communicates from its anterior by means of a white layer
with the brain.
Fig. 3. The brain of a goose laid on its superior surface.
Side B. The half of the brain and medulla oblongata, as in
B, fig. 2; but the inferior and posterior part of the cerebellum
is here cleft, to exhibit its lamellar structure. The optic
ganglion and the optic nerve are taken away, to show the
continuation of the anterior commissure, 61, and the en-
trance of the crus, p, into the hemisphere.
Plate XI.
Fig. I. The human brain laid on its base; the primary
part of the cerebellum and the corpus callosum are split along
the median line. The posterior surface of the medulla ob-
longata, the posterior pyramidal bodies, the primary bundles
6f the cerebellum, and a third portion of the restiform bodies,
e — e ; the interior of the fourth ventricle, m, the communica-
211
tion of the cerebellum y, with the quadrigeminal bodies, n, o,
and their union, x, the origin of the sympathetic nerve, 13, the
pineal gland E, with its two anterior cords; the posterior com-
missure, v, the soft commissure, 46, and the anterior commis-
sure, 61, cleft. On the side A, only the fornix and the sep-
tum lucidum are removed to expose the striated bodies and
thalami lying beneath. Side B. The primary bundle of the
cerebellum and the parts, seen on the surface of side A, have
been removed by a horizontal cut from within outwards, and
from before backwards, on a level with the white fibres situa-
ted in the fourth ventricle, t. The internal portion of the an-
terior part of the brain has also been cut away by a vertical
incision, in order to expose the division of the striated bodies
into two parts, I, L, by the passage of the great bundles P.
This vertical cut is not made to begin more posteriorly, for
fear of injuring the commissures visible along the median
line. To exhibit the passage of the bundles from the medulla
oblongata across the great ganglions, and the augmentation
of the parts, the cut must be carried, pi. vi. fig. 1, through the
middle of the anterior pyramidal bodies, 1—c, through the
annular protuberance, b, the crura of the brain g, in the di-
rection of the organ IX.
Fig. 2 and 3. The brains of geese laid on their inferior
surfaces. Fig. 3. Side B. The hemisphere of the brain put
aside, and the cerebellum cleft along the median line, to show
its lamellar structure and internal cavity, 62. Side A. The
optic ganglion exposed, n ; a part of the crus and interior in-
ternal portion of the brain to the junction of the optic nerves
removed ; part of the crus and of the striated bodies seen in-
teriorly remaining.
Fig. 2. The cerebellum cleft along its interior surface, and
spread out on each side ; the pretended optic thalami on both
sides, n, and the striated bodies, I, I, on side A, are in the nat-
ural state; whilst on side B the radiated layer of cerebral
substance is visible anteriorly, P ; the anterior commissure,
61, is perceived in its full length. The posterior portion of
the hemisphere, 60, untouched.
212
Explanation of the Signs in the Figures.
A and B. Sides of preparation.
D. Convolutions at the bottom of the figure of Sylvius.
E. Pineal gland.
L. External portion of the great superior ganglion, (stria-
ted body.)
M. Third ventricle.
N. Great lateral ventricles.
P. The bundles of the striated bodies.
a. Olivary bodies.
b. Annular protuberance.
c. Entrance of the anterior pyramids under the annular
protuberance.
d. Transverse band below the pons Varolii in mammif-
erous animals.
e. Restiform bodies.
/. Passage of the anterior pyramids through the annular
protuberance.
g. Peduncles of the brain.
h. Section of the bundles which go to the middle lobe.
i. Original of the trigeminal nerve.
k. Exit of the trigeminal nerve.
I. Internal part of the great superior ganglion (striated body.)
m. Fourth ventricle.
n. Anterior pair of the quadrigeminal bodies.
o. Posterior pair of the quadrigeminal bodies.
p. Great inferior ganglion (thalamus )
q. Corpus geniculatum externum.
r. Corpus geniculatum internum.
s. Ganglion of the cerebellum (corpus dentatum.)
t. White fibres in the fourth venticle.
u. Junction of the optic nerves.
v. Posterior commissure.
w. Fibres in communication with the external edge of
the optic nerve.
x. Commissure of the quadrigeminal bodies.
213
y. Valve of Vieussens.
a. Lateral fissure of the spinal cord.
(3. Mesial edge of the spinal cord.
y. Anterior pillar of the fornix.
8. Abdominal root of the spinal nerves.
*j. Dorsal root of the spinal nerves.
5. Commissure of the spinal cord.
X. Extremity of the corpus callosum.
,u. Middle part of the corpus callosum.
cp. Aqueduct of Sylvius.
1. Decussation of the anterior pyramids.
2—3. Accessory nerve.
4. Hypoglossal nerve.
5. Mesial fissure of the spinal cord.
6. Pneumogastric nerve.
7. Glossopharyngeal nerve.
8. Ganglion of the auditory nerve.
9. Auditory nerve.
10. Abductor nerve.
11. Facial nerve.
12. Trigeminal nerve.
13. Superior oblique nerve.
14. Section of the annular protuberance.
15. Common oculo-moter nerve.
16. Mammillary body.
17. Cineritious tubercle.
18. External root of the olfactory nerve.
19. Middle root of the olfactory nerve.
20. Optic nerve.
21. Internal root of the olfactory nerve.
22. Infundibulum.
23. Bulb or ganglion of the olfactory nerve.
25—26. Anterior lobe of the brain.
26—27. Middle lobe of the brain.
27—28. Posterior lobe of the brain.
29. Convolutions, which, viewed from within, form the
pes hippocampi.
214
30. Black substance in the peduncles of the brain.
31. Commissure of the olfactory nerve in animals.
32. Semicircular band or tape-worm of Haller.
33. Transverse band at the upper edge of the annular pro-
tuberance.
34. Transverse band in the midst of the peduncles.
35. Transverse band under the optic nerve.
36. Transverse band of the bundles which go to the mid-
dle lobe.
37. Transverse band of the great superior ganglion.
38. Place where the diverging and unitingfibres decussate.
39. Anterior fold of the corpus callosum.
40. Posterior fold of the corpus callosum.
41. Upper surface of the primary part of the cerebellum.
42. Inferior surface of the primary part of the cerebellum.
43. Tubercle of the fourth ventricle in fishes.
44—45. Cerebral convolutions behind the great fissure of
Sylvius.
46. Soft or middle commissure.
47—48. Situation of the organ of amativeness.
48—49. Situation of the organ of philoprogenitiveness.
49—50. Situation of the organ of inhabitiveness.
50—51. Situation of the organ of self-esteem.
51—52. Situation of the organ of firmness.
52—53. Situation of the organ of veneration.
53—54. Situation of the organ of benevolence.
54—55. Situation of the organ of comparison.
55—56. Situation of the organ of eventuality.
57, 58, and 59, Septum lucidum.
60. Fornix.
61. Anterior commissure.
62. Interior of the nervous substance of the cerebellum.
63. White fibres, which unite with the septum lucidum.
65. Horizontal section of the cerebellum.
70. Nervous bundles of the organs of the affective faculties.
86, 87, 88, 90. Intermediate layers of fibres between the
two halves of the cerebral masses.
215
i. Organ of Destructiveness.
n. . Amativeness.
in. . Philoprogenitiveness.
IV. . Adhesiveness.
v. . Inhabitiveness.
VI. . Combativeness.
VII. . Secretiveness.
VIII. . . Acquisitiveness.
IX. . Constructiveness.
X. . Cautiousness.
XI. . Approbativeness.
XII. . . Self-esteem.
XIII. . . Benevolence.
XIV. . . Reverence.
XV. . Firmness.
XVI. . . Conscientiousness.
XVII. . . Hope.
XVIII. . Marvellousness.
XIX. . . Ideality.
XX. . . Mirthfulness.
XXI. . . Imitation.
XXII. . Organ of Individuality,
XXIII. . Configuration.
XXIV. . Size.
XXV. . . Weight and resistance.
XXVI. . Coloring.
XXVII. . Locality.
XXVIII. . Order.
XXIX. . Calculation.
XXX. . . Eventuality.
XXXI. . . Time.
XXXII. . Tune.
XXXIII. . Language.
XXXIV. . Comparison.
XXXV. . Causality.
TL.l.
Fig. 5.
Fig./f- Tig-L
tzm
■ig. /
^9. 9.
I'ub h\ M.irsh. Cup en & Zyc/n-
/'/../[-.
Fie 1.
Fiet- O
/i./-'X. ,/e/
rw< ft/ M."--h I'V", ^iu">
tl. r
Fia.h
Fig. /.,
XXX
xxn jxyii
VI.. VI.
1'i't - b\
/<>.>/. 1.
I'L.VTI.
fiff.a.
TL.VJH
Tub. by Mn'-.vh Cap en & J^y o rv.
FeTvdleU'risLith.Scsten .
VI.. IX
)
J'L.X.
Fa/1.
hu,.'J
rt'<).:'> 3
J-'u/i d hi Marsh, C'lven