TM 8-300 WAR DEPARTMENT TECHNICAL MANUAL NOTES ON EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT IN AVIATION MEDICINE V f TM 8-300 TECHNICAL MANUAL* No. 8-300 WAR DEPARTMENT, Washington, November 26, 19^0. NOTES ON EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT IN AVIATION MEDICINE Prepared under direction of The''Surgeon General Section I. General 1-8 II. Nosei 9-15 III. Maxillo facial injuries 16-20 IV. External and middle ear 21-25 V. Effect of flight on middle ear 26-37 VI. Anatomy and physiology of inner ear or labyrinth__ 38-18 VII. Vestibular nystagmus 44r-45 VIII. Vestibular vertigo 46-55 IX. Vestibular tests 56-60 X. Blind flying 61-62 XI. Miscellaneous ear conditions , 63-68 XII. General methods and equipment for eye examina- tion 69-70 XIII. Visual acuity 71-79 XIV. Depth perception 80-89 XV. Ocular movements 90-119 XVI. Accommodation 120-126 XVII. Inspection of the eye 127-138 XVIII. Color vision 139-155 XIX. Field of vision for form and colors 156-162 XX. Refraction 163-171 XXI. Ophthalmoscopic examination 172-180 Page Appendix. Abbreviations and references 270 Index *-^5 Paragraphs 271818*—40 1 1 TM 8-300 1 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Section I GENERAL Paragraph Mouth 1 Face 2 Teeth 3 Tongue 4 Ranula 5 Tonsils . 6 Pharynx 7 Larynx 8 1. Mouth.—a. General.—This organ has four main functions: It is the entrance of the alimentary canal; it aids in the mastication of food; it has an important role in speech; and it is an auxiliary entrance and exit for the respiratory system. Practically all defects which would be disqualifying are easily seen or are distinguished while watching and listening to the indi- vidual talk. Applicants with gross defects of the mouth will rarely present themselves for examination, and for this reason the inspec- tion of the mouth may frequently be neglected. h. Disqualifying defects.— (1) Harelip.—This developmental de- fect occurs in a variety of forms varying from a simple defect of the lip to a bilateral defect involving the lip, the alveolar ridge of the maxilla, and the hard and soft palates. Any extensive defect is usually accompanied by some defect in speech even when corrected. Obviously, applicants with marked defects of this nature will sel- dom present themselves for examination. Certain minor cases which have had only the lip involved, and which have been satisfactorily corrected so that the resulting scar is not ugly, the function is not interfered with, and the speech is normal are acceptable for training. (2) Loss of whole or large part of either lip or unsightly mutila- tions of lips from wounds, hums, or disease.—Such defects require little comment. They are so obvious that they will probably never be missed, but occasionally an applicant will appear who has a rather severe, unsightly scar or deformity of the upper lip camouflaged beneath a mustache; so particular attention should be paid to the mouths of applicants who have such ornaments. (3*) Tumors of lip.—Hemangioma are frequently located in the lower lip. If small and nonprogressive, they may be disregarded. The larger ones are disqualifying, both from a cosmetic standpoint and because of the possibility of severe hemorrhage following trauma. 2 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 1-2 TM 8-300 It is to be remembered that the lower lip is a very frequent site for the appearance of epitheliomas, especially in males above the fourth decade of life. The exact cause for this is not known, but overexposure to sunlight and chronic irritations are predisposing causes. Aviators, espe- cially in this part of the country, have had extreme exposure of the lips to the sun, and they are subjected to special trauma in the use of oxygen nipples. Particular attention should be paid to the in- spection of the lips in the semiannual examination as these cancers can be cured in a large percentage of cases if recognized early. 2. Face.—a. General.—Inspection of the face is done automati- cally during the examination of the eyes, nose, mouth, and ears. b. Disqualifying factors.— (1) Extreme ugliness. (2) Deformities. (a) Large birthmarks. (b) Large hairy moles. ( 271818°—40 2 17 TM 8-300 2G-27 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Section V EFFECT OF FLIGHT ON MIDDLE EAR Paragraph General 26 Anatomy 27 Physiology I 28 Terminology 29 Definition 30 Etiology 31 Symptomatology 32 Diagnosis 33 Complications : 34 Treatment 35 Pathology 36 Summary 37 26. General.—Those familiar with aviation medicine are well aware that airplane pilots suffer more frequently from disturbances of the middle ear than from all other occupational diseases combined. The phenomenal growth of commercial air transport, which carried approximately one million passengers in 1936, makes this problem of interest and importance to the general medical profession, for airplane passengers are exposed to the same influences as the pilots during flight and in most instances are much more adversely affected. Since the. deleterious effects of flight on the middle ear depend entirely on the peculiar structure and functioning of the eustachian tube, a brief anatomic, normal physiologic, and special physiologic review of the latter structure will be presented first, followed by a discussion of the new clinical entity “aero-otitis media.” 27, Anatomy.—a. General.—The eustachian tube is a slitlike, potential tube extending from the middle ear to the nasopharynx. It is formed of bone, cartilage, and fibrous tissue. h. Bony 'portion.—The bony portion begins at the upper part of the anterior wall of the tympanic cavity and. gradually narrowing, passes downward, forward, and mediad. for about 12 millimeters, ending at the angle of the junction of the squamous and petrous por- tions of the temporal bone, c. Cartilaginous portion.—The cartilaginous portion of the tube extends from the bony portion to the nasopharynx. This section is about 24 millimeters in length and is formed of a triangular plate of elastic fibrocartilage with its apex attached to the bony portion and its base placed directly under the mucous membrance of the naso- pharynx, where it forms a prominence, the torus tubarius. The upper edge of the cartilage is bent laterally and takes the form of a hook 18 TM 8-300 27 KOTES OK EYE, EAR, ETC., IK AVIATIOK MEDICIKE on cross section, open below and laterally. These walls of the canal are completed by fibrous tissue. (1. Lumen.—The lumen of the eustachian tube, is narrowest at the junction of the bony and cartilaginous portions, the isthmus, and expanding rapidly in both directions reaches its largest diameter at the pharyngeal orifice. At rest, the lumen of the cartilaginous por- tion of the tube is a vertical slit with its walls opposed. e. Tissue.—The mucous membrane of the eustachian tube is a direct extension of that of the nasopharynx and continues backward to line the middle ear completely. The mucous membrane of the bony portion of the tube is thin, but in the cartilaginous portion it is thick and very vascular, contains numerous mucous glands and is composed of ciliated columnar cells. Near the mouth of the eusta- chian tube is a variable amount of adenoid tissue known as Gerlach’s or tubal tonsil. The pharyngeal ostium of the eustachian tube is located high up on the lateral wall of the nasopharynx. This opening is triangular, bounded behind by the torus tubarius and in front by the nasal cavity. /. Musculatum.—The muscles that are attached to the eustachian tube and their actions are as follows: (1) Levator veil palatine.— (a) Origin.—Inferior aspect of the pyramidis ossis temporalis and from the lateral end of the medial lamina of the eustachian tube. (6) Insertion.—Downward medially and forward parallel to the inferior margin of the medial lamina of the eustachian tube, uniting in the soft palate with the corresponding muscle of the opposite side. (c) Action.—Elevates the soft palate, narrows the eustachian ostium, and dilates the isthmus. (2) Tensor veli palatini.— (a) Origin.—Scaphoid fossa of the sphenoid bone, lateral and membranous lamina of the eustachian tube, and angular spine of the sphenoid bone. (h) Insertion.—The fibers run downward and forward around the sulcus of the pterygoid hamulus and, radiating mediad into the soft palate, attach to the hard palate and to the corresponding muscle of the opposite side. (c) Action.—Tenses the soft palate and opens the eustachian tube. (3) Salpingopharyng&us.— (a) Origin.—Inferior part of the os- tium of the eustachian tube. (h) Insertion.—Blends with the posterior fasciculus of the pharyn- gopalatinus muscle. (c) Action.—Raises the upper and lateral parts of the pharynx; opens the ostium of the eustachian tube. 19 TM 8-300 28 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT 28. Physiology.—a. Normal.—The eustachian tube drains the middle ear and ventilates it. The motion of the cilia and the flutter- valve like action of the tube favors the motion of material from the ear to the nasopharynx and opposes motion in the opposite direction. The tube, while normally closed, is opened by contraction of its dilator muscles, and at such times any air pressure differential existing between the middle ear and the atmosphere is equalized. This contraction may occur during swallowing, yawning, and other similar physiologic acts. b. /Special.— (1) Altitude-pressure relationship.—Aircraft flights involve changes in altitude, and this in turn involves changes in atmos- pheric pressure, the relationship between the two being shown in figure 1. It is to be noted that, with ascent, equal changes of pressure in- Figure 1.—Altitude-pressure curve. volve increasing intervals of altitude. The rates and degrees of atmos- pheric pressure changes during flight depend on the rates and degrees of ascent or descent, and these factors become important when it is remembered that the ear is an air-filled, closed cavity with pressure equalization possible only when the eustachian tube is opened. (2) Results of laboratory investigations.—Laboratory investiga- tions of the physiology of the eustachian tube under marked variations of atmospheric pressure were carried out on five healthy men, pressure variation rates being from 5.4 to 27 millimeters of mercury per minute (200 to 1,000 feet per minute) through pressure ranges of from 760 to 141 millimeters of mercury (0 to 40,000 feet altitude). The results of this study were briefly as follows ; 20 TM 8-300 28 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Beginning at sea level pressure and decreasing the pressure at a constant rate, a pressure change of from 3 to 5 millimeters of mercury (110 to 180 feet altitude) was required before any effect was perceptible in consciousness. At this point there appeared a slight sensation of fulness in the middle ears and examinations showed the tympanic membranes to be slightly bulged. This bulging and the sensation of fulness increased with the decreasing of pressure until at 15 milli- meters of mercury (500 feet altitude) differential there was a sudden annoying “click” heard and felt in the middle ear, the drum snapped back to, or almost to, normal position, and the sensation of fulness disappeared. The eustachian tube had been forced open by the excess pressure in the tympanic cavity and the ear pressure relieved by a sud- den rush of air from the ear to the nasopharynx. During the remainder of the pressure decrease this cycle was repeated except that all succeeding “clicks” occurred at intervals of only 11.4 millimeters of mercury (435 feet altitude) pressure change. This in- dicated that it requires 15 millimeters of mercury excess pressure in the middle ear at sea level conditions to force the eustachian tube open, and that it remains open until the pressure is reduced to about 3.6 milli- meters of mercury, when it again closes, leaving 3.6 millimeters of mer- cury (130 feet altitude) excess pressure in the ear. It had been as- sumed that, since the pressure altitude curve is not a straight line (fig. 1), the eustachian tube would open at equal intervals of pressure but at increasing intervals of altitude during ascent. Actually the reverse was found to be true, the tubes opening at 425-foot intervals (except the first), which amounts to 11.4 millimeters of mercury pressure at sea level but only 3.5 millimeters at 40,000 feet. The explanation of this phenomenon is probably based on the fact that air of the higher alti- tudes, being less dense, passes more readily through the eustachian tubes. These figures are averages based on repeated tests. Actually there was considerable variation between individuals and in the same individual. These variations ranged from 5 to 30 millimeters at sea- level conditions, but the averages for individuals were remarkably constant. When the atmospheric pressure was increased instead of decreased, a totally different effect was obtained. Here the eustachian tube, acting like a flutter-valve, remained closed under all degrees of pressure (one subject tested to —470 millimeters of mercury pressure) and the tym- panic membrane finally ruptured. In the course of these studies, swallowing and other voluntary ef- forts to open the eustachian tubes were suppressed. However, in a subsequent series of tests it was found that opening the eutachian tubes 21 TM 8-300 28-31 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT by voluntary effort immediately equalized the ear pressure completely except that, after a negative pressure of from 80 to 90 millimeters of mercury or more had developed in the tympanic cavity, it was then impossible for the eustachian muscles to overcome the negative pressure which held the fibrocartilaginous portion of the eustachian tube tightly collapsed, and it was then necessary to decrease the atmospheric pres- sure below that point before the eustachian tubes could again be volun- tarily opened. 29. Terminology.—Since the condition being discussed is already a recognized occupational disease and seems destined to become of general professional concern, it seems logical to suggest a proper terminology. In the United States the term “aviator’s or aviation ear” has be- gun to appear in the literature, while in Germany the terms “baro- trauma” and “tonetrauma” have been suggested. The former are obviously unsuitable and the latter may be criticized as not being descriptive of the disease. We therefore suggest “aero-otitis media” (aero, combining form from the Greek app, akpos, air, + otic, Greek utlkos, pertaining to the ear, + itis, Greek -ms, inflammation of) as suitable descriptive term for the new clinical entity about to be described, and that term will be used throughout the remainder of this paper. 30. Definition.—Aero-otitis media is an acute or chronic, trau- matic inflammation of the middle ear caused by a pressure differ- ence between the air in the tympanic cavity and that of the surround- ing atmosphere, commonly occurring during changes of altitude in airplane flights and characterized by inflammation, discomfort, pain, tinnitus, and deafness. 31. Etiology.—Aero-otitis media is due to the lack of ventilation of the middle ear during changes of atmospheric pressure to the extent that the tympanic cavity is traumatized. There are two principal causes of improper middle ear ventilation: one a failure to open the eustachian tube voluntarily when necessary, the other the inability to open it. Failure to open the eustachian tubes during changes in altitude in aircraft flights is most often due to ignorance of the necessity to do so but may be due to carelessness or to being asleep or may arise from the influence of analgesics or anesthetics or from coma. The first two of these instances usually occur among inexperienced pilots and pas- sengers. the third in sleeper airplanes, and the last group on ambu- lance planes. 22 TM 8-300 31-32 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Inability to ventilate the middle ear voluntarily is much more prevalent than is generally recognized. Some of the most frequent causes of eustachian stenosis are acute and chronic infections of the upper respiratory tract, nasal obstructions, sinusitis, tonsillitis, tumors and growths of the nose and nasopharynx, paralysis of the soft palate or superior pharyngeal muscles, enlargement of the pharyngeal or tubal tonsil, inflammatory conditions of the eustachian tube or middle ear, scar tissue about the ostium of the eustachian tube following adenectomy, and malposition of the jaws. The latter two conditions have but recently been recognized. Con- siderable scar tissue about the pharyngeal ostium of the eustachian tube sometimes occurs as the result of adenectomy when the adenotome had been allowed to pass too far laterally, causing trauma or lacera- tion to the torus tubarius. The effect of malposition of the mandible in relation to stenosis of the eustachian tube was first reported by Costen1 and later applied to aviation by Willhelmy.2 They showed that in individuals with edentulous mouths, ill-fitting dental plates, marked overbite, malocclu- sion, worn or lack of molar teeth either unilateral or bilateral or with any other condition in which there was a shortening of the vertical position of the lower jaw a compression-stenosis of the eustachian tube was likely to occur from a relaxation of the surrounding soft tissues. 33. Symptomatology.—The symptoms of aero-otitis media de- pend on the duration, frequency, and severity of the trauma sustained. a. Aero-otitis media, acute.— (1) Subjective symptoms.—Positive pressures of from 3 to 5 millimeters of mercury in the middle ear are perceptible in consciousness to most individuals as a feeling of fullness in the middle ear. At about 10 to 15 millimeters of mercury pressure the feeling of fulness is distinct and somewhat annoying and affects the hearing by imparting a distant sound and a lessened intensity. Pressures between 15 and 30 millimeters of mercury usually increase the discomfort and may be accompanied by tinnitus. The latter is of a steady hissing or roaring character or crackling and snapping. In some individuals there may be actual pain and vertigo of a mild nature. Above 30 millimeters of mercury pressure in the middle ear there is increasing pain, tinnitus and vertigo, which finally becomes unbearable. 1J. B. Costen, “A Syndrome of Ear and Sinus Symptoms Dependent upon Disturbed Function of the Temporomandibular Joint,” Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngol- ogy, 43 : 1 (March), 1934. 2 G. E. Willhelmy, “Ear Symptoms Incidental to Sudden Altitude Changes and the Factor of Overclosure of the Mandible : Preliminary Report,” United States Naval Medi- cal Bulletin, 34 : 533-541 (Oct.), 1936. 23 TM 8-300 32 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT In normal cases about 15 millimeters of pressure is sufficient to force air out through the eustachian tube, which relieves the pressure in the tympanic cavity and consequently the accompanying symptoms. However, this relief is initiated by an annoying “click,” which is both felt and heard as the drum snaps back to normal position. In stenosis of the tube the pressure required to force it open varies with the degree of stenosis. In these cases the pressure may be relieved gradually over a period of time instead of instantaneously, and a greater amount of pressure remains in the middle ear after the tube has again closed. In descent, in which the atmospheric pressure is increasing and the pressure in the ear becomes negative, the symptoms are the same as already described except that the pressure is never relieved through its own force acting on the eustachian tube because of the flapper- valve like action of the latter. For this reason the greatest difficulty usually occurs during descent in aircraft, and the highest pressure differentials have been seen and studied experimentally under this condition. At about 60 millimeters of mercury negative pressure the pain in the ear is severe and resembles that of acute otitis media. The tinnitus is marked and there is usually vertigo with beginning nausea. At from 60 to 80 millimeters of mercury negative pressure the pain is very severe and radiates from the ear to the temporal region, the parotid gland, and the cheek. Still higher pressures produce agoniz- ing pain, which seems to localize not in the ear but deep in the sub- stance of the parotid gland. Deafness is marked and vertigo and tinnitus usually increase, but the latter may disappear. At between 100 and 500 millimeters of mercury pressure the tympanic membrane ruptures. This occurrence is a dramatic episode in which the patient feels “as though hit along the side of the head with a plank,” a loud explosive report is felt and heard in the affected ear, there is a sharp piercing pain on the affected side, vertigo and nausea become marked, and col- lapse or generalized shock follows. With rupture of the tympanic membrane the acute pain quickly subsides, but a dull ache persists for from 12 to 48 hours. Hearing is distinctly diminished and vertigo and nausea may persist for from 6 to 24 hours. With both positive and negative pressures, voluntarily opening the eustachian tube will immediately relieve all acute symptoms; but it is to be remembered that with a negative pressure in excess of about 80 or 90 millimeters it becomes impossible to overcome this by muscular action, and relief is obtained only by a return to a higher altitude and a pressure difference of less than from 80 to 90 millimeters of mercury. In cases in which the pressure has already produced 24 TM 8-300 32 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE trauma, opening the eustachian tube will not relieve the symptoms of that trauma, and they persist until recovery has taken place. The symptoms following trauma to the middle ear depend on the extent and duration of the trauma. Pressures that may be only uncom- fortable at first finally become painful. Moderate trauma to the ear is followed by a sense of soreness in the ears and deafness lasting from 1 to 12 hours. Severe trauma is followed by pain, deafness, vertigo, and tinnitus for from 4 to 48 hours. The pain is similar to that of suppurative otitis media, the tinnitus usually of a hissing or roaring character, the deafness of the conduction type, and qualitative as well as quantitative. (2) Objective symptoms.—The objective signs depend on the amount of trauma sustained. In mild cases the drum may appear normal except for a moderate degree of bulging or retraction when a small amount of pressure differential still persists. An increased pressure in the tympanic cavity is denoted by a bulging of the tym- panic membrane with a decrease or loss of the light reflex. A negative pressure in the tympanic cavity is denoted by a retraction of the tympanic membrane with a decrease in size and brilliance of the light reflex and an increased prominence of the short process of the malleus with a foreshortened and more horizontal handle. Following more severe trauma, the drum may be retracted or bulging as already described, and in addition there is also an inflam- mation, which in appearance varies from a slight pink tinge to an angry red. In all cases the inflammation is most marked along the larger vessels that follow the malleus handle and around the drum periphery. When the inflammation is severe it cannot be distin- guished from acute infectious otitis media and has frequently been mistaken for it. Traumatic ruptures of the tympanic membrane are usually linear and quite extensive and may involve any portion of it. The margins of fresh ruptures are red and the whole drum is highly inflamed. There is usually a small amount of blood in the external auditory canal. If the labyrinthic wall is visible through the freshly ruptured drum membrane, it is seen to be red, congested, and swollen. Audiometer tests shown a variable diminution of hearing, depend- ing on the severity of the injury. b. Aero-otitis media, chronic.—(1) Subjective symptoms.—In these cases there is a “full and stuffy” feeling in the ears and difficulty in “clearing” them. There is a partial loss of hearing, which is either unilateral or more pronounced on one side and in some instances may vary from day to day. Head noises may be present, but rarely ver- 25 TM 8-300 33-35 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT tigo or pain. The condition is worse after flights, during acute infec- tions of the upper respiratory tract, during changes of weather, and during fatigue or debilitated states. (2) Objective symptoms.—The eardrums are bulging or retracted, usually the latter. The drum membrane is dull, lusterless, and slightly thickened and the light reflex is diminished or absent. Hear- ing acuity is diminished either unilaterally or bilaterally, and in the latter case there is usually a considerable difference between the two sides. This deafness is of the conduction type, the lower tones of the scale being lost first. Rinne’s test is negative. Weber’s test is posi- tive and bone conduction is prolonged beyond the normal. Exami- nation of the ostium of the eustachian tube shows the presence of a chronic inflammatory process or a mechanical obstruction. 33. Diagnosis.—The diagnosis of aero-otitis media is simple only if the history is known. The different traumatic inflammatory stages closely resemble the various stages of infectious otitis media and, as previously stated, have frequently been mistaken for it. Likewise, chronic aero-otitis media may be easily mistaken for a chronic infec- tious middle ear process unless a history of exposure to repeated changes of altitude is obtained. 34. Complications.—While trained pilots usually try to avoid flying during periods when they have an acute infection of the upper respiratory tract, because of the discomfort and pain in the ears which almost invariably occurs, nevertheless a considerable amount of such flying has been done. It might naturally be expected in these cases that during descent the intermittent blasts of air from the pharynx to the tympanic cavity would carry with it septic material to the mechan- ically irritated tympanum and readily set up an acute inflammatory process. While it is possible or even probable that this septic mate- rial is carried into the tympanus there are no reports of such cases, 35. Treatment.—a. Prophylaxis.—Those who take up aviation as a profession are, and should continue to be, carefully tested for patency of the eustachian tubes. This may readily be done by means of the Politzer bag. Those who have a stenosis of the tube should be exam- ined for chronic infection of the ear, sinuses, nose, and pharynx, the mouth of the tube inspected for mechanical obstructions, and the eustachian tube catheterized, if necessary. When any of these condi- tions are found and corrected, it is likely that the tube will become nor- mal. Persons examined during periods of an acute infection of the upper respiratory tract should be reexamined after the infection has subsided before a final decision is made. 26 TM 8-300 35 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Probably the most useful prophylactic measure in all cases is proper instruction of the individual concerned. As long as the patency of the eustachian tube is under voluntary control there is no reason why any person in command of his faculties need experience difficulty at any rate of ascent or descent possible in present commercial aircraft. A simple explanation of the functioning of the eustachian tube followed by instructions as to how to ventilate the tympanus, when to ventilate it, and how frequently this is necessary should suffice. Probably the simplest maneuver to actuate the normal eustachian tube is to swallow. It may also be accomplished by yawning, by singing, by shouting, by autoinflation, and by contracting the salpingopharyngeal muscles. The last named defies description and can be learned only by practicing the suppression of a simulated yawn, at which time a roaring in the ears will indicate when the effort is successful. Since the average person swallows involuntarily about every 60 to 75 seconds, it can be seen that a rate of climb or descent of 200 feet per minute will usually cause no discomfort, 500 feet per minute slight dis- comfort, and 1,000 feet per minute moderate discomfort even though no effort is made to ventilate the tympanum artificially. Descents above 4.000 feet per minute may catch an individual unaware and create a tympanic vacuum which it is impossible to relieve by any method except a return to higher altitudes. Chewfing gum, eating, drinking, or inhaling oxygen reduces swallow- ing to intervals of from 1 to 30 seconds. Sleeping and comatose indi- viduals swallow at increased intervals and present a serious problem. The allowable rate of ascent and descent of commercial air lines is set by the Department of Commerce at 300 feet per minute, and some such companies limit themselves to 200 feet per minute, although unusual conditions such as weather may require that both of these rates be exceeded to insure the safety of the flight. Those who are suffering from either temporary or permanent stenosis of the eustachian tube should be enjoined from flying except under controlled conditions of gradual changes of altitude through a maximum range not to exceed 2.000 feet. Those with an acute infection of the upper respiratory tract who insist on aerial flights should be prepared by gargling hot physio- logic solution of sodium chloride or by having a detergent spray di- rected well back into the nasopharnyx followed by the instillation or inhalation of atrophine, ephedrine, or benzedrine compounds. h. Active treatment.—Relief of pain is the first consideration in acute cases. The tympanum should be gently inflated by Politzer’s method if the drum indicates the existence of either positive or nega- tive pressure. Heat, dry or wet, is very effective. The installation 27 TM 8-300 35-36 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT of copious quantities of water into the external auditory canal at from 110° to 115° F., followed by dry heat is the method of choice. Inflammation of the ostium of the eustachian tube requires treat- ment, and hot saline gargles followed by the instilling or mopping of astringents over that area will shorten the period of discomfort. In severe cases analgesics and even the injection of morphine, from one-eighth to one-fourth grain (0.008 to 0.016 gm.), may be neces- sary for the first 24 hours, c. After-treatment.—The after-treatment consists of the applica- tion of dry heat to the ear and the inhalation or instillation of as- tringents into the nasopharynx every 4 hours. A plug of cotton in the external canal seems to add to the comfort, especially during cold weather. If the condition does not subside materially in 24 hours, acute infectious disease of the middle ear should be suspected or a stenosis of the eustachian tube looked for and corrected. Ruptures of the tympanic membrane should be left alone and treated expectantly. In chronic aero-otitis media, stenosis of the eustachian tube should be looked for and treatment directed to its relief. Chronic infec- tions of the ear, tonsils, sinuses, nose, and pharynx should be con- sidered as possible causative factors and corrected. Mechanical ob- struction of the eustachian ostium or tube should be removed. If there is no apparent infection and no obvious obstruction of the tube, a malposition of the lower jaw with compression of the tube may be assumed and the jaw temporarily repositioned by the technic of Willhelmy 3 for clinical test and, if successful, permanent meas- ures applied. The eustachian stenosis having been relieved, the chronic inflam- matory process in the tube and middle ear usually subsides spon- taneously, but this may be hastened by gentle inflation of the ear or, if the congestion is marked, by the application of heat to the external ear and nasopharynx. Until the condition is entirely re- lieved, flying should be avoided as a potential source of irritation. 36. Pathology.—In acute cases the first change is a passive hyperemia of the mucous membrane of the middle ear and eustachian tube from negative pressures or an ischemia from positive pressures. On relief of the pressure in either case there follows a period of active hyperemia, the degree and duration depending on the severity ,and duration of the trauma. With high pressures actual traumatic inflammation takes place accompanied by a serous exudate. The 8 ma. 28 TM 8-300 36-38 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE mucous membrane becomes congested and swollen, the eustachian tube blocked, and the tympanic cavity a closed cavity. The epidermal layer of the tympanic membrane takes part in the reaction and becomes inflamed and may rupture. Chronic aero-otitis media depends on frequently repeated insults to the tympanic cavity in which the tympanic membrane and the mucous membrane of the middle ear and of the eustachian tube be- come chronically congested and thickened. The pain in aero-otitis media is not merely local. In severe cases it may reflexly or directly affect the facial nerve and its branches and thus produce a neuralgic-like pain which radiates over the distribu- tion of that nerve. 37. Summary.—A new clinical entity is presented which consists of a traumatic inflammation of the middle ear caused by a pressure difference between the air in the tympanic cavity and that of the surrounding atmosphere, commonly occurring during changes of altitude in airplane flights and characterized by inflammation, dis- comfort, pain, tinnitus, and deafness. Section VI ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF INNER EAK OR LABYRINTH Osseous labyrinth 38 Membranous labyrinth 39 Cochlear and vestibular branches of auditory nerve 40 Special sense organs of inner ear 41 Physiology of vestibule and semicircular canals 42 Cochlea 43 Paragraph 38. Osseous labyrinth.—Two distinct mechanisms are to be con- sidered as comprising the inner ear or labyrinth. a. The cochlea, the essential organ of hearing. b. The vestibular apparatus, a contributory organ of equilibrium or orientation consisting of the saccule, utricle, and three semicircular canals. These structures are contained within a series of little communicat- ing cavities located within the petrous portion of the temporal bone and known as the osseous labyrinth. Within the cavities of the bony labyrinth and surrounded by a supporting fluid, the perilymph, is the essential structure known as the membranous labyrinth, the cavity of which is filled with endolymph. c. Anatomically the osseous labyrinth is divided into three main portions, a central cavity, the vestibule; an anterior portion, the 29 TM 8-300 38 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT cochlea; and a posterior portion, the semicircular canals. The vesti- bule lies internal to the middle ear and but for the foot plate of the stapes would communicate with it by means of the oval window. Anteriorly it communicates with the cochlea and posteriorly with the semicircular canals. (1) Vestibule.—The oval window of the middle ear opens into the vestibule and marks the center of its outer wall. It measures 5 or 6 millimeters from before backward and approximately 4 millimeters each in width and height. On the inner vestibular wall, nearer the anterior than the posterior wall, is a slight ridge, approximately ver- tical, the crista vestihuli. In front of this is the recessus sphericus, which lodges the saccule, while behind is the recessus ellipticus, which lodges the upper part of the utricle. There are eight openings into the vestibule, as follows: (a) Five openings for the three semicircular canals. The ampullar ends of the horizontal and anterior vertical enter the vestibule through the roof, above the oval window. The ampulla of the posterior vertical canal is in the floor. Behind this, that is, in the posterior wall near the roof, is the opening of the small end of the horizontal canal. The common opening of the anterior and posterior canals is near the angle formed by the inner and posterior walls with the roof. (b) Opening of the scala vestihuli of the cochlea in the anterior and outer corner. (c) Aqueductus vestihuli. On the inner wall. (d) Maculae cribosae. Small openings in the crista vestihuli, reces- sus sphericus, and recessus ellipticus. These give passage to sacular and utricular branches of the vestibular nerve. (2) Cochlea.—The cochlea is a bony tube about 1 y2 inches in length having two and one-half coils about a central core, the modiolus, the coils resembling a snail. The diameter of the tube near the vestibular end is about 2 millimeters, this calibre rapidly diminishing after the first turn to about 1 millimeter. From the center of the base to the apex, its axis lies in a horizontal plane and is directed forward and outward. The lamina spiralis, a bony projection, passes outward from the modiolus into the lumen of the spiral canal and extends about half- way across the cochlear tube. The basilar extends from the outer edge of the lamina spiralis to the outer edge of the cochlear tube, dividing it into two channels, an inner, communicating with the middle ear through the round window, and known as the scala tympani, and an outer, opening into the vestibule, known as the scala vestihuli. These two cavities communicate with each other at the apex of the pyramid, the helicotrema. On the floor of the scala tympani near its beginning at the round window is the aquaductus cochleae, a small, 30 TM 8-300 38 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE short canal, leading to the basal surface of the skull near the jugular- foramen. The membranous canal, however, is continued inward and perforates the dura to communicate directly with the subarachnoid space. The cochlear branches of the auditory nerve pass through the core of the modiolus to reach by minute openings the osseous lamina spiralis and thence to the basilar membrane and organ of Corti. (3) Semicircular canals—(a) Number.—The semicircular canals are three in number, horizontal, anterior vertical, and posterior vertical. (b) Description.—Each semicircular canal comprises about two- thirds of the circumference of a circle, the diameter of which is 7 to 8 millimeters. They are elliptical on cross section and each canal has a slight enlargement at one end, called the ampulla. The canals are a little over 1 millimeter in diameter, except at the ampullae where they are millimeters in diameter. Both ends of each semicircular canal open into the vestibule. The narrow ends of the anterior vertical and posterior vertical join before they reach the vestibule into which tl*ey open by means of a common limb. The ampulla of the anterior canal lies at its outer end. The ampulla of the horizontal canal lies at its anterior end. The ampulla of the posterior canal lies at its lower end. The ampullated ends of all three canals lie nearer to the middle ear than do the small ends, also all are the more anterior ends. The middle portion of the horizontal canal lies exposed in the attic and aditus. It forms a prominence on the inner wall of the attic and aditus, lying just above and behind the facial nerve, and here suppurative processes in the middle ear frequently cause erosions and, as a matter of fact, this is the most common situation for a laby- rinthine fistula. The horizontal canal is a guide in the simple mas- toid operation, exposure of same indicating that the aditus has been reached. In the radical mastoid it indicates the limit to which the posterior wall of the external auditory meatus may be taken down fVithout injury to the facial nerve. (c) Position.—With head erect and chin drawn inward, the hori- zontal or external lies very nearly in a horizontal plane. The other two are both vertical, the anterior vertical lying in a vertical plane directed from within outward and forward, and the posterior vertical occupying a vertical plane at right angles with that of the anterior vertical, that is, outward and backward. It is obvious that each canal lies in a plane at right angles to the other two. The posterior verti- cal is on a much lower plane than the anterior vertical. 31 TM 8-300 38-39 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT It has been noted from a large number of corrosion specimens that the planes of the canals, as well as the angles between them, are very inconstant. The horizontal canal is rarely horizontal. Its plane is tilted downward and backward and forms with the horizontal plane an angle varying from 0° to 30°. This fact is important in inter- preting the results of functional examination of the static labyrinth. The angle which the plan© of the anterior vertical canal makes with the medial plane, varies between 30° and 65°. There may be a differ- ence of 20° between the right and left sides. The angle between the anterior vertical and horizontal canals varies between 65° and 90°. The angle between the horizontal and inferior vertical canals is most constant, varying from 90° to 100° and being usually 90°. The anterior vertical canal of the right side is parallel to the pos- terior vertical of the left side, and the anterior vertical of the left side is parallel to the posterior vertical of the right side. 39. Membranous labyrinth.—a. General.—The membranous labyrinth consists of— (1) The membranous vestibule, which is divided into the saccule and the utricle. (2) Three membranous semicircular canals, namely, horizontal, anterior vertical, and posterior vertical. (3) The membranous cochlea. The membranous labyrinth is partly surrounded by a supporting fluid, the Perilymph, partly because in most regions it is connected at some points with the endosteum lining the walls of the osseous labyrinth. On account of the perilymph separating it, in many places, from the osseous canal, it is necessarily smaller than the osseous portion. Wherever the labyrinth filaments of the auditory nerve perforate the bony capsule to reach the membranous labyrinth, the membranous parts so supplied are attached to the bony surface thus perforated, for example, cristae acusticae of the ampullae and the maculae acusticae of the utricle and saccule. The perilymph space communicates with the subarachnoid space of the skull through the aquaductus cochleae, which opens on the basilar surface of the petrous portion of the temporal bone, internal to the jugular fossa. The cavities and canals of the membranous labyrinth are filled with endolymph. The endolymph space does not communicate with the subarachnoid space of the skull as is the case with the perilymph space. b. Membranous vestibule.—The membranous vestibule consists of the saccule, in the recessus sphericus, and the utricle, in recessus ellipticus. 32 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 39 (1) Saccule.—The saccule communicates with the scala vestibulae and scala media (ductus cochlearis) of the cochlea through the canalis reuniens and only indirectly with the utricle. It lies in front of the crista vestihuli, being partly separated from the utricle by this ridge. The recessus sphericus and the anterior surface of the crista ves- tibuli present numerous small perforations through which the sac- cular branches of the vestibular nerve pass. Penetrating the con- tiguous surface of the saccule, they present a circumscribed bulging of the inner wall known as the macula acustica. The canal reuniens, a membranous canal, passes downward from the lower part to enter the ductus cochlearis of the cochlea. Another small canal from the posterior part passes downward and backward to unite with a similar canal from the utricle, aquaductus vestibuli, or aqueduct of the endo- lymph. This is the only communication between the saccule and utricle. (2) Utricle.—The utricle, about 5 millimeters in length, is attached to the posterior part of the inner wall of the bony vestibule. Its upper half is lodged in the recessus ellipticus, behind the crista vesti- buli, The portion resting against the recessus ellipticus, the recessus utriculi, is perforated by numerous small foramina for the passage of the utricular branches of the vestibular nerve forming on its inner wall a circumscribed bulging which is the highly specialized neuro- epithelial end-organ, the macula acustica. The cavity of the utricle communicates by five openings with the three semicircular canals. The ampullar ends of the horizontal and anterior vertical open on its roof, the ampullar end of the posterior vertical perforates its floor, and the opening of the small end of the horizontal canal and the common opening of the two vertical canals enter the posterior wall. From the lower end of the utricle a small membranous tube passes forward, inward, and downward. This unites with a similar tube from the saccule to form the aquaductus vestibuli. The aquaductus vestibuli enters a small bony opening on the inner wall of the bony vestibule and traverses the bone in a curved direction inward and somewhat backward to emerge by a slit-like opening upon the posterior surface of the petrous portion of the tem- poral bone, seven-eights millimeter behind the internal auditory meatus. It here expands into a closed sac, the saccus endolym- phoaticus, with no direct communication with the cerebrospinal chan- nels or subarachnoid space. The perilymph, as previously stated, escapes through the aquaductus cochlea to mingle directly with the cerebrospinal fluid. c. Membranous semicircular canals.—(1) Membranous semicircular canals are elliptical’ on transverse section and are attached to the 271818°—40 3 33 TM 8-300 39 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT greater circumference of the bony tubes. The peripheral portion of the ellipse is fixed to the periosteal lining of the bony canal, while the opposite part is free, except that it is connected by irregular bands, the ligament labyrinthi canaliculorum, which pass through the peri- lymph space to the bony wall. Like the bony canals, each mem- branous canal is dilated at one extremity into an ampulla, which is especially developed toward the concave aspect of the tube. While the membranous ampullae nearly fill the corresponding portions of the bony tubes, the calibre of the remaining parts of the membranous canals is equal to only about one-fourth of that of the osseous canals. (2) Histology.—Histologically there is a great similarity in the membranous semicircular canals, the utricle, and the saccule. They are composed of three layers, an outer fibrous layer, a delicate basal membrane, and a single layer of squamous epithelial cells. The fibrous layer is similar to the periosteum lining the bony canals and vestibule with which it is continuous, at the points where the mem- branous canals are attached to the bone. The basal membrane is homogeneous and very thin. This as well as the connective tissue layer becomes thicker at the maculae and cristae. As we approach the maculae and cristae the squamous epithelium becomes columnar and in the end organs changes into ciliated neuro-epithelium. (3) Overhanging the ciliated neuro-epithelial cells is a structure containing a mass of minute crystalline grains of carbonate of lime imbedded in a gelatinous substance. This structure is known as the otolith membrane where it overhangs the maculae acusticae in the vestibule and as the cupula where it overhangs the cristae acusticae in the semicircular canals. It is claimed by some authors that the otolith membrane and the cupula are attached to, or are a part of, the walls of the vestibule and semicircular canals, opposite the neuro-epithelial cells and is not a free membrane. d. Membranous cochlea.— (1) The membranous cochlea consists of— {a) Basilar membrane. (b) Reissner’s membrane. (I 8-300 61 (6) Cat with one ear not working plus absent cerebellar hemisphere of opposite side.—(The “cerebellum” is the “small brain”; it coordi- nates movements.) Indefinite but generally poor, both in delayed turnings and variable landings. (7) Cat with one ear not working plus absent cerebellar hemisphere of same side.—Same result, (8) Cats with removal of one cerebellar hemisphere without oper- ation on either ear.—Turn over very well, both with eyes open and blindfolded. Performance almost as exact as that of normal cats. v. Conclusion.-—When these studies were started it was known that the ear played a part; when they were completed it was found that the ear is really the answer. When the cats were actually dropped very little could be told. It all went too fast. But the slow-motion pictures were so beautifully taken that each animal could be seen to turn over and then slowly drift to the mat. The normal animals, blindfolded, turn over just as promptly as when their eyes are open and then float downward until they make a perfect landing. But these same cats, after their ears are operated upon, do not turn over at all, even when their eyes are open. On descending they roll over and over and make no attempt to right themselves. So the importance of the ear in sensing motion was realized. The complete “feel of the ship,” which is the “sense com- plex” that makes for a first-class pilot, requires internal ears that are working. w. Vestibular sense.—From the pilot’s standpoint it is all very interesting for him to know how important his ears are in his detec- tion of motion. He realizes that he is constantly receiving im- portant information from his ears (without being in the least con- scious of it). But it is so much more important for him to under- stand his vestibular sense because of the illusions. These illusions have disturbed every man or woman who has ever gone up into the air. All pilots have come to disregard them so long as they can see. The serious thing is for the pilot not to understand these illusions when he can’t see. But while one studies “how the ear can fool one” he must always keep in mind that it is the normal ear that is capable of giving the illusions. But the eye or ear is not merely an organ that may at times give illusions. They are both constantly giving good and helpful information, invaluable in seeing, in hearing, and in sensing motion. x. Conception of airplane.—It was originally thought that the air- plane was an automobile with wings, that this contrivance simply went up in the air instead of running along on the ground, and 61 TM 8-300 61 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT that, relying upon senses, one could drive it just as he was accustome'd to drive an automobile. The change in the attitude of flight sur- geons as well as pilots during the succeeding months and years came with experience. They came to realize that the airplane is not an automobile, but a ship—a ship navigating the ocean of the air. One of the great reasons for advancement and safety in flying is this change in mental attitude toward it. y. Birds and Mind flying.—Any overconfident pilot who still feels that he can rely upon himself and his own senses in meeting all the conditions in the air is in danger. He is apt to come to grief. Through all the ages the bird has been meeting the conditions in the air. He has internal ears of tremendous size and he has used these and the parts of the brain that are connected with the ear through countless experiences from the time when the first bird took to the air. Yet a bird always avoids a fog or a cloud. A bird is not found flying in the clear air above the clouds—no doubt because he did not wish to fly up through them. In short, even the bird is not at his ease when he is flying blind. z. History of 'precision instruments.—(1) The answer to the prob- lem of blind flying is instruments of precision. Much time passed before instruments of precision were available for the airplane. Through all the early years most aviators felt that their natural instincts were better guides for operating their craft than the best instruments available. Years were required to convince the majority of fliers that their old belief was wrong and the cause of so many fatal crashes. Wilbur and Orville Wright were conspicuous excep- tions. They designed the first instrument ever used. It was a string about 8 inches long. This cord was fastened in front of the pilot with one end swinging free. So long as this string pointed directly at the pilot’s nose the ship was flying without slipping or skidding. This was in 1912. In 1814 they brought out a pendulum bank-indi- cator, and also an accurate rate-of-climb indicator. These two were used in conjunction with the earlier string. But they found it diffi- cult to get even their own student pilots to use these instruments because of the humiliation when other aviators would say that the sudents of the Wright Brothers found it necessary to use instruments in flying. In other words, for many years instruments were not popular. Fliers took pride in scorning them. (2) Until the World War began in 1914, airplanes carried few instruments. These as a rule were crude; and air activity ceased dur- ing foggy weather. The reverse will probably be true in future wars, and most of the flying will purposely be done in bad weather. 62 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 61 (3) Blind flying had its real beginning for our country in 1917 when a “turn indicator” was produced. That year, with the aid of this instrument, a fog flight over the Allegheny Mountains from Washington, D. C., to Ohio was made. This turn indicator was described by its inventors as a “crutch” for the magnetic compass. (4) The modern instruments include the Sperry gyro-horizon, the directional gyro, the turn-and-bank indicator, the rate-of-climb, the rate-of-speed, the sensitive altimeter, and the flight integrator, now being perfected. It is the combined use of all these instruments that stabilizes the mind of the pilot. Each instrument makes its own con- tribution. The end result is. that the pilot has a definite conception of his position in relation to the external world. In other words, all of the precision instruments combine to tell the pilot his position and his relation to the horizon. (5) These instruments are so reliable that one would think it a simple matter for anyone to rely upon them and fly safely in any kind of weather. To this day, however, many a pilot is sending his instruments back to the manufacturer to have them corrected—only to have the instruments returned with the information that they are in perfect condition. In brief, such a pilot does not trust his instru- ments, because his own sensations conflict with what the instruments tell him. All his anxiety is unnecessary. aa. Ear deaths.—If one analyzes the disasters of modern aviation, he cannot fail to be impressed with what has come to be an old story: “bad weather,” “fog,” “storm”—in brief, blind flying. During the World War each crash was investigated by a committee consisting of the commanding officer, the officer in charge of flying, and the flight surgeon. The results of all of these crash reports were studied and tabulated. Many of the accidents were caused by weakness of the airplane and motor failure. Certain airplanes burned in the air and in those days there were no parachutes. In addition many crashes were due to what was even then termed “ear deaths.” There is a great contrast today. Structural defects and motor failures are practically unknown. The “ear deaths” still occur. db. Illusions.— (1) In the railroad station one is sitting at the win- dow of the car looking out. Suddenly his train starts to go forward. He rushes out to the platform to wave good-bye to friends, but the friends are still there and so is the platform. He was not moving; it was another train that was coming toward him. Immediately he is at his ease. He thoroughly understands the situation. He knows where he is and why. 63 TM 8-300 61 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (2) Now consider the illusions that may come from the ears, par- ticularly in blind flying. In a fog, after the pilot has rotated in the air and the ship straightens out, his own head is no longer actually turning. But anyone who knows the first thing about the ear knows why he feels that he is rotating in the opposite direction. In other words, in the railroad station one has an illusion from his eyes, and now one has an illusion from his ears. He feels that he is going in the opposite direction when he is not. He has “vertigo”—a sensation of movement contrary to fact. All he needs to do is to look at the instruments in front of him and believe in them. Immediately he dis- regards the feeling that he is turning. He pays no attention to his feelings. He understands them. The sensation in the head of the pilot is exactly the same as when one comes out of the railroad train and looks at the platform. The illusion is gone. He knows the facts of his position. ac. Buggies orientator.—In 1918 the Buggies orientator was a great step in advance. It is, so to speak, a turning-chair that re- volves in every direction. The pilot sits in a cockpit and rotates it in every possible direction. This was, and is, a great help, because it enables the pilot to get used to these queer sensations while he is safely on the ground. ad. Training.—The pilot should be trained to understand his ears. First he taught with the “instrument box on the turning-chair.” Then he is instructed in actual flight “under the hood.” * (1) Instrument hox on turning-chair.— (a) The “instrument box on the turning-chair” is the key to blind flying. The principle is to show the pilot how to understand his ears and his instruments at the same time. The procedure is simple. The pilot is rotated—prefer- ably in the presence of other pilots. He is turned to the right and then the chair is stopped. He calls out, “I am turning left, to the left.” He is then turned very rapidly to the right, and then slowly to the right. He may say, “I am not moving,” or “I am turning to the left,” whereas all the observers assure him he is turning to the right. (h) The pilot is then told to look into the instrument box. A flashlight in the box shows a turn-and-bank indicator and a compass. As he looks at the instruments he is again rotated. He watches the instruments while he is being rotated. He says, “I am turning right; the indicator also shows I am turning right.” When the speed of the chair is slightly reduced, he will say, “The indicator shows I am turn- *Blind Flight, by Lt. Col. William C. Ocker and Capt. Carl J. Crane, published in 1932 by the Naylor Company, San Antonio, Texas. 64 TM 8-300 61 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE ing right; my senses tell me that I have stopped,” or “The indicator says that I am turning to the right, but I feel that I am turning to the left.” (c) It frequently happens that the pilot who has just had this dem- onstration will argue that his sensations are correct, in spite of what the instruments tell him. In that case it is helpful to have him stand by and watch someone else go through the same performance. As a rule a few such experiences in the turning-chair will convince the pilot that he can rely upon the instruments. His thought then is “Oh, yes, I have that feeling of turning, but I am not actually turn- ing.” From that moment his problem is solved. (2) Hood.—(a) The next step is actual flight under the hood. Such instruction requires more time, largely because of the element of fear. But after the pilot has mastered flying under the hood he no longer has any fear or apprehension when he suddenly enters a cloud. The most difficult fliers to train are the old veterans. Blind flying should be taught early—the sooner the better. As soon as a pilot is at his ease in flying blind he has then mastered the situation. When he suddenly enters a cloud he feels at home. When he is able to see, so much the better. (5) To teach the pilot to interpret his instruments and respond automatically to what the instruments tell him, a hood was put over the cockpit in the Buggies orientator, which can be “flown” by either the pilot inside or his instructor outside. The orientator ro- tates the pilot in every conceivable manner; the more recent Link trainer has more limited movements, but it duplicates many of the motions of the airplane—the turns, banks, glides, and climbs. After he gets out of the orientator or the trainer, the pilot can see how well he responded to his instruments, because his performance is recorded on a chart. In addition to becoming familiar with the in- struments the pilot is also receiving messages through his earphones and learns to interpret the direction signals by radio. This not only helps him to fly blind but instructs him in “avigation,” which means the navigation of the air. When flying blind, the pilot must not only be able to keep the plane in proper position but must be able, by avigation, to arrive at his destination. This is best accomplished by radio messages which guide him to the proper place. ae. Sperry gyro pilot.—There is one motion-sensing device that is perfect. It detects changes of motion accurately. It has absolutely no illusions. This device is the Sperry gyro pilot. It is now in use on most of the air lines everywhere. The attractive feature of the gyro pilot is that it is automatic. The human element is eliminated. The gyro pilot not only detects motion—it actually operates the 271818°—40 6 65 TM 8-300 61-62 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT controls and holds the airplane in correct flying attitude. About 75 percent of all air-line flying in the United States is now done by the gyro pilot. af. Ignorance of 'pilots concerning ear.—A thorough knowledge of the ear has been available to the pilot since 1918. Of course, fliers would not see, or understand, the medical books; but it is hard to understand why, in some way, they have not become acquainted with at least the simple facts that have been known for such a long time. To this day, out of one thousand pilots there is perhaps only one who knows that he has a vestibular part of the ear. Any intelligent pilot simply needs to be told the following: Remember that just because the eye can fool one, it does not mean that one does not need his eyes. And just because the ear can fool one, it does not mean that one does not need his ears. It is highly desirable to have good eyes; and in flying more than in any other occupation one will be helped by nor- mal motion-sensing impulses from the ears. With all one’s organs and senses, the nearer one is to a full normal the better it is for him. One must simply come-to understand his ears, not only for the correct information which they give, but for the incorrect information which they may give, when flying blind. In clear weather the ear helps to give the “feel of the ship.” In a fog, after one comes out of a spin and the ship straightens out, he feels that he is spinning in the op- posite direction. If the pilot simply looks at his instruments and believes in them, he will know the facts of his position. He will realize that his sensations are contrary to fact. In brief, just as the eyes can fool one, so the ears can fool one. But the difficulty is that although one knows perfectly well what the eyes are for he does not understand his ears. The ear is thought of as the motion-sensing organ that receives impulses from without—what is called hearing. But it is the other part of the internal ear, the part that senses the motion of the airplane and the pilot, that is far more important to understand. Knowledge of this little organ may make all the differ- ence between safety and disaster. A pilot will be much better and safer if he studies and understands this little motion-detector in his internal ear. 62. Medical contribution.—a. Conception of term.—Many avia- tors and most laymen have no proper conception of the term “blind flying.” To the vast majority it simply means flying when the pilot cannot see things clearly. To the properly trained aviator, the ability to do blind flying, and to understand the basic underlying human reactions governing the same, means the difference between life and death. 66 TM 8-300 62 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE b. Popular education,—The pioneer medical research work accom- plished during 1926 firmly established the basic principles from the human standpoint on which the art of blind flying is founded. A re- port of this research and the conclusions arrived at were placed on file in the office, Chief of the Air Corps, War Department, in 1926. Fol- lowing the filing of this report, articles began to appear in many popular and technical magazines, newspapers, etc., on “blind flying.” These articles were prepared by “feature writers”, usually after an interview, and were released for the purpose of bringing this new dis- covery in aviation to the attention of the public and to create an in- terest in the matter by aviators in general. Naturally, the dramatic side was emphasized. Many lectures and demonstrations before civic bodies, clubs, and technical societies were given. In all of the articles published and the lectures and demonstrations given, the basic prin- ciples discovered were freely discussed and explained, resulting in a more or less general understanding by the public of the underlying physiological reactions experienced in blind flying. c. Definitions.— (1) Blind flying.—Research work attains value of importance only when its results can be directly applied to either clinical findings or practically used in some one of the vocations of life. In order that the relationship between blind flying and the research accomplished may be understood, it is essential to know what blind flying is. Blind flying is that flying accomplished in which any visible reference to the earth for the purpose of recog- nition of position is impossible by reason of fog, storms, dust, com- plete darkness, thick clouds, etc. (2) Simulated blind flying,—Simulated blind flying is that flying in which visual reference to the earth for the purpose of recogni- tion of spatial position is limited to the pilot’s cockpit and its equipment by means of proper coverage of the cockpit. Simulated blind flying is that flying accomplished by visual reference to the instruments installed in the airplane; therefore, the proper term is “instrument flying” or, as it is expressed by airmen, “flying under the hood,” d. Efforts to fly.—Since the beginning of time and until recent years mankind has traveled almost entirely on the surface of the earth or the oceans. Man’s invasion of the air began when small animals attached to balloon? were sent into the air to ascertain if life were possible above the earth’s surface. They all returned safely to earth, with the exception that one suffered a broken leg, thus establishing the fact that this contemplated flying era would produce its category of ills and problems for the doctors to treat 67 TM 8-300 62 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT and solve. From this time to the epoch-making flight of the Wrights at Kitty Hawk mankind continued his efforts to fly. From the Wrights at Kitty Hawk to the present space-annihilating air- plane is a long way in terms of transportation. It has been a much longer way in terms of adapting the human body to the changes and the solution of the medical problems presented. e. Special senses.—The present generation is the first to move freely in three dimensions asi do the birds and fishes. The evolution of mankind had reached a more or less fixed stage when aviation was) born, and, as flying was not concerned in this evolution, mankind developed into an “earthbound” entity. Every human being, in arriving at his present state, has had one common factor in his de- velopment—contact with mother earth. Individually each is noth- ing more than the sum total of his experience stored up in the brain, plus the body he lives in. These experiences are obtained for storage as the result of the action of our special senses: sight, hearing, smell, taste, and touch. Two more, vitally important to the aviator, are muscle sense and vestibular or kinetic-static sense. /. Fwnction of senses.—Stimulation of any of these senses arouses action, and action eventually results in consciousness, a knowledge of environment, and a perception of the physical facts constituting that environment. It is apparent that all stored up experience has had a common stimulator, contact with the earth and its material objects. Experience, based on special senses, has taught one how to adjust himself to various environments. The lessons learned from experience are indelibly impressed in the consciousness. One of the earliest lessons learned is how to maintain equilibrium. Undoubt- edly equilibrium is a bodily function maintained by the action of all of the special senses. Some of these senses enter very little into this maintenance; taste, smell, and hearing have little, if any, effect; tactile sense or touch, used in connection with muscle sense, has a little more effect. Chiefly, if not entirely, our equilibrium is main- tained by means of a coordinated cooperation of sight, muscle sense, and vestibular sense. By the use of this “trinity sense” one is able to maintain and realize position, rate, and direction of motion, and generally orient himself in relation to the earth, g. Equilibrium..—(1) Man’s equilibrium on the grovmd consists of the ability to maintain his body in any position it is possible or desir- able to put it. Man’s equilibrium in the air consists of the ability to maintain an airplane, which he has become a part of, in any posi- tion it is possible or desirable to put it, plus the added factor that all contact with the earth is entirely lost except for two things, sight 68 TM 8-300 62 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE and the column of air the plane is flying in. Human sensations, reac- tions, and consciousness having developed after countless years of con- tact with the earth, it is not possible for man to invade the air and safely function using the prior experiences stored up in his brain by a set of special senses evoluted in contact with the earth. (2) Stabilized equilibrium, either in the air or on the ground, is maintained only when each of the trinity of senses—sight, muscle sense, and vestibular sense—functions correctly, and their stimulations are correctly interpreted by the. brain. Some equilibrium and orienta- tion may be present with all three imperfectly acting. Adjustment to environment will take place if two remain unimpaired. The com- binations of impairment, function, and adjustment are many.- All compensatory equilibrium or adjustment to environment, however, has been developed on the ground, and in taking to the air we separate ourselves from the universal common factor, earth contact, and attempt to function with a set of senses and stored up experience developed for earthbound use only. h. Interpretation of senses.—Since time began human beings have been receiving sensations in their brains and storing them away for future use and guidance when the same situation again arises. In the process of storing away these sensations the brain has accomplished many seemingly weird things. The image of objects on the retina is upside down; yet the brain properly interprets, and we actually see right side up. That portion of our equilibrium sense having its origin in the vestibular apparatus is stimulated into action by body motion. This may be accomplished by motion of the body itself or motion of any object with which the body is in contact. This mov- ing body need not be in contact with the earth to have this sense stim- ulated into action. One walks, runs, rides a merry-go-round, rides in an airplane, and by use of equilibrium sense is able to maintain any possible or desirable position. Although the three senses—sight, muscle sense, and vestibular sense—are bound into one combination sense (the equilibrium triplets), each of them may be brought into separate action, and send its messages to the brain. One can “muscle sense” his position without sight. He can “sight” his position with- out the aid of the others.. Acting alone, the “vestibular sense” will give the brain information regarding motion, rate of motion, and di- rection of motion. i. Reliability of equilibrium triplets.—(1) Sight is the reliable one of the “triplets.” “Muscle sense” is an alert, variably acting “triplet.” The bad actor of the three is the vestibular sense. This sense, constantly alert, delicately sensitive, and easily stimulated 69 TM 8-300 62 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT into action, must be continually checked up on and kept under con- trol by the other two of the combination if proper and continuous equilibrium is maintained. If the body on the ground or the body and an airplane in the air be turned, as in a spin, all three of these senses, acting in coordination, will give reliable information regard- ing body motion and position both on the ground and in space. Ground equilibrium and spatial orientation will be complete. (2) Sight is the same in the air as on the ground. Muscle sense (the so-called seat sense of the airman) cannot be as good in the air as on the ground, having been developed by years of contact with the earth and its material objects. Automatically on taking to the air one is robbed of a great portion of his muscle sense. All airmen have developed “seat sense”—some to a high degree, and most of them have that essential thing, “the feel of the ship.” No false impressions are received from sight and muscle sense. They may be much lessened in case of lack of proper vision or poorly developed muscle sense. Their messages to the brain, "whether jointly or separately, are reliable. This is not true of the messages re- ceived from the vestibular sense. If your body is rotated in any dimension of space, certain definite and fixed messages will be sent the brain by the vestibular sense acting in coordination with sight and muscle sense. Provided this body motion is not so violent or long-continued as to produce loss of perception, the brain will receive reliable information from this trinity of senses and one will maintain his equilibrium and know accurately at all times what position his body occupies with relation to the earth’s surface and in what direction, if any, it is moving. If, during this rotation, the body is stopped or retarded, one will have a momentary sensa- tion of giddiness, but will immediately, by the use of sight, adjust himself to the earth (gravity) and maintain equilibrium. (3) Whenever the human body is rotated in any dimension of space, with the eyes closed, thus removing vision from the trinity of sight, muscle sense, and vestibular sense, and this rotation is re- tarded, stopped, reversed, or continued until rotating body motion is coincident with the motion of the fluid in the vestibular (semi- circular) canals, just as definite and fixed messages will be sent the brain as if the eyes were open and vision intact, but each and every one of these messages will be false. One will be able to cor- rectly interpret the original starting motion only. He will recall that the vestibular apparatus is primarily composed of three tiny sets of fluid-filled canals placed at right angles to each other in the 70 TM 8-300 63 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINI labyrinth, in the sagittal, coronal, and horizontal planes, thus pro- viding motion-sensing in any dimension of space. j. Experiment.—Now consider a practical experiment in order to visualize what happens when vision is absent and the body is ro- tated. Any smoothly running revolving mechanism will do. A barber chair is ideal. Flight surgeons use the Jones-Barany chair. In such a mechanism only right or left rotation is possible; there- fore only the action of the horizontal motion sensing vestibular canals can be demonstrated in this chair. In an orientator any of the spatial positions may be assumed, and the corresponding canals tested. The results are the same in any case. If there is such a thing as acquiring immunity, the horizontal canals, being the ones in constant daily use, should be less sensitive than the others. It was found that there was no difference- and that constant use did not affect the sensitivity. With the eyes covered, rotation at any average speed is started. One will immediately and correctly inter- pret right or left rotation Having always experienced this same sensation during prior experience of body motion to the right or left, one will be positive as to what is happening. The number of rota- tions should be limited in order that too violent reactions will not be produced. If on the sixth to eighth rotation the motion of the chair is now retarded to a slow speed, one will have an immediate sensation of turning in the opposite direction, and a message will be sent the brain to that effect by the vestibular apparatus, which is now acting without its “control coordinator,” sight. One will be positive his body is now turning in the opposite direction of prior motion. If the rotating chair is now stopped, the s&nsation of opposite turn- ing will be much intensified. This sensation of opposite turn- ing will last from 5 seconds to as high as 25 seconds in some indi- viduals. The average is about 23 seconds. This average was estab- lished after examining hundreds in the Jones-Baraiiy revolving chair during the research conducted. Every human being has his own individual threshhold of vestibular stimulation and reaction. If the rotation of the body is continued (usually 8 to 10 turns) until the motion of the body and the motion of the fluid in the semi- circular canal being stimulated is the same speed, the vestibular sense will apparently be put out of action and the brain will receive an immediate message that all motion has ceased and that the body is sitting still. Tc. Vertigo.—[Repeated reversal of rotation, without stopping, creates an utter confusion of motion sensing. What has happened? Vertigo (to turn or turning) has been produced. Vertigo consists 71 TM 8-300 62 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT of two things, a sensation of turning in the opposite direction to prior motion, and a sensation of falling in the same direction as prior motion. Vertigo is medically defined as “the subjective sensation of a dis- turbed relationship in space.” l. Effect on flying.—It will now be realized that the pioneer avia- tor dependent on messages sent to his brain by his vestibular sense, acting without the “control coordinator, vertigo stopper” sense of sight was in a dangerous predicament. The above is exactly what happens in blind flying. All visual ref- erence to the earth or any object the prior position of which in relation to the earth’s surface is part of the consciousness is absent, and only muscle sense and vestibular sense remain. As a matter of fact, a real blind man would function better in this predicament, because after all there is no difference in not being able to see cmy- thing and not being able to see, except that the real blind would have usually developed compensatory equilibrium to a high degree. m. Immunity.—All of the published literature and the research work accomplished prior to 1926 concentrated on one idea, the find- ing of a way of producing immunity against vertigo by means of placing pilots in a freely movable, revolving apparatus, and turn- ing their bodies through the various spatial positions in the belief that constant repetition of motion would establish an immunity. Every old-time pilot has had “a ride” in the Jones-Barany chair and most of them have spent hours in some type of revolving orien- tators. Nothing came of all this except to establish the fact that pilots who could expertly handle an orientator were possessed of a high degree of muscle sense and a keen sense of perception. No immunity was obtained against the reactions experienced in that deadly enemy of the airman—the spin. No immunity was possible from a physiological or a technical standpoint because no research or experiment conducted had arrived at a solution of the problems involved. There was no scientific basis established on which to build instructions to airmen in the practical application of these perfectly normal physiological reactions experienced when doing blind flying. n. Vertigo as an entity.—Vertigo is one of the oldest symptoms found in medical literature and when clinically present is believed to have a pathological basis. No study of vertigo as a purely phys- iological entity, to establish it as a causative factor in the behavior of human beings in their adjustments to their environment, has been found in the medical literature prior to 1926, and none is believed to exist. Induced physiological vertigo (that is, vertigo without 72 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-3UU 62 pathological basis) may be a common causative factor in human behavior in vocations other than flying. The automobile, swiftly and freely movable in any horizontal plane, subjects the occupants to many of the factors that tend to produce “induced vertigo.” o. Universality of reactions.—All vertigo reactions follow certain definite and fixed lines and all humans are subject to the same reac- tions varying only in intensity. A human being in whom these re- actions are absent or distorted is abnormal and has either met with a physical disaster or was bom without properly functioning sense apparatus. In such cases some of the normally acting senses must compensate for this lack in order that the individual may avert dis- aster and simulate normal control in following the ordinary rou- tines of life. p. Aviation history.—In order to understand how and why this research was started it is necessary to digress into aviation history. From the beginning of man’s invasion of the air, fog, the universal menace to travel of all kinds, has taken a great toll of human life. Old- time pilots learned to fly without artificial instrumental aids of any kind, and developed to a remarkable degree what -was called “flying sense.” Until long after the War flying for any length of time without sight of earth or sky was impossible. About 1918 a “turn” indicator was invented. Since that time many artificial aids to spatial orientation have appeared. Bank-and-turn indicators, artificial horizons of various types, flight integrators, etc. All of these instruments are gyro-controlled to overcome the pull of gravity and allow their “indicators” to show ship movement and spatial position. Two of the common ones in use are illustrated in figure 3. The Sperry artificial horizon consists of a miniature gyro-controlled airplane which will assume the position of the carrying plane -while in flight. The pilot controls the position of his airplane by visualizing wdiat the miniature plane is doing, and correcting his position accordingly. The bank-and-turn indicator mounted at bottom has a pointing arrow which indicates all turns of the plane, and a small ball, seen in the runway at the tip of the arrow, which moves from side to side when the plane is banked up in a turn to right or left. Every artificial aid to spatial orientation lets the pilot “mentally” keep one foot on the ground. » The original intent of these instruments was to improve technical flying ability by showing pilots when smooth turns and banks were being made and to function as a crutch to the magnetic compass. All 73 tm y-aoo 63 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Figure 3,—Spatial orientation indicators. 74 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TjVE 8-300 62 of the old-timers were, however, taught to fly by instinct, and the terms “inherent flying ability,” “seat sense,” and “flying sense” came into use. Artificial aids showing them when their ships were turning or banking were considered as entirely useless, as all they had to do was look at the earth and see what their plane was doing. So, although all planes were later equipped with indicators, they went unserviced and unused. Unfortunately for many, the visibility did not always stay good on every flight, so that pilots could look overboard at God’s horizon and see what their ships were doing. When visual contact with the earth was obliterated they discovered that it was impossible to maintain their planes in the air. If the fog was not completely on the ground, they would come down under it and “hedge hop” over trees and houses in a desperate attempt to keep visual contact with the earth until they could find a place to land. Only extreme skill and good luck brought many of them through. If the fog was completely on the ground, the only alternative was to climb high above possible danger. This was little comfort, for there is the disturbing knowledge that when the ceiling is reached the fuel may be gone and the plane must come down. Flying in fog without proper navigation instru- ments, the pilot will become hopelessly lost from a directional stand- point. q. Flying in jog without instruments.—Flying in fog without a visual reference to gravity (the earth), such as a bank-and-turn indi- cator, or some form of artificial horizon, the pilot will be unable to sense the position of his ship with relation to the earth’s surface, unable to sense the speed and direction of motion and will eventually go into circular motion, experience the vertigo described, and crash unless he takes to his chute before volitional control is lost. r. Instrument jlying.—Blind flying is the grim specter of aviation. The remedy for all this is to practice simulated blind flying under the hood until instrument flying is as easy as flying under clear skies and with perfect visibility. About 1919 the Air Corps advocated the use of any instruments that would add to the safety of flying, and there was a revived interest in various flying instruments. However, no progress was made until 1926. A report made by Donald E. Key hoe in 1929 follows: * * * Full credit must be given these men (pilots) who have tested the vari- ous instruments and methods suggested by scientists. The invention of the bank- and-turn indicator was the first step. But the pilots who first used it tried differ- ent methods without instruction, so few became expert. Those who succeeded were able to go through fog or snow for 20 or 30 minutes and at the end of that time their strained nerves would stand no more. Sight of ground or sky became vitally necessary to clear away the confusion that was swiftly taking control. 75 TM 8-300 69 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Two men were mainly responsible for progress beyond this stage. Ocker and Myers proved that this strain was caused by the pilot’s disbelief in his instruments and a strong tendency to trust his own senses, which are alway misleading. The Ocker-Myers method takes into account the three elements which give balance: muscle sense, sight, and restibular sense. * * * s. Overconfidence.—There is a tendency in an occasional young aviator to become so cocky over his flying ability that it becomes necessary to partially deflate it for his own safety. For many years the following plan was used in such cases: they were placed in the Jones-Barany revolving chair and turned right or left and asked which way they were turning. Their replies were naturally correct. Their eyes were then covered and the rotation repeated. After a few turns the chair was gently stopped and they were asked which direction they were turning. Vertigo having been induced, their replies were invariably that they had started to turn in the oppo- site direction of prior motion. The eyes were uncovered and to their amazement the chair was not turning at all. If this did not quite satisfy them, the rotation was continued until they experienced the sensation that the body was not moving. On being uncovered and finding they actually were turning right or left their chagrin was. very evident. For an aviator to suddenly discover his inability to. tell which way his body is turning, if at all, is, to say the least,, disconcerting. t. Origin of research.—In January 1926 this induced vertigo test was given to an old-time pilot. Following the test he disappeared without comment of any kind but soon returned with the view box (fig. 4) in his hand. The test was repeated in all combinations of rotation, using the unlighted box to cut out the light and thus; remove sight from the trinity of equilibrium senses. There was the usual induced vertigo with the usual inability to correctly tell which, way his body was turning, etc. The gyroscope was then started and the bank-and-turn indicator put in action. The flash was lighted and. the tests repeated. This time every answer was correct as to direc- tion of motion, stopping, and starting. Even the confusion of re- versals was absent. The sensations were felt the same as before;: but by giving the answer shown by the pointer on the bank-and-turn indicator, instead of the answer prompted by his senses, it was found impossible to confuse him. This demonstration started the research into blind flying. It was immediately recognized that here was the answer to pilot’s inability to do blind flying without a visual refer- ence to gravity. u. Bank-and-turn indicator.—By lighting the box the equilibrium trinity senses was restored to a coordinated action. Merely restoring 76 KOTES OK EYE, EAR, ETC., IK AVIATIOK MEDICIKE TJYL S-aUU 62 Figukb 4.—View box. 77 TM 8-300 62 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT sight to the equilibrium sense, however, is not enough. There must be something within the pilot’s range of vision that will act as a vertigo stopper and tell him what position his ship is in with relation to the earth. In other words, allow the pilot to mentally visualize “where the ground is.” The hand on the bank-and-turn indicator will accurately show motion in either direction, right or left, and will come to a dead center and remain there when there is no rotation. There will be the same false impressions of reversal of movement and falling received by the brain following the rotation; but by means of sight one will be able to correct these false impressions of movement and vertigo will be almost immediately overcome, 'provided one believes the instrument. v. Importance of discovery.—That there was any connection between the normal physiological reactions of a pilot and his lack of ability to do blind flying had not been considered until experience with, and belief in, the action of the bank-and-turn indicator crashed head-on into the knowledge of induced vertigo and the physiological reactions involved in the special senses concerned. Out of the wreck emerged several things of vital importance to aviation. w. Fovmdation sense.—The foundation sense of all spatial orienta- tion is vision. There is no substitute for visual reference. It makes no difference what the pilot sees so long as it gives him that vital in- formation, “Where is the ground and what is the position of the air- plane with reference to it.” Many accidents have resulted from ig- norance of this vital fundamental factor. Many have resulted because there were pilots who knew they could do blind flying by using their “flying sense.” There is no longer any excuse for ignorance regarding blind flying. Without exception the present day pilot who can do so obtains training in instrument flying. This is a far different reac- tion than the general attitude of most aviators when it was an- nounced, in 1926, that “no one could do blind flying without artificial aids” and that it had been “discovered how to do it.” The idea was promptly labeled as being enthusiastically crazy. x. Flying sense.—In the past those pilots who had discovered they could not fly blind did so through bitter experience. They, however, had nothing of value to report as an aid to their fellow pilots. They merely labeled themselves as better fliers than the average. However, it was noted they avoided fog flying. Having been taught to fly- by instinct it was hard to convince the average pilot that his flying sense would not bring him back from every flight. A knowledge of the uses of any one of the special senses and the care of and our reactions under all conditions toward these special 78 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 62 senses become of vital importance and value only to the individual making special and expert use of that special sense. The airman being vitally concerned in his ability to sense his posi- tion, change of position, and relation to the earth’s surface, has called all his special senses into play and has developed “flying sense.” What is flying sense ? It is not an inherent sense. It is an acquired sense. It is something the airman has that others do not. In its entirety it is composed of the special senses of sight, hearing, taste, smell, touch or tactile sense, muscle sense, and vestibular or inner ear sense. Just as there are degrees of “doctor sense,” there are airmen with varying degrees of “flying sense.” Being an acquired sense and de- pendent for its development on the ability, adaptability, aptitude, and knowledge of the person, it is evident that, everything else being equal, the airman who correctly understands and interprets the sensa- tions received from his various senses will have the most “flying sense.” y. Reactions in air.—Constant repetition of demonstrations with the “vertigo stopper box” finally convinced pilots that it was a real lie detector and that, on the ground at least, they could not tell which way they were turning if they could not see. Many con- tinued to fly by instinct in the stubborn belief that these “reverse” reactions could not happen in the air. In answer to this a covered cockpit ship with one exposed control pilot seat was devised and many hours were spent testing out the various reactions of pilots. It was proven beyond a doubt that these reactions do take place in the air and, in addition, are much intensified. Later the findings were communicated to the National Advisory Committee for Aeronautics and elaborate flight tests were made with a covered hood and a control pilot. Their findings verified the theory that circular movement invariably resulted during flights made by a pilot flying in a totally dark cockpit. These findings veri- fied our original contention regarding movement and the production of induced vertigo. Carroll and McAvoy published the following conclusions: Many pilots have felt that the flying sense was largely one of muscular balance and that visual reference played a more or less insignificant part. These experiments should serve to remove this idea and develop the apprecia- tion of the fact that muscular balance plays an extremely small part in flying, excepting in correlation with visual reference in the development of a polished technique. Visual reference of some sort must be provided, either by the horizon, by the reflection of the sun or moon while in dense fog or clouds, or by proper instrumental equipment. 79 'I'M 8-«0U 62 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT The fact should not be neglected that the use of proper navigational instru- ments provides an artificial horizon, if not in a single instrument, then in the correlation of several instruments, such as a turn-and-bank indicator and an air-speed meter. It can be recommended to all pilots that a careful self training in the use of and reliance on navigational instruments of this character will provide them not only with definite mechanical assistance, but likewise will go far to remove the psychological hazard of blind flying. z. Spiral movement.—Many years ago experiments were conducted with blindfolded individuals and proved conclusively that spiral movement always resulted when running, walking, swimming, driv- ing a car, etc. Continuing the experiments on the lower forms of life it was concluded that any forward-moving organism (including man) would move in a spiral path provided no orientating sense, such as sight, touch, etc., guided it. aa. Value of viem box.—The value of the Ocker-Myers view box became generally recognized as the only available means of instructing pilots and prospective pilots while on the ground in the sensations they would experience and the reactions they would have if they attempted to do blind flying without an artificial horizon. db. Artificial horizon.—The term “artificial horizon” was originated and given the folloAving definition in the original report: Any instrument or combination of instruments that will quickly, easily, and reliably give the pilot information that he may mentally visualize in terms of where is the ground. ac. Reference.—In the United States Air Service magazine, issue of April 1928, there appeared an article, “The Artificial Horizon Seeks Recognition,” by Frederick R. Neely, in which the research conducted was fully explained. ad. Results of research.—Instrument flying and simulated blind fly- ing were the logical outcome of this research work, because there had been established the physiological foundation on which to build the technical superstructure. The device (except chair) illustrated in fig- ure 4 was originated and a course of ground instruction in blind flying was formulated which was adopted by the Air Corps as routine in May 1934. In addition, a routine course in actual instrument flying follows the ground instruction. ae. Function of demonstration box.—The Ocker-Myers demonstra- tion box is based on sound physiological fundamentals and its value as a preliminary to actual instrument flying in an airplane is essential because only in this way can the student be actually shown that he cannot depend on his own sensations when God’s horizon is not available. 80 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 62-63 af. Developments.—The problem of how to keep the pilot in the air when he did not have a horizon to look at having been solved, it became necessary to solve the problem of guiding him to his intended destination and safely landing him on an airport he could not see. The development of blind take-off, blind navigation, and blind landings was much stimulated by the discovery of the basic fundamental fac- tors underlying blind flying. Today they are accomplished facts. Figure 5.—Instrument board of blind flying ship. ag. Key to problem.—If this type of flying is participated in for a length of time sufficient to train the pilot in automatic control he will finally become as proficient as if he were flying in ideal weather. The successful blind flier must correlate his senses to his instruments. Section XI MISCELLANEOUS EAR CONDITIONS Paragraph Costen’s syndrome 63 Tinnitus aurium 64 Meniere’s symptom complex 65 Labyrinthitis 66 Deafness in aviators 67 The “leans” 68 63. Costen’s syndrome.—a. History.—These neuralgias and ear symptoms associated with disturbed function of the temporoman- dibular joint were first proposed as a symptom complex in 1933. At 271818°—40 6 81 TM 8-300 63 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT that time, it was considered to be of infrequent occurrence, but since then a rather large number of cases have been observed and the condi- tion may now be considered quite common. b. Cmse.—The chief function of the jaw is mastication, and.it is equipped with powerful muscles, the masseter, the temporalis, and the pterygoid internus, which act to close the jaw. Most of the action of closure is brought against the posterior third of the jaw and so, when the molar teeth are missing or the grinding teeth are reduced in size, the mandibular joint is liable to destruction. Anatomically, the symp- toms resulting may be explained by— (1) Erosion of the bone of the glenoid or mandibular fossa and im- paction of the condyle against the thin bones separating them from the dura. (2) Irritation by the uncontrolled movements of the condyles back- ward or mesially, of the auriculo-temporal nerve. (3) Production of reflex pain and sensory disturbances in the various connections of the chorda tympani nerve, the condyle irritating it where it emerges from the tympanic plate at the mesial edge of the glenoid fossa through the petrotympanic fissure. (4) Compression of the eustachian tubes; in overclosure of the joint, the tensor veli palatini muscle bordering the membranous ante- rior edge of the tube and the adjacent spheno-meniscus muscle are seen to wrinkle and crowd the eustachian tube, closing it firmly. Normally, during the act of swallowing the tensor palatini muscle should be tensed and effect a temporary opening of the tube. c. Symptoms.—The following symptoms either singly or in com- bination are found in this condition: (1) Ear symptoms. (a) Intermittent or continuously impaired hearing. (b) Stopping or “stuffy” sensation in the ears, marked about mealtime. (c) Tinnitus usually low buzz in type, less often a snapping noise while chewing. (d) Dull or drawing pain within the ears. {e) Dizziness, with nystagmus. (2) Pain and irritative symptoms. (a) Headache about the vertex and occiput and behind the ears, increasing toward the end of the day. (b) Burning sensation in the throat, tongue, and side of the nose. (c) Dry mouth with almost total absence of saliva; rarely, exces- sive saliva. 82 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 63-65 (d) Occasional herpes of the external ear canal and buccal mucosa, most marked on the edentulous side. d. Treatment.—The treatment of these cases is reposition of the mandible by opening the bit, increasing the vertical dimension, and correcting the malocclusion. 64. Tinnitus aurium.—a. Description.—Tinnitus aurium is a subjective sensation of sound in one or both ears. It is a common symptom of aural disease. It is sometimes divided into two types: external, produced in the middle ear by actual stimulation of the tympanic membrane; internal, beginning in the labyrinth and cre- ated by some upset of metabolic equilibrium or disease of the nerves, cerebrum, or associated organs. Tinnitus may or may not be associated with deafness. The sounds may be constant or intermittent and the severity may increase or diminish. It may precede or follow the onset of deafness and may be associated with dizziness or staggering. The noises may be simple or complex and vary in character and intensity. They are described as hissing, whistling, humming, rus- tling, burring, blowing, ringing, clicking, shrieking, jangling, roar- ing, rumbling, and rasping. Occasionally they may have the form of a specific tune. b. Classification.—Tinnitus is sometimes classified according to the underlying lesion or disorder; (1) Obstruction sounds—Noises due to occlusion or impaired motility of some portion of the sound-conducting apparatus. (2) Labyrinth sounds.—Noises due either to structural changes in the cochlea or to alterations, either increase or diminution of the intralabyrinthine pressure. (3) Neurotic sounds.—Noises due to abnormal instability of the auditory nerve. (4) Cerebral sounds.—Noises due to abnormal conditions acting upon the auditory centers in the cerebral cortex (auditory hallucina- tions). (5) Blood sou7\ds.—Noises produced by the blood currents in ves- sels in or near the ear and due either to disturbances in the local or general circulation or to abnormalities in the size, shape or posi- tion of the vessels. The first four types will cause,a subjective tinnitus, while the fifth type may be both objective and subjective. 65. Meniere's symptom complex.—a. History.—In 1860 Meniere reported a case of spontaneous hemorrhage into the labyrinth. In his report he gave a clear picture of the symptoms which accompany 83 TM 8-300 65-66 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT recurrent aural vertigo. There has been some confusion as to the terminology and it is generally conceded that the term “Meniere’s disease” be used to define true cases of the rare labyrinthian hemor- rhage and “Meniere’s symptom complex” or “Meniere’s syndrome” be used in cases of recurrent aural vertigo. h. Symptoms and characteristics.—This condition is characterized by the following symptoms: (1) A sudden onset in a previously healthy auditory apparatus. (2) Vertigo, uncertain gait, rotations, and falling associated with nausea and vomiting. (3) Tinnitus, continuous or intermittent. (4) Deafness, unilateral and progressive. Occasionally hearing may be suddenly completely abolished. In a few cases there occurs in the attacks a transitory loss of vision and diplopia, and cases in which loss of consciousness occurs have been noted. These attacks are recurrent, the period between the attacks varying from weeks to years. These attacks are so distressing that even the symptomless intervals are marred by the dread of their recurrence. The disease is one of middle life and affects males more frequently than females. There is a tendency for the process, whatever it may be, to progress for several years until the function of the ear is lost. c. Cause.—The cause of the condition is unknown; the following causes having been suggested by various authors: (1) Local infection. (2) Allergy. (3) Lesions of the auditory nerve. (4) Cyclic changes, either chemical or physical, in the endolymph. d. Treatment.—There have been many types of treatments advo- cated, and the following have met with some success: (1) Surgical. (a) Section of the vestibular fibers of the VIII nerve. (h) Alcoholic injection of the labyrinth. (2) Dietary.—The use of a salt-free diet with addition of massive doses of ammonium chloride. 66. Labyrinthitis.—a. Recognition.—The symptoms are so severe and the findings so positive that the question of labyrinthitis in the examination of applicants for flying training presents no difficulties. Also, it is most unusual to have a case of involvement of the labyrinth without an accompanying deafness of the involved ear. Labyrinthian disorders will occasionally occur in the active per- sonnel, and a brief description of types, causes, and symptoms is indicated. 84 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 66 h. Sources of infection.—True infection of the labyrinth with pyo- genic organisms occurs from three sources: metastitic, meningeal, and tympanic. The meningeal source is usually associated with cerebro- spinal meningitis. The tympanic cases are usually by direct extension from acute or chronic suppurative otitis media, or mastoiditis. Laby- rinthitis is said to occur in about 1 percent of cases of middle ear suppuration. The end results may vary from recovery with normal function to complete destruction of the labyrinth. c. Symptoms.—The symptoms consist of— (1) Vestibular nystagmus (slow movement in one direction and a quick movement in the oppo- site direction).—This nystagmus varies from the mild type, in whicli the movement only occurs when the eyes are directed in the direction of the quick movement, to the type in which the nystagmus persists no matter where the eyes are turned. (2) Vertigo.—Rotary in character with the subjective impression that the surrounding objects are rotating in the plane of the nystag- mus. There is a tendency of the body to fall in the direction of the slow component. (3) Nausea and vomiting. (4) Tinnitus and deafness. Cases show great variations in the types and severity of the symp- toms, One recent case showed only nystagmus when looking to- ward the direction of the quick movement, and complained only of a feeling of “lightness” in the head and a tendency to fall to the left. d. Tests.—Vestibular tests should not be made during the acute stage of the disease, but on subsidence of the acute attack, the ves- tibular reaction will usually be absent. e. Other cases with similar symptoms—Labyrinthian symptoms are seen in cases other than those with suppurative labyrinthitis and many causes are given. All may produce typical findings: (1) Cold, heat, and sunstroke. (2) Hyperemia or hemorrhage into the labyrinth. (3) Sudden changes in pressure in the middle ear, seen in divers and aviators. (4) Mechanical disturbances produced by movements, airsickness, trainsickness, seasickness, and swingsickness. (5) Drugs: alcohol, quinine, tobacco, morphine, and salicylates. (6) Detonations and shock, probably by sudden changes of pres- sure in the middle ear. (7) Repeated loud noises. (8) Mental disturbances, such as fright. (9) Injuries to the labyrinth, such as may occur while doing a paracentesis or from penetrating wounds which strike the labyrinth. 85 TM 8-300 66-67 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (10) Focal infection, toxemia, circulatory disturbances. (11) Associated with other diseases such as syphilis, exanthemata, nephritis, mumps, leukemia, and others. /. Airsickness.—Airsickness is a major problem, as many cadets are unable to continue training because of the severity of the attacks. So far it has been impossible to tell which men will be airsick. Labyrinth tests usually do not give any reliable findings. Probably a truthful history of train or swingsickness would be indicative, but such histories are difficulut to obtain, as young men are as a rule some- what ashamed to admit these defects. Most probably airsickness is a complex reaction to the consciousness of disordered orientation of the body in space and results from a combination of causes, ocular, vestibular, cerebellar, cerebral, and psychogenic, particularly the feeling of anxiety, combined with an overactivity of the sympathetic nervous system. 67. Deafness in aviators.—a. Terminology.—It has been noticed for many years that the older pilots have a diminished auditory acuity. This condition has sometimes been called aviator’s deafness. Liberty Motor deafness, flying deafness, and aero otosclerosis. It is doubtful that this condition warrants a specific name. b. Causes.—The diminished hearing is due to a number of causes; (1) Repeated exposure to noise of high decibel rating of fairly constant pitch over prolonged periods of time. After riding in an open airplane for any great period of time, one is more or less deaf for a subsequent variable period of time. Repeated exposures tend to cause this temporary condition to become permanent. Several theories as to the cause of this type of deafness have been given, but it is probable that it results from derangement or injury to the external hair cells of the organ of Corti. (2) In addition to this cause, the factors mentioned in section V must be added, and, of course, these factors are variable in different individuals. (3) Of late years the widespread use of radio has added still another insult to the auditory apparatus, and it is still too early to say what the final result of this additional factor will be. c. Incidence.— (1) Past.—It is noted clinically that the deafness in any marked degree has occurred in the older pilots, particularly some of the wartime pilots, one of the reasons for this being the general reduction in auditory acuity that occurs in age. However, other conditions must be considered. These pilots flew in ships with cockpits which were very little protected from the slipstream. The engines with open stacks were either directly in front or directly to 86 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 67-68 the side and very close. Furthermore, they did not know or did not use prophylactic measures such as plugging the external auditory canals with cotton or other material and the maintenance of middle ear pressure equilibrium during ascent and descent. (2) Present.—At the present time we find many younger pilots who have more air hours than the older pilots who show little or no decrease in auditory acuity. This is due to the better construction and protection of the cockpit, change in type of engines used, greater distance from engine to pilot, streamlining with decrease in struc- tural noises, decreased vibration, and a better knowledge of prophy- lactic measures. (3) Future.—With increasing refinements in engineering and noise control, it is to be expected that the deafness of aviators will grad- ually decrease so that their auditory acuity will be but little below that of other males of the same age group. In the meantime, it is essential to select only those with good hearing as applicants. d. Mixed type.—As a rule the deafness noted in the pilots is of a mixed type—diminished reception of the low tones with a marked decrease or even total loss of the notes above 2048 D. V. This type of hearing curve is to be expected for a condition resulting from both a conduction and a perception deafness. It is interesting to note that the right ear is usually more involved than the left. 68. The “leans.”—This is a term used by some pilots to describe an interesting reaction to a disordered orientation of the body in space. The phenomenon occurs following prolonged flights by instruments, and is described as being a feeling that the ship is flying in a banked position. The degree of lean is variable in different individuals and at different times, the length of time varying from a few seconds to 1 or 2 minutes. Either the right or the left wing may seem to be down. Some pilots describe the condition as being merely a momentary disturbance following a turning movement, while others describe the sensation as being very definite, with a strong urge to right the ship. In practically all cases where this occurs the pilots are experienced instrument flyers, and a glance at the instruments, which show level flight, corrects the false sensation. The condition is important in that if an inexperienced pilot should neglect his instruments and attempt to correct his impression by the use of his ailerons, disaster could easily follow. Most pilots of this day have had instrument-flying experience and have confidence in the accuracy of their instruments. There is, however, a period of transi- tion—that is, in changing from flying contact to flying instruments— when our false physiological conceptions of our position in space are confusing even to some of the best instrument flyers. 87 TM 8-300 69 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Section XII GENERAL METHODS AND EQUIPMENT FOR EYE EXAMINATION Paragraph General 69 Purpose 70 69. General.—a. Importance.—There is no vocation as yet under- taken by man that is so dependent upon “vision” as that of flying. “Reduced to its simplest terms the function of the eye is the light sense. This is the discrimination between light and darkness, and various degrees thereof” (Adler), Going a little further it may be said that “vision” is the proper appreciation of light, of form, of color, and of distance, and we see how all of these are of vital im- portance to the aviator; and a defect, which may be considered as minor or insignificant in the ordinary walks of life may be serious indeed for the pilot of aircraft. h. Equipment.—In order that a thorough and intelligent examina- tion of the eye and its adnexa may be conducted, it is essential that adequate and proper equipment is used, and that ample floor space and lighting facilities are available. For certain phases of the sub- jective examination a dark room of sufficient length to permit a dis- tance of 6 meters between the examinee and certain test objects em- ployed is necessary, and in addition there should be a well-lighted room, particularly for the objective parts of the examination. In the dark room there should be a convenient lamp situated over or near the chair of the examinee, controlled by a switch which is accessible to the examiner. It should be remembered that a conclusion may be arrived at in some instances by two or more methods, and where possible the examiner should verify his findings by two or more methods before making a final interpretation. c. Accuracy.—In all probability there is no other type of physical examination where the time element is so important and where the adage “make haste slowly” applies more forcibly." A complete ocular examination is at best a tedious procedure and requires an infinite amount of patience on the part of both the examiner and examinee, and where “short cuts” are employed, or decisions made hastily, the accuracy of the findings is at best questionable. d. Type.—In the examination of applicants for appointment or detail to the Air Corps for flying training, it should be borne in the mind of the examiner that such an examination differs materially from the examination of patients reporting to a clinic for treatment 88 TM 8-300 69 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE of some condition that they themselves perhaps have recognized. It is to be presumed that all applicants for flying training are anxious to qualify physically, and in some instances may attempt to conceal or to minimize some ocular defect, provided they are aware of its existence. Therefore, it is not infrequently the problem of the examiner to uncover and recognize defects without the assistance and cooperation of the examinee. For this reason, all phases of the examination should be so conducted that, where possible, findings are based upon objective methods of examination. Obviously, this is not altogether possible. In many instances, however, subjective findings may be checked by objective methods. e. Phases.—In the examination of the eyes of applicants for flying training in the Air Corps, there are 14 phases or steps toward its completion, and where each phase is carefully and painstakingly accomplished there is little that may be overlooked. It will be noted, and will be explained later, how some of these phases may overlap or be very closely associated with one another. Below are listed these various steps of the ocular examination and the equipment employed. In some instances more than one method may be em- ployed to arrive at one finding, and in such cases those pieces of equipment as are called for in All 40—110 are mentioned first. (1) Visual acuity {right and left).—Apparatus Required: Il- luminated Snellen test types at 6 meters, or various modifications of test types incorporated with electrically lighted cabinet or projection device. (2) Depth perception.—Apparatus required: Howard-Dolman depth perception apparatus with proper illumination. (3) Maddox rod screen, test at 6 meters.—Apparatus required: Phorometer trial frame equipped with multiple Maddox rods, right and left, Risley rotary prisms, right and left; or prisms, Maddox rod and trial frame from trial lens case; and in either case, whether or not the phorometer trial frame is used, a spotlight 1 centimeter in diameter placed at 6 meters in front of examinee’s chair. (4) Red lens test.—Apparatus required: Red piano lens, trial frame, flat surface such as a blackboard, and meter rule. (5) Prism divergence.—Apparatus required: Risley rotary prism in phorometer trial frame, or prisms from trial lens case. (6) Associated parallel movements {and tangent curtain).—Ap- paratus required: Small test object, as white-headed pin, Bjerrum tangent screen, or blackboard in conjunction with tangent rule, and spotlight as used in Maddox rod test at 33 centimeters. (7) Inspection.—Apparatus required: Adequate illumination, con- densing lenses as required for oblique, illumination (from trial 89 TM 8-300 69-70 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT lens case), and, where available, binocular loupe (Beebe), hand slit lamp, ophthalmic lamp, slit lamp with binocular corneal microscope, and electric ophthalmoscope. (8) Pupils.—Apparatus required: Same as for inspection. (9) Accommodation.—Apparatus required: Prince rule with card, or Thorne rule. (10) Angle of convergence.—Apparatus required: Prince rule, or small millimeter rule and small test object, as white-headed pin, and table for computing angle of convergence. (11) Central color vision.—Apparatus required: The Ishihara test, and the Stillings’ pseudoisochromatic test and three sets of Holm- gren’s wools. (12) Field of vision.—Apparatus required: Perimeter with test objects, Bjerrum tangent curtain or blackboard with tangent rule, campimeter if available. (13) Refraction.—Apparatus required: Cycloplegic, retinoscope, trial lens case, and Snellen test types. (14) Ophthalmoscopic examination.—Apparatus required: Oph- thalmoscope, preferably electric. Each of these phases of the examination will be discussed in turn. In many instances references will be made to physiologic optics, physiology, histology, anatomy, and pathology of the eye in ex- planation of the procedure of the examination. It is suggested that the student familiarize himself particularly with the physiology of the eye. 70. Purpose.—The purpose in the preparation of the following part of the manual is primarily to enable the officers to conduct the required examination of Air Corps personnel in a thorough manner and to be able to interpret intelligently the findings of such examinations when he is assigned to duty as flight surgeon. The flight surgeon’s responsibility does not cease with routine, original, annual, and semiannual examinations of flying personnel under his care. While he is not expected to become an ophthalmologist (nor by any means will his practice be limited to ophthalmology) he should be able to arrive at an intelligent conclusion regarding the diagnosis of an ocular defect, disease, or injury, and within limita- tions, to treat such conditions where treatment is indicated. In some instances he may be able to detect an abnormality in its in- cipiency and by proper measures prevent its development to the ex- tent that the services of a capable pilot are lost. He should re- member that he is the “engineering officer” of flying personnel and is as much concerned with “maintenance” as with “selection” of the aviator. 90 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 71-72 Section XIII VISUAL ACUITY Physiological and psychological factors concerned 71 Formation of retinal image 72 Resolving power of eye 73 Acuity of retina 74 Alining power of eye 75 Conditions that interfere with normal visual acuity 76 Snellen test letters 77 Metric system 78 Readability of various letters 79 Paragraph 71. Physiological and psychological factors concerned.—It should be remembered that in asking an individual to read certain test letters on a chart some distance away there are various phycho- logical as well as physiological factors that enter into the examination These are the power of attention, the willingness and ability of the examinee to exert an effort to cooperate, and the power of concen- tration. There is another factor which, in some instances, may af- fect the findings obtained, namely, the ability to accurately and precisely control the extrinsic ocular muscles so that the retinal im- age of the test letters falls exactly upon the fovea, the most sensitive portion of the retina when the definition of form is concerned. Accuracy of fixation is a factor that should be given consideration, particularly in the examination of children. 72. Formation of retinal image.—Before going further into the determination of visual acuity or the appreciation of form, optics may be reviewed to the extent of how an image is formed upon the retina. Consider first a single point seen in the distance, or at in- finity ; from this point, which may be lighter or darker than its sur- roundings, rays of light radiate in every direction until interrupted or interfered with in some manner. Of these rays of light there will be a certain number, forming a cone, that will strike the cornea of the eye of the observer. Of the cone of rays of light striking the cornea there will be a certain number of rays that will pass through the pupil, and of these there will be one ray, the axial ray, that will pass through the optical center (nodal point) of the dioptric sys- tem of the eye. This ray will not be refracted and eventually will strike the retina at a point where one cone will be stimulated. If the eye be emmetropic, all other rays of light emanating from the point and passing through the pupil will be refracted and will converge upon the point on the retina as stimulated by the axial ray, and the result will be the stimulation of a single cone on the retina. Con- 91 TM 8-300 72-74 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT sider a line as being made up of a series of points immediately adjacent to one another, and follow the formation of a retinal im- age as by points forming the line. The end result will be the forma- tion of an inverted retinal image greatly reduced in size, of the line being observed. The observation of two lines crossing one an- other explains the retinal picture in two dimensions. 73. Resolving power of eye.—?n determining visual acuity the visual angle is utilized, and this angle is the guide by which test objects have been constructed. By the visual angle is meant the minimal angle formed by the intersection of the two (axial) rays crossing at the nodal point of the eye when two points are seen as separate and distinct points. It is thought that in order to distin- guish two points as separate and distinct points, separate cones of the retina must be stimulated and there must exist at least one un- stimulated. It is believed that in order to perceive two points as separate and distinct points, the angle formed by the crossing of the two axial rays must be not less than 1 minute, and it is upon this assumption that the Snellen test types are designed. More re- cently it has been determined that the minimal angle is considerably less, probably around 50 seconds. “The discrimination of two points as separate has been sometimes spoken of as the optical resolving power of the eye” (Adler). There are two other factors which may influence the “visual angle” even in emmetropic eyes, these being aberrations, spherical and chromatic, and irradiation, the latter caus- ing an intensely bright object to appear larger than one of the same size but darker. Quoting Adler directly— A good example of this is seen in light from the stars. The visual angle of light coming from the stars is infinitesimal. The rays of light are practically parallel. In spite of this they are visible to us. * * * The factor which determines the visibility of a point of light has nothing to do with the visual angle it subtends, therefore, but depends upon the amount of light energy falling on the visual receptors. The brighter the star the larger it appears. 74. Acuity of retina.—Furthermore, various portions of the retina differ as to degree of sensitivity in perception of form, or acuity, the most acute area being the region of the fovea, and visual acuity rapidly diminishes as the retinal image approaches the periph- ery of the retina. This is considered as being due to two factors, that the macular area is made up of cones, packed very closely to- gether as it were; and that each of these cones has its own fiber leading back to the brain. In the periphery of the retina there are both rods and cones; these are widely separated, and several of them may be connected with the same fiber so that when one is stimulated the same sensation is produced as when another or several in the 92 NOTES ON EYE. EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 74-76 same group are stimulated. A comparison may be made as to the sense of touch upon the skin surface of various parts of the body. For example, one can distinguish two separate points of contact, as pin points, on the finger tip very readily even though they be quite close together, while on the skin of the back the two points must be fairly widely separated in order to be recognized as separate points. The difference in the ability of varying portions of the retina to recognize detail of form may be easily demonstrated by the use of a perimeter with an ordinary Jaeger near vision card. The acuity will be found to decrease rapidly as the card is moved away from the points of fixation. 75. Alining power of eye.—In the recognition of test letters, such as are used with the Snellen test charts, there is a factor in- volved in addition to the resolving power of the eye. This may be described as the “alining power of the retina” by which is meant the ability to perceive changes in position or a break in a line. “Although the minimum angle which the eye can resolve is nearly 60 seconds of arc, the alining power of the eye is much more sensitive than this. It is probably the most acute of the human senses. The precision of the eye in adjusting a mark on a vernier scale is extraordinarily deli cate. Under the best conditions a skilled observer will make read- ings on the scale with an average error of not more than 3 seconds of arc. Thus, the alining power can be 20 times as delicate as the resolving power” (Adler). 76. Conditions that interfere with normal visual acuity.— In the testing of visual acuity (central or foveal) there are some factors to be considered as influencing the findings. These are, in probable order of importance: a. Errors of refraction or ametropia, in all its varieties, in which the retina is “out of focus” with the dioptric system of the eye, and consequently the retinal image appears poorly defined. h. The size of the pupil. This may have two effects upon the find- ings. An increase in the diameter of the pupil naturally allows more light to enter the eye, and with low degrees of illumination will improve visual acuity. Still, a dilated pupil permits an intensification of spherical and chromatic aberrations, which do interfere with a retinal image having sharply defined borders. Where an error of refraction exists, a reduction in the diameter of the pupil will result in an improvement in acuity. This can be easily demonstrated. Granted that the individual is emmetropic, or nearly so, render him myopic by placing before his eye a plus sphere lens of 4 diopters and determine his acuity, which will probably be approximately 10/200. Now place a pinhole disc before the lens and the acuity will be markedly improved, 93 TM 8-300 76-77 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT probably to 20/40 or even better. When an individual is moderately myopic his acuity will be improved by a contracted pupil. Conse- quently an examinee may present altogether different findings as to acuity when examined with a brilliant light before him (causing a con- traction of the pupil) and when examined in a dark room with only the test letters illuminated. Cobb has shown that the pupil should be between 1 and 5 millimeters for optimal visual acuity when the test object is under ordinary illumination. c. The amount of illumination upon the test objects will affect the findings in an estimate of acuity. The effect of irradiation has already been mentioned. Above a certain point of illumination of test objects acuity will become less and below a certain degree of illumination it will decrease also. These are factors that vary with individuals and are problems necessitating further experimentation before definite con- clusions may be reached. In the use of the ordinary electrically lighted test cabinets, it is probable that too much illumination is made use of, rather than too little. It has been found that the most acute vision is obtained when the test letters are illuminated by about 9-foot candles. Recent researches have determined that the maximum amount of illu- mination before reduction in visual acuity (with test objects) is approx- imately 300-foot candles for normal individuals. This may be considered as the “glare point.” d. Test objects when illuminated by monochromatic light are seen with sharper definition of borders, therefore with increased visual acuity, due to the fact that the factor of chromatic aberration is eliminated, e. Pathological changes in the eye itself, or optic nerve, chiasm, optic tracts, visual pathways, and higher centers can cause a reduction in visual acuity. For example, choroidoretinitis, retinal hemorrhage, optic atrophy, retrobulbar neuritis, intracranial tumors, etc. 77. Snellen test letters.—The acuity of vision is determined clinically by the use of test letters. These are so designed that each component part, or stroke of the letter, subtends an angle of 1 minute and the letter as a whole subtends an angle of 5 minutes, these dimen- sions being based on test letters being used at a distance of 20 feet from the examinee. The Snellen test letters, constructed on this prin- ciple, are in universal use. Each Snellen test letter is of such shape that it can be placed in a square, which is in turn divided into 25 smaller squares of equal size. Therefore the entire letter subtends an angle of 5 minutes, and each stroke of the letter subtends an angle of 1 minute. It will be noted that on the Snellen test charts there are rows of letters of different sizes, there being usually a row of small- 94 TM 8-300 77-78 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE sized letters of the alphabet, and above or below this row a row of somewhat larger letters, and above still larger, etc. The size of the letters in the different rows is determined as follows: For the smaller row each individual letter subtends an angle of 5 minutes at 10 feet, or approximately 3 meters’ distance; in the next row, above or below as the case may be, each letter subtends an angle of 5 minutes at 15 feet; the next, 20 feet, and so on. If an individual being examined, seated 20 feet away from the test types, is able to read readily the row of letters, each of which subtends an angle of 5 minutes at this dis- tance, his vision is recorded as being 20/20, the numerator being the distance from the examinee to the test charts, and the denominator being the size of the letters in the row which is read. If at 20 feet he can read the letters in the row, each of which subtends an angle of 5 minutes at 15 feet, his vision is recorded as being 20/15. Thus we have the findings 20/20, 20/30, 20/40, and so on. The acuity of each eye is tested separately; the eye not being tested is screened by an opaque card or disc. Where an examinee can read all of the 20/30 row of letters, and only three of the letters in the 20/20 row, his vision is recorded as 20/30 plus 3 for the eye being tested. 78. Metric system.—Some examiners use the metric system. In such instances the acuity is recorded as being 6/6, 6/12, etc. These 1. Axial rays from extreme points of object (a) passing, unrefracted, through nodal point (n), forming inverted image (6) upon retina. Associated rays from extreme points of object are divergent and refracted upon entering eye and come to focus on axial rays at retina. lb. Axial rays forming angle of 5 minutes. Test letters are constructed under this angle. Distance from eye determines size of letters. Figure 6.—Diagrammatic illustration of visual angle and method of constructing visual test letters. 95 TM 8-300 78-79 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT fractions may be reduced, and an acuity of 6/6 (20/20) may be re- corded as 1; an acuity of 6/12 (20/40) being recorded as 1/2 or 0.5. But a visual acuity of 0.5 does not mean that the examinee has only one-half normal vision from a practical viewpoint. These “fractions” do not signify a percentage of “vision” in that form, being strictly empirical units. In explanation of repeated references to the use of Snellen test charts at a distance of 20 feet (6 meters), it may be said that this distance is chosen as a mean because at this distance, or distances greater than 20 feet, accommodation plays a very insignificant part in visual acuity of the emmetropic eye. At any distance less than 20 feet accommodation is brought into play, particularly at near dis- tances. Hence, 20 feet is selected as being the minimum as far as convenience and accommodation are concerned. As a matter of fact, the emmetropic eye must accommodate to a certain extent at this distance, presumably one-sixth of a diopter, this strength of lens having a focal distance of 6 meters. The use of a mirror at 10 feet distance with reverse Snellen charts alongside or above the examinee (the mirror may be used at any distance, provided the dis- tance from the chart to the mirror plus the distance from the mirror to examinee is exactly 20 feet) is a great convenience, but care must be taken that the mirror is silvered on the front surface in order that accurate findings are obtained. Otherwise the two reflecting surfaces may cause a blurring of the reflected letters. 79. Readability of various letters.—It is to be remembered that all letters of the alphabet do not possess the same “readability.” For example, consider the capital letter “L,” which in block form repre- sents a right angle, the apex of which is down and at the left. If visual acuity is defective to such an extent that the outline of the two strokes of the letter is blurred, the letter may be still recognized by* the fact that a right angle is formed. The letter “A” may be recog- nized as A when acuity is below normal because it is the only letter which is characterized by an acute angle apex up, and the examinee may be unable to distinguish the cross bar. The same conditions apply to the letters “T,” “V,” and even “F”; the latter may be recog- nized as a right angle apex up and to the left, and the cross bar of the upward stroke not seen. Sheard has concluded after extensive investigation that the letter “B” is the most difficult to distinguish and has prepared a table of comparison of the letters of the alphabet,, as follows, using the letter “B” as the unit of measurement 1.00. L 0. 71 C 0. 79 T 0. 74 O 0. 80 V 0.78 Y 0.81 96 TM 8-300 79 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE P 0. 81 R 0. 88 F 0. 81 S 0. 89 D 0. 82 G 0. 92 Z 0. 84 H 0. 92 N 0.85 B 1.00 E 0. 85 The usual Snellen test charts are so arranged that in each row of letters there are an equal number of those that are distinguished readily and those that are more difficult to recognize. Theoretically, the Snellen test types at 20 feet (6 meters) leave much to be desired as an accurate test of visual acuity, but from a practical viewpoint in conducting a clinical examination they have proven satisfactory. While 20/20 (6/6 or 1) may not be the maxi- mum acuity for the optically normal eye, it serves as a working basis. As a matter of fact, there are many instances of an acuity of 20/15 and better, without hesitation, encountered. Evidence seems to indi- cate that actually an acuity of 20/15 or better is much more near thei normal standard. The examiner should consider a defect in visual acuity as a symptom rather than a disease entity. In the examination of Air Corps personnel, the examiner should have accessible as great a variety of rows of test types as possible, particularly of the 20/20 size, as the probability of memorizing a row of letters, in many instances seen repeatedly on different occa- sions by the examinee, is not infrequently encountered. It is sug- gested that' where several rows of the 20/20 types are on hand, one or more be kept in reserve for use in questionable cases. In addi- tion, all letters except one at a time may be screened, which may elicit the fact that an examinee is repeating the letters in sequence from memory when he is permitted to see the entire row at once. If such is the case he will show some confusion where only one letter is exposed and all others screened. Section XIV DEPTH PERCEPTION General 80 Basic and adjunctive factors 81 Monocular and binocular factors 82 Size of retinal image , 83 Physiological diplopia 84 Binocular parallax 85 Adjunctive group of factors 86 Measuring parallactic angle 87 Etiology of defective depth perception 88 Summary 80 Paragraph 271S180—40 7 97 TM 8-300 80-82 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT 80. General.—By depth perception is meant the ability to judge distance, or the power to appreciate the third dimension. It is this power that materially aids in rendering an individual capable of cor- rectly orientating himself in relation to surrounding objects. It can be appreciated readily that this is an extremely important factor in aviation. It is this factor that enables the pilot to level off his air- plane at the proper distance from the ground in landing, to take off with a safe margin over obstacles, to be proficient in gunnery, and maintain his position in formation flights. 81. Basic and adjunctive factors.—a. Basic.—The factors composing the basic group constitute a part of the physical functions of the individual; they are constant, and when considered together may be termed his “inherent ability” to judge distance. These factors consist of— (1) Physiological diplopia. (2) Accommodation. (3) Convergence. (4) Binocular parallax. b. Adjvmctive.—The factors composing the adjunctive group may function independent of the individual; they are inconstant, and are common to all persons. They may be termed as factors which assist or enhance the basic group. These factors consist of— (1) Size of retinal image. (2) Motion parallax (movements of head or object). (3) Terrestrial association. (a) Linear perspective. (b) Overlapping of contours. (c) Light, reflections, and shadows. (4) Aerial perspective, that is, the changes with respect to color, brightness, and contrast which different objects undergo on account of variation in the clarity of the intervening atmosphere. 82. Monocular and binocular factors.—a. Some of the factors operating to constitute depth perception are common to monocular and binocular single vision alike, while others pertain to binocular single vision only. b. yactors common to monocular and binocular single vision are— (1) Size of retinal image. (2) Accommodation. (3) Motion parallax. (4) Terrestrial association, (6) Aerial perspective. 98 TM 8-300 82-83 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE c. Factors which operate with binocular single vision only are— (1) Physiological diplopia. (2) Binocular parallax. (3) Convergence. d. In employing a test for the purpose of determining an individ- ual’s ability to judge distances, it is necessary to utilize only those factors which operate to make for an individual difference in ability; that is, it is necessary to measure an individual’s inherent or acquired ability. In order to do this, the method employed must eliminate all exter- nal assistance that experience has taught us to employ. For instance, motion parallax is produced either by movement of the observer or by objects within his field of vision. For that reason it may be con- sidered as an artificial factor employed to enhance the already exist- ing facility. It should, therefore, be eliminated as a factor not related either to inherent or acquired ability. Factors external to ourselves which assist all of us equally, such as terrestrial association and aerial perspective, should be eliminated for the same reason. Factors which normally operate only at distances of less than 6 meters, such as accommodation, do not need to be considered when examining prospective aviators. Fliers are not, as a rule, called upon to form judgments at a distance of less than 6 meters. e. When all external factors and those operating at a distance of less than 6 meters are eliminated, there remains to be considered— (1) Size of the retinal image. (2) Physiological diplopia. (3) Binocular parallax. (4) Convergence. 83. Size of retinal image.—As the size of the retinal image oper- ates with monocular as with binocular single vision, the relative value of this factor can be obtained with the same testing apparatus by examining first both eyes and then one eye, the other being covered. With the test described herein it has been demonstrated that the ability to judge distances is many times more accurate with binocular single vision than with monocular vision. In monocular vision, there are eliminated the binocular parallax, convergence, and physiological diplopia. Therefore, judgment must depend upon the size of the retinal image alone. Since judgment of distance is many times less,accurate when the decision depends upon the size of the retinal image alone it follows that the important factors to be considered are physiological diplopia, binocular parallax, and convergence. 99 TM 8-300 84 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT 84. Physiological diplopia.—The faculty of recognizing differ- ences in distance between objects, which are located in space within our visual fields, is founded upon physiological diplopia, although we do not recognize it as diplopia. When the eyes fix an object (binocular fixation), the image of that object falls upon the maculae of both retinae. This image is projected outward and we see the object at the point where the visual lines cross, which is the place where the object is actually located. The image of another object within the field of vision, at a greater or lesser distance than the object fixed, falls upon the retinae at points outside of the maculae. If the second image falls upon the nasal side of the macula of the right eye, it is projected to the temporal field and the object is located to the right of the point actually occupied by the real object, and at a distance equal to the distance between its point of contact on the retina and the macula. If the imago falls upon the temporal side of the macula, it is projected to the nasal field and is located in the same manner. The same applies to the left eye. These two images are not focused by the cerebral fusion center and diplopia occurs. If the image falls upon symmetrical points of the retinae, that is, exact points on the nasal side of one and the temporal side of the other, the images are fused and are projected to the same point in space and diplopia does not occur. Two objects are located in the field of vision and but two objects are seen. However, if the image of the second object falls upon different points of the nasal and tem- poral portions of the retinae, the two images are not fused and two objects will be seen. The image or images of the second object are not to be confused with the object fixed. The object fixed appears as one and in its proper position. Figure 7 0 illustrates two objects (a) and (b) located at dif- ferent distances in space but within the visual field. The eyes have fixed (a), the far object. The image of (a) falls upon the macula of both eyes, and appears as one object at the place it actually occu- pies. The image (&), the near object, falls upon the temporal side of the macula of both eyes at (£, t). Therefore, the image (b) seen by the right eye is projected to the left at (V) and the same image seen by the left eye is projected to the right at (&"), giving rise thereby to bilateral crossed diplopia. In figure 7 (D the eyes have fixed (a), the near object. The image of (b), the far object, falls upon the nasal side of the macula of both eyes at (n, n). The image of (5) seen by the right eye is pro- jected to the right at (&"), and the same image seen by the left eye 100 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8—300 84-86 is projected to the left at (b') giving rise thereby to bilateral ho- monymous diplopia. Therefore objects located nearer than the object fixed give rise to* crossed diplopia, and those more remote than the object fixed give rise to homonymous diplopia. It is because of this homonymous and crossed diplopia that we receive the impression that objects are nearer or farther away in relation to other objects. In figure 7 ® the objects (a) and (b) are located in space in posi- tions similar to those occupied by the objects in the Ho ward-Dolman testing apparatus. The eyes have fixed the near object (a). It will be noted that the image of the far object (b), falls upon the temporal side of the macula of the right eye and upon the nasal side of the left eye, but at a greater distance from the macula of the left eye than the right. These points not being identical, the images are therefore not used but are projected into space as two objects (b') and (b"). 85. Binocular Parallax.—While we perceive, through physio- logical diplopia, the existence of difference in distance between objects, the binocular parallax augments this perception by giving rise to the impression of relief and solidity. When we fix an object the right and left eye obtain somewhat dif- ferent views of that object. The right eye sees a little farther around the object to the right, and the left a little farther around to the left. This gives rise to two retinal images which are not exactly alike. When these images are fused the disparity is responded to by percep- tion of relief and solidity. When we perceive a difference in distance between objects our conception of the amount of difference may be accurate or it may be decidedly inaccurate. It is, therefore, desirable to determine the degree of acuracy and this is accomplished by utilizing the binocular parallactic angle. Figure 8 (T) is a representation of two objects (o) and (o') located at unequal distances from the eyes (a) and (b). The two distances may be represented by two imaginary lines, one from (o), the near object, and the other from (o'), the far object, to a point midway between the two eyes, that is, an imaginary Cyclopean eye (an eye occupying the center of the forehead). These differences may be compared with less difficulty by using a more diagrammatic illustration. Let us therefore consider one eye (b) and the two objects (0) and (O') in a straight line as illustrated in figure 8 ®. 86. Adjunctive group of factors.—The adjunctive factors, with the exception of motion parallax, exist and operate independent of 101 TAE 8-300 86 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT the individual, but they are probably of as much value in judging distances as are the basic factors. We are primarily equipped with the essentials for forming judg- ments and we soon learn, through experience, to utilize all other assistance that comes to hand. However, if these essentials do not function properly, then the value of all external assistance that is available is proportionally reduced because we do not know how to employ it. Because of this it is necessary to determine whether or not the prospective flier possesses all the basic factors of depth per- ception, and if these factors are functioning properly. With a normal foundation for judging distances the student soon learns, in his new environment in the air, to utilize all external assist- ance that is available. After one becomes experienced in flying he may be deprived of all basic factors, with the exception of the size of the retinal image, and still be able to judge distances sufficiently accurately to fly a ship. This is especially true if he is familiar with the ship, the terrain, etc. This fact is demonstrated with experienced fliers who have lost an eye. At first they experience some difficulty, but this is soon over- come. Their ability to continue flying is due to the fact that flying, landing, etc., have become partially mechanical and subconscious and they have learned from experience how to utilize all external assist- ® Eyes fixing far object. a. Far object, ft. Near object. m-m. Maculae. t— t. Temporal side of retinae. Eyes are fixing far object »• Image falls upon maculae at m-m. Image of ft, near ob- ject, falls upon temporal side of maculae at t-t and is projected to 6' witli right eye and to ft" with left eye, inducing thereby increased diplopia. Figukh 7.—Physiological diplopia. 102 KOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 86 a. Near object. b. Far object. © Eyes fixing near object. m—m. Maculae. n—n. Nasal side of maculae. Eyes are fixing near object a. Image falls upon maculae at m—m. Image of b, far object, falls upon nasal side of maculae at n—n and is projected to b” with right eye and to b' with left eye, inducing thereby homonymous diplopia. ® Objects arranged similar to those in testing apparatus, a. Near rod. * m—m. Maculae. 5. Far rod. t. Temporal side of retina. n. Nasal side of retina. Eyes have fixed near rod a. Image falls upon maculae at m—m. Image of far rod & falls upon temporal side of right macula and upon nasal side of left, but image falls upon retina of left eye at greater distance from macula than in right eye. Therefore image of b is projected to b' with right eye and to b" with left, inducing thereby homony- mous diplopia with left eye and crossed diplopia with right eye. Figure 7.—Physiological diplopia—Continued. 103 TM 8-300 86-87 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT ance to the utmost. However, when these individuals are deprived of all external assistance, and their judgment must depend upon the size of the retinal image alone, as with the testing apparatus, they are many times less accurate than when both eyes are functioning. 87. Measuring parallactic angle.—The Howard-Dolman appa- ratus employed to measure the parallactic angle is designed to utilize only those factors belonging to the basic group. All external factors d Distance to near object 0. P Interpupillary distance Or~b. D Distance to far object O'. D minus d = difference in distance, or depth difference. Angle 1 minus angle 2 equals angle of binocular parallactic angle. P Interpupillary distance a-b. d Shorter distance h—O. D Longer distance b—O'. D minus d = depth difference. Let angle 1 equal aOh, angle of convergence upon near object, angle 2 equal aO'b, angle of convergence upon far object, and angle 3 equal angle OaO'. Then angle 1 minus angle 2 equals angle 8, which is the binocular parallactic angle represented by depth difference D—d. Figure 8.—Binocular parallax. 104 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 87-88 which usually operate to assist in judging distances are eliminated. The examinee is given two objects at unequal distances from him. His physiological diplopia, binocular parallax, and convergence tell him that a depth difference exists between the two objects. It is his task to eliminate this difference, that is, to place the objects at equal distances from him. The accuracy with which he accomplishes this determines his inherent degree of ability to judge distances. The testing apparatus consists of two upright rods, 64 millimeters apart, laterally, viewed through an aperture in a screen at a distance of 6 meters. All that is visible to the examinee is an illuminated white background crossed vertically by two black rods. One rod is stationary and the other can be moved backward and forward in a groove along the margin of a millimeter scale by means of cords. The examinee en- deavors to place the adjustable rod beside the stationary one so that both are at equal distances from him. When the rod is accurately placed the reading on the millimeter scale is zero, and the parallactic angle and physiological diplopia cease to exist. The abolition of diplopia and the parallactic angle can be demon- strated with the figures 7 (D and 8 @. If the object (&), figure 7 (a), is placed at the same distance from the eyes as (a), then the image of (h) will fall upon symmetrical points of the retinae, become fused, and be projected to the left of (&), as a single object. In figure 8 (2), if (O') the far object, is placed beside (O) the near object, the binocular parallactic angle (3) will be eliminated. The testing apparatus is so constructed that the size of the binocular angle will equal 10.3 seconds when the interpupillary distance is 64 millimeters, the stationary rod is located at 6 meters distance and a depth difference of 30 millimeters exists between this and the adjustable rod. Among Americans, interpupillary distances range from to 74 millimeters with an average of 64. In conducting tests it is impracti- cable to compute the size of the parallactic angle of every individual whose interpupillary distance is greater or less than 64. Therefore, a depth difference of 30 millimeters is taken as the outside limit and those who persistently exceed this limit are considered as defective in judg- ing distances. It has been demonstrated that an individual who persistently pro- jects the adjustable rod more than 30 millimeters from zero, and pos- sesses an ocular defect, experiences great difficulty in learning to fly. 88. Etiology of defective depth perception.—a. Occurrence of defective perception.—Ninety-nine and one-half percent of persons who are free from nervous, ocular, and general defects exhibit a per- 105 TM 8-300 88 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT sistent parallactic angle average 10.3 seconds or less, that is, they will persistently project the adjustable rod an average of not more than 30 millimeters from zero. The remaining one-half percent of this class of individuals will project the adjustable rod more than 30 milli- meters from zero for a large number of trials, but will eventually project it less than 30. Just why this occurs has not been definitely determined, but the cause seems to point to nervousness, poor com- prehension, and particularly to carelessness in performing the test. These men usually project the rod between 30 and 50 millimeters from zero. If they persist in this indefinitely they are considered as defective in judgment, but if they correct this error, after two or three series of trials, they are considered normal. Observation has taught us that these men experience no difficulty in flying, that is, insofar as judgment is concerned. Furthermore, practice on the testing apparatus will not reduce the average error when an ocular defect exists. 5. Known factors operating for poor depth perception. (1) Inequality of vision. (2) Refractive errors resulting in accumulative ocular fatigue and manifested by— (a) Accommodative asthenopia. (h) Heterophoria. (c) Insufficiency of convergence. All individuals, with the exception of the small number mentioned above, who are inaccurate in their judgment of distances are found to have one or more of the above defects. These individuals will project the adjustable rod 75 millimeters from zero in one direction, in the next trial will project it 150 millimeters in the opposite direc- tion, and in still another trial may place it at zero. In other words, they have no conception of the relative or actual positions of the rods. However, it must be remembered that some individuals may have all the defects enumerated above, with the exception of inequality of vision and still possess a very accurate estimation of distances, but in general, it may be stated that when any of the defects mentioned are severe enough to cause symptoms, for example, muscular fatigue, blurring of vision, diplopia, etc., judgment of distances is inaccurate, and the degree of inaccuracy is in proportion to the degree of symp- toms manifested. c. Inequality of vision or anisometropia.—Given a case with a visual acuity of 20/20 in one eye and 20/50 in the other, and assum- ing that there is no strain of accommodation in the defective eye, 106 TJVE 8-300 88 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE the defective judgment is due, under these conditions, to an inability to utilize fully the assistance alforded by ohysiological diplopia and binocular parallax. The cerebral interpretation of this diplopia FLOOR SIDE ELEVATION A-SLIDE. C-C- RODS 2" APART. B-CORD FOR MOVING SLIDE. Figure 9.—Depth perception apparatus. 107 TM 8-300 88 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT into distances is partially lost because of the diminished vision, also the impression of relief is not as marked as normal. Therefore the individual is utilizing monocular judgment in his estimation to a greater or less extent and the greater the reduction in vision the nearer will binocular approach monocular vision. d. Hyperopic refractive errors.—The existence of refractive errors creates a defect in the ocular mechanism and when accumulative fatigue ensues as a result of work, nervous strain, infection, etc., it is the existing defective structure that is first to become impaired in function. When hyperopic refractive errors exist, the individual must at all times accommodate to bring far as well as near objects into focus. Therefore, in order to do this the ciliary muscles must receive a greater amount of enervation than is required by a normal eye, and the increased amount is interpreted into distance by the cerebrum. Consequently the amount of nervous force employed gives rise to the sensation of a proportional amount of distance. Usually this ap- parent distance is less than that which actually exists. When the refractive error is equal in both eyes and is fully com- pensated for by accommodation, the defective judgment resulting therefrom frequently can be corrected. The increase of nervous impulse required to correct the defect is equal in both eyes. If the object upon which the eyes fix appears nearer or at a greater distance than it really is, the other objects or object will be falsely projected in proportion, and the relation between the two will remain un- changed. This type of defect gives rise to faulty landings. The student will persistently level off too high or too low until he has learned to compensate for his error. When the refractive errors are unequal in the two eyes, for ex- ample, one-half of a diopter in one and one diopter in the other, the higher error will require the greater amount of nervous impulse to accommodate. The unequal nervous strain will alter the apparent relative position of objects, and defective and erratic judgment results. e. Accommodative asthenopia.—When accumulative ocular fatigue is present, there are usually errors in refraction responsible for it. As it is necessary to accommodate constantly for distant objects, the eyes become tired as a result of the nervous and physical strain, accommodation relaxes, and objects are seen out of focus. Because of the indistinct vision an accurate estimation of distance is im- possible. This condition is particularly noticeable in some individuals who are undergoing flying training. One day they can judge dis- 108 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 88-89 tances very accurately and experience no difficulty in landing their ships, while on the following day they cannot level off and land consistently or accurately. These cases have latent hyperopic errors, and because of the mental and physical strain incident to training, their accommodative powers become reduced and objects are not brought into focus. The wearing of correcting lenses usually im- proves the faulty judgment. /. Heterophoria.—Heterophoria may be considered as fatigue of one or more of the extrinsic ocular muscles due to refractive errors, to a deficiency in the normal amount of enervation from some cause, or to an actual defect in the muscle structure itself. In order to fix both eyes upon an object the defective muscle must receive a greater amount of enervation than its fellows. This increased amount of enervation is interpreted into distance by the cerebrum just as it is in refractive errors, therefore the relative position of objects is in- accurately judged. If the defective muscle becomes sufficiently fatigued, or the required amount of nervous impulse cannot be sup- plied, the visual line of the eye concerned will deviate and objects will become blurred or pathological diplopia will ensue. When diplopia occurs all objects within the field of vision appear double, the true objects appearing more distinct than the false objects. g. Insufficiency of convergence.—Convergence must occur at all times in order to fix both eyes upon an object, no matter how remote, but the degrees of convergence required for distant objects are necessarily less than those for near objects. When the eyes become fatigued, convergence relaxes and objects are blurred or pathological diplopia results. This condition is usually associated with refractive errors, as is the condition of heterophoria. h. Myopic refractive errors.—Myopic errors have little or no effect upon depth perception, until the errors are sufficient to cause indis- tinct images and a strain upon convergence. When the errors are sufficient to cause these symptoms, depth perception becomes defec- tive as in insufficiency of convergence and inequality of vision. 89. Summary.—Accurate judgment of distances is important to the aviator because, if this sense or function is impaired, it is im- possible for him to learn to fly an airplane safely unless he is given an enormous amount of flying training. He may under these condi- tions become sufficiently prpficient to fly with comparative safety, but he will never attain even an average degree of proficiency. Inherent depth perception for objects located in infinity is de- pendent upon physiological diplopia and augmenting this diplopia are binocular parallax, size of the retinal image, and convergence. 109 TM 8-300 89 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT Physiological diplopia is interpreted into distance by the cerebrum and the binocular parallax gives rise to the impression of relief and solidity. An adjunctive group of factors assist inherent depth perception. These factors are common to all persons; they are inconstant but they play a very important part in accurate judgment. The degree of accuracy of judgment is determined by the size of the binocular parallactic angle. The testing apparatus is so con- structed that with an interpupillary distance of 64 millimeters, the stationary rod at 6 meters distance, and a depth difference between this and the adjustable rod of 30 millimeters, the binocular paral- lactic angle thus formed subtends an angle of 10.3 seconds. In determining the degree of accuracy in judging distances only the basic group of factors is considered, because if these are not functioning properly the adjunctive factors cannot be utilized fully. An experienced flier who has been deprived of some of the basic factors may be able to fly a ship with reasonable safety because he has learned from his flying experience to utilize to a marked degree the adjunctive factors. However, he is not as safe or reliable as the normal person. Ninety-nine and one-half percent of persons who are inaccurate in judgment have a defective ocular mechanism. These defects may be reduction of vision in one or both eyes or refractive errors result- ing in ocular fatigue and manifested by accommodative asthenopia, heterophoria, or insufficiency of convergence. A persistent parallactic angle of more than 10,3 seconds, that is, a depth difference of more than 30 millimeters, is considered as defective provided an ocular defect is present. Any hyperopic error in refraction may cause inaccurate judgment. Myopic errors have little effect upon judgment unless the error is sufficient to cause diminished acuity of vision and insufficiency of convergence. If in the first series of trials the candidate makes an average error of over 30 millimeters, for example, between 30 and 60, and no ocular or general physical defect can be found to account for it, it is advisable to recheck him every day for at least 3 days. Invari- ably these candidates will eventually attain an average error of 30 millimeters or less. Experience has taught us that these individuals experience no difficulty in flying, insofar as judgment of distances is concerned. 110 TM 8-300 90 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Section XV OCULAR MOVEMENTS ’Monocular and binocular projection 90 Field of fixation 91 Orthophoria and heterophoria 92 Review of physiology of extrinsic ocular muscles 93 Binocular movements 94 Prisms 95 Numbering of prisms 96 Rotary prism 97 Maddox rod 98 Phorometer trial frame 99 Sighting eye 100 Technique of determining imbalance : 101 Optical principles involved in horizontal deviations 102 Vertical deviations 103 Heterophoria at 33 centimeters 194 Etiology of heterophoria 105 Incidence of heterophoria 106 Other methods of detecting heterophoria 107 Associated factors 108 Power of abduction 109 Optical principles involved 110 Power of adduction 111 Prism convergence 112 Angie of convergence 113 Near point of convergence 114 Interpupillary distance 115 Meter angle 116 Associated parallel movements 117 Squint 118 Complete oculomotor paralysis 119 Paragraph 90. Monocular and binocular projection.—a. General.—After the estimation of visual acuity and ability to judge differences in depth or distance, there is to be determined whether or not there exists any abnormality in the motility of the eye. Under this heading there are to be considered, in the examination of flying per- sonnel, the Maddox rod screen test at 6 meters, the power of diver- gence, power of convergence, and associated parallel movements. Before taking these up in order, it is worth while to discuss briefly orientation of objects, binocular vision, the field of binocular fixation, the action of the extrinsic ocular muscles, defects in ocular move- ments, and the methods used in arriving at a conclusion in this phase of the examination. h. Monocular vision.—First, consider monocular vision, using the right eye alone. When a point {A) (fig. 10 ©) upon the horizon or at 111 TM 8-300 90 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT infinity fixed upon, the extrinsic ocular muscles of the eye are brought into play so that it is rotated to such a position that the retinal image of that point is formed on the fovea of the retina, where it is most clearly defined. Now suppose there is another point (B) to the right of point or the point of fixation, and it is located 10° to the right of point {A). The image of point (B) will be formed on the retina 10° away from the fovea on the nasal side of the right eye. If there is a third point (G) located 20° to the left of point (A) its image will be formed on the temporal side of the right retina 20° away from the fovea. The position of objects in space is deter- mined by their relation to the nodal point of the eye. If an image of an object is formed on the retina a number of degrees away from the fovea, this image will be projected in space the same number of degrees to the opposite side of the point of fixation, that is, if an image is formed on the retina at 10° to the nasal side of the fovea, it will be projected in space 10° from the point of fixation in the temporal field of vision. Or if an image is formed 10° above the fovea the object in space is projected 10° below the point of fixation. c. Binocular vision.—Now let us suppose both eyes are brought into use, and both eyes are fixed on point (.4) (fig. 10 (2)), point (B) (to the right of point will have its image formed at a point 10° medial to the fovea of the right retina, and 10° lateral to the left fovea. Point (C) (to the left of point (J.)) will have its retinal image formed 20° away from the right fovea on the temporal side, and, 20° from the left fovea on the nasal side. However, point (B) seen with both eyes will be seen as a single point, the images on the right and left retinae being fused into one. The same is true, of course, with point (C). When fusion occurs we may conclude that it is because images are formed on the two retinae at corresponding points. The two foveae are corresponding points; 10° to the nasal side of the right fovea and 10° to the temporal side of the left fovea are corresponding points; and so on. However, 10° immediately above the right fovea has a corresponding point 10° above the left fovea. When we fix upon an object in the distance, images of ob- jects to the right and left of the point of fixation will be formed on corresponding points of the retinae and binocular single vision re- sults. Points of the two retinae which are not corresponding are known as disparate points. When retinal images of the same object are formed on disparate points diplopia results. d. Convergence.—When an object nearer than infinity is the point of fixation there is necessarily a certain amount of convergence, or adduction, accomplished in order that the visual lines (visual line 112 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM S-C30 90-91 being a line from point of fixation through nodal point to fovea) intersect at the point of fixation, and the image of this point is formed upon the fovea of each retina. ® Monocular. A ig point of fixation. The image of B, A Is point of fixation. The Image of S to the right of A, would fall on the retina would fall on corresponding points on the at B' on the nasal side of the fovea, F. right and left retinae. The image of 0 would fall on O’ on the temporal side of the fovea. ® Binocular. Figueb 10.—Projection, 91. Field of fixation.—a. Definition.—“The field of fixation may be defined as the base of a cone, the apex of which is the center of rotation of the eyeball as the eye is rotated to its extreme limits in all directions, foveal vision at all times marking or defining the base of the cone” (Peter), Thus, foveal or central vision is limited to within the field of fixation, is limited by the possible amount of rotation of the globe about its center. The average field of fixation extends upward 33°, up and in 47°, inward 50°, in and down 47°, downward 57°, downward and outward 47°, and outward 45°. The field of fixation, like the visual field for form, is limited on the medial side by the contour of the nose, so that a proper determina- tion of the maximum degree of adduction of the eye is difficult to determine. 271818°—40 8 113 TM 8-300 91 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT 6. Binocular fixation.—The field of binocular fixation may be con- sidered as consisting of the overlapping portions of the right and left fields of fixation. It is only within the binocular field of fixa- tion that binocular single vision is possible, and it is only within this area that the factors which operate with binocular single vision in depth perception (physiological diplopia, binocular parallax, and con- Ftgttrh 11.—Composite field of binocular vision of 50 men, showing concentric constric- tion when flying goggle B-6 is worn (outer line normal field ; shaded area field with goggie worn). vergence) can be employed. Therefore accuracy in depth percep- tion is limited to the field of binocular fixation. We may say that the field of binocular fixation is limited by the nasal field of fixation of each eye. This fact should be remembered and given considera- tion in aviation goggle design. 114 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 91-92 c. Normal 'position.—Normally, the eyes are held in their proper position in the orbits by the combined action of the extrinsic ocular muscles and are so acted upon by these muscles that the visual lines are held practically parallel. d. Diplopia.—While the extrinsic ocular muscles hold the eyes in their proper positions, these muscles in turn are directed to their action by nervous impulses originating in the fusion centers. These fusion centers operate to maintain binocular single vision under all normal conditions. In order to maintain such vision, the lines must be held in their proper relation to each other. When visual lines fail in this respect, diplopia or double vision results. 92. Orthophoria and heterophoria.—When toe action of the fusion centers is weakened or abolished, normally balanced eyes will maintain parallel visual lines (orthophoria). If the visual lines deviate from the parallel under these conditions, heterophoria exists, that is, a deviation becoming manifest only when fusion control is weakened or abolished. If the visual lines deviate from the parallel while the fusion centers are presumably functioning, and fixation is maintained with one eye only, heterotropia or squint exists, that is, a manifest or obvious deviation of the visual lines occurring inde- pendent of the action of the fusion center. The term heterophoria includes all varieties of latent tendencies to deviation from parallel positions of the visual axes. In heterotropia the deviations are mani- fest and do not have to be uncovered by weakening the fusion con- trol. The following table of nomenclature is generally accepted: a. Orthophoria.—Normal binocular balance. b. Heterophoria.—A latent imbalance, or tendency toward devia- tion of the two visual lines from parallel. It includes the following: (1) Esophoria.—A latent deviation of the visual lines inward. (2) Exophoria.—A latent deviation of the visual lines outward. (3) Hyperphoria.—-A latent deviation of the visual lines of one eye above that of the other. It is designated as right or left. (4) Hypophoria.—A latent deviation of the visual lines of one eye below that of the other. Designated as right or left, (5) Double hyperphoria.—A latent elevation of both visual lines. Also termed anaphoria. (6) Double hypophoria.—A latent lowering of both visual lines. Also termed kataphoria. (7) Cyclophoria.—A latent deviation of the globe about its antero- posterior axis, that is, an intorsion (rotation of the upper portion of the cornea about the antero-posterior axis of the globe nasally or 115 TM 8-300 92-93 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT inward) or an extorsion (outward rotation), necessarily designated as right or left. c. Heterotropia.—A manifest deviation of the visual lines (as de- fined previously). (1) Esotropia.—A manifest deviation of the visual lines inward; a convergent squint. (2) Exotropia.—A manifest deviation of the visual lines outward; a divergent squint. (3) Hypertropia.—A manifest deviation of one visual line above the other—right or left. (4) Eypotropia.—A manifest deviation of one visual line below the other—right or left. Also termed catatropia. (5) Cyclotropia.—A manifest deviation about the antero-posterior axis. d. Other diagnostic terms used in connection with defective ocular movements are: (1) Hyperkinesis.—Excessive action of an individual muscle. (2) Hypokinesis.—Deficient action of an individual muscle. 93. Review of physiology of extrinsic ocular muscles.—a. Movement of globe.—The globe is capable of limited movements about the center of rotation, these movements being brought about by contractions of the extrinsic ocular muscles. The center of rota- tion is located about 10 millimeters in front of the posterior pole and millimeters behind the anterior pole (Donders). The globe may be rotated about three axes, the horizontal axis (as eleva- tion and depression), the vertical axis (as abduction and adduction) «nd the antero-posterior axis (intorsion and extorsion). b. Action of muscles.—Certain of the extrinsic ocular muscles have but one action from the position of “eyes front,” these being the external rectus (abduction, external rotation) and internal rectus (adduction, internal rotation). Each other muscle has a primary and subsidiary action. Taking them up in order we find that the vertical recti (superior and inferior) in addition to their vertical action have an adducting or internal rotating effect because of their origins being located more medially than their insertion. In addi- tion each has some effect in rotating the globe about its antero- posterior axis. Because of the forward and medial origin of the obliques and their insertions behind the center of rotation, each is an abductor in addition to its elevating and depressing and torsion actions. The primary and subsidiary actions of the muscles can best be appreciated after a study of diagrams showing their origin and insertions. 116 TM: 8—300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 93 Table of primary and subsidiary actions of individual ocular muscles (Peter) Muscle Primary action Subsidiary action Internal rectus External rectus. Superior rectus Internal rotation External rotation Elevation None. None. f Internal rotation. \lntorsion. f Internal rotation. \Extorsion. j Depression. \ External rotation, f Elevation. 1 External rotation. Inferior rectus Depression Superior oblique. Intorsion Inferior oblique __ _ Extorsion AB. Orbital axis. CF. Optical axis. M. Center of rotation. Pigdbe 12.—Planes of action of extrinsic ocular muscles. Inferior oblique lies immedi- ately beneath superior oblique, and inferior rectus immediately below superior rectus. 117 TM 8-300 93 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT c. Synergists and antagonists.—Xo movement of the globe is ac- complished by the action of an individual muscle alone. Those that aid one another in a movement are called synergists. For every movement made by the globe there is a diametrically opposed move- ment. The muscles that oppose one another in contraction may be termed antagonists. Figure 13.—Diagram showing action of individual muscles in the six cardinal directions of gaze from position of “eyes front.” 118 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 93 Table of synergists (Peter) Muscle Synergist Internal rectus 1 \ \ \ \ \ Superior rectus. Inferior rectus. Superior oblique. Inferior oblique. Inferior oblique. Internal rectus. Superior oblique. Internal rectus. Inferior rectus. External rectus. Superior rectus. External rectus. External rectus. _ _ Superior rectus Inferior rectus _ _ _ __ __ Superior oblique . Inferior oblique _ _ Figure 14.—Diagram showing conjugate or yoke action of extrinsic ocular muscles ia binocular movements. 119 STM 8-COO ©3-95 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Table of direct antagonists (Peter) Muscle Antagonists Internal met,us /'External rectus. < Superior oblique. /Inferior oblique. /Internal rectus. < Superior rectus. /Inferior rectus. JInferior rectus. \ Superior oblique. /Superior rectus. /Inferior oblique. /Inferior oblique. /Superior rectus, f Superior oblique. /Inferior rectus. External rectus Superior rectus Inferior rectus ' Superior oblique __ Inferior oblique _ 94. Binocular movements.—When binocular movements are considered it is found that a change of position from “eyes front” to a different position within the field of binocular fixation is accom- plished by the combined action of muscles of each eye. The muscles that by their contraction maintain the two visual lines parallel in combined movements of the two eyes are termed conjugate or yoke muscles. For example, in looking toward the right from the position “eyes front” the right external rectus and left internal rectus contract. Below is a table showing the conjugate or yoke muscles brought into play in movements in the six cardinal directions from the position of “eyes front.” Table of conjugate muscles Direction Muscles To right Right external rectus and left internal rectus. Left external rectus and right internal rectus. Right inferior oblique and left superior rectus. Right superior oblique and left inferior rectus. Left inferior oblique and right superior rectus. Left superior oblique and right inferior rectus. To left Up and to right Down and to right. Up and to left Down and to left 95. Prisms.—In the recognition of heterophoria and heterotropia Idle use of prisms is of great value, particularly in arriving at a quanti- 120 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AV1IATTON MEDICINE 95-96 tative diagnosis. Therefore a consideration of the optical properties of prisms is now essential. A prism may be defined as a portion of a refracting medium bounded by two plane surfaces which are inclined at a finite angle. The two plane surfaces are called faces, the line of their intersection the edge, the angle between the faces the apical angle, and the side opposite this angle the base. If a prism is of a denser medium than that surround- ing it (for example, glass in air), a ray of light on entering it will be bent toward the base as it enters the prism, upon passing through the denser medium and reentering the rarer medium, the emergent ray will further be bent toward the base as it leaves the prism. When the ray of light, while passing through a prism, is parallel to the base, it is said to traverse the prism symmetrically and in this case the angles of incidence and emergence are equal. The total amount of deviation between the incident ray and the emergent ray is called the angle of deviation. A ray of light passing through is deviated toward the base, and the image of an object seen through a prism is projected or displaced toward its apex (Duke Elder). 96. Numbering of prisms (Peter).—As to the nomenclature of prisms, three methods have been proposed and used: a. Numerals indicating angle in degrees formed hy the two refract- ing surfaces or faces.—This method is unsatisfactory inasmuch as the amount of deviation produced varies with the index of refraction of the material as well as the angle of the apex, or in other words depends upon the material from which the prism is made in addition to the angle of the apex. Therefore, it does not express the amount of deviation undergone by a ray of light passing through the prism. h. The centrad method of Dennett.—The unit, centrad, is designated symbolically by the Greek letter delta inverted (triangle base up). A prism of 1 centrad value will deviate a ray of light l/100th part of the arc of the radian. The radian of the arc is obtained by measuring off on the circumference of a circle a distance equal to the radius of the arc. A radian equals 57.295°, therefore one centrad, l/100th part of the radian, is 0.57295°. This unit of measure is constant. c. Prentice's method of prism diopter.—Designated by the Greek letter delta (triangle base downward). A prism of 1 diopter’s strength will deviate a ray of light 1 centimeter, at 1 meter’s distance, or 1 inch at 100 inches’ distance. It is purely a tangential measurement. One prism diopter equals 34.3764B minutes. This unit is not constant, that is, a 10-diopter prism does not equal 10 times a 1-diopter prism, but equals 5°42.6355'. The two units of measure, centrad and diopter, are so nearly equal up to 20 that either system may be used with ap- 121 TM 8-300 96-98 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT proximately equal accuracy. But beyond 20, while the centrad con- tinues to be uniform the prism diopter loses in value. The prism diopter is the method we shall use in the quantitative determination of heterophoria. 97. Rotary prism.—If two prisms of equal value, for example 10 diopters, are placed together base to apex, one will neutralize the other. If one is rotated against the other until the two bases are together there will be a prism equal in value to the sum of the two, that is, 20 diopters. If the two prisms of equal strength are placed together, one (a) base up and the other (h) base down and rotate them against one another in opposite directions an equal amount, that is, {a) with base up and out at 45°, and (b) base down and out at 135°, there will be a prism of a certain value base out only, as the base up and base down effects are neutralized. This illustrates the principle employed in the construction of the Risley rotary prism; the two prisms are rotated in opposite directions by a rack and pinion arrangement so that when properly calibrated any prismatic effect may be obtained from zero to the sum of the two. Further, the base may be placed in any position desired, that is, base up or down, in or out. A rotary prism amounts to a case of simple prisms and, while a great convenience and time saver, is not essential in the determina- tion of heterophoria. The prisms from the trial lens case may be used quite satisfactorily if a rotary prism is not available. 98. Maddox rod.—The Maddox rod is used in the determination of the existence of heterophoria. In its simplest form it is a section of small glass rod, probably 4 or 5 millimeters in diameter, mounted in an opaque black disc which is fitted in a trial lens frame. The type found on the usual phorometer trial frame is multiple, or compound, that is, several rods superimposed with the axes parallel and within the same plane. It is believed that the multiple, or compound, Maddox rod gives sharper definition. The Maddox rod, either single or com- pound, refracts only in one meridian and that at right angles to its axis. If a small luminous point is seen through a Maddox rod the point appears as a luminous line at right angles to the axis of the rod. When the rod is placed in a horizontal position before the eye, and a luminous point seen through it, the luminous point appears as a vertical line of light. When the Maddox rod is placed before an eye, the retinal image of a luminous point is therefore distorted. If a Maddox rod is placed before the left eye and the right fixed upon a luminous point of light (1-centimeter lamp at distance of 20 feet), the left retinal image of the luminous point is so distorted that it is not recognized as the same object seen with the right eye, fusion control 122 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 98-99 is greatly weakened or reduced, and the left visual line will follow the lines of least resistance. If a muscular imbalance exists, the eye behind the rod will deviate inward or outward, etc., as the case may be. We may say then that the Maddox rod has the optical effect of converting a point of light into a line of light at right angles to its axis, and has the psychological effect of weakening or abolishing in part, at least, fusion control. 99. Phorometer trial frame.—a. Description.—The phorometer trial frame as used routinely in the examination of Air Corps per- sonnel consists of a triple cell trial frame, on© cell capable of being rotated, as for cylindrical lenses, a Risley rotary prism, and a com- pound Maddox rod for each eye. The rotary prisms and the Maddox rods can be swung into place, that is, one each before each triple cell, as desired. The rotary prisms may be adjusted to any position, A Single Maddox rod. B Leveling device. C Bubble. D Triple cell (one rotary) for trial lenses. E Risley rotary prism. F Multiple Maddox rod. O Adjustment for interpupillary distance. Figure 15.—Phorometer trial frame and single Maddox rod. 123 TM 8-300 100-101 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT base up or down, in or out, as desired, and with it any prismatic effect, from 0 to 30 diopters may be obtained. The Maddox rods may be placed with the axes in any meridian, being capable of being rotated through 180°. The instrument is equipped with a level bubble and leveling device, has an adjustable forehead rest, is adjustable for inter- pupillary distance, and mounted on a telescoping floor stand or wall bracket. b. Use.—It is believed advisable that first the student disregard the phorometer trial frame and use only the equipment from the trial lens case in arriving at conclusions regarding heterophoria in order to realize the optical principles involved. Later the phorometer trial frame may be used as a matter of convenience, but as has been stated, it is not essential. 100. Sighting eye.—Before beginning the tests for the detection of heterophoria, it is necessary that the sighting or fixing eye be determined. When an object is fixed it is habitually done with one eye, while the other eye adjusts itself to take up fixation after this act has been accomplished by the former. The eye that sights an object first is referred to as the sighting, fixing, or directing eye. As a rule, a right-handed person will sight with his right eye, and a left-handed person with his left. However, this rule is not infalli- ble, and too much reliance should not be placed on it. Assuming that the eye one employs habitually for sighting is the more steady or nondeviating of the two, it is advisable, therefore, when measuring deviations to allow the examinee to sight with the eye he customarily employs for that purpose. When this is observed the tests are carried out with the nonsighting eye, as this is the eye that deviates more readily should any deviation occur. Investigations show that the findings are considerably more consistent when this procedure is followed. 101. Technique of determining imbalance.—a. Determining sighting eye.—For determining the sighting eye, a blank card about 13 by 20 centimeters, wTith a 1.6-centimeter round hole in the center is employed. The examinee is seated facing the spotlight 6 meters away; he grasps the card by the short side with both hands. While looking intently at the light, he slowly raises the card at arm’s length and locates the light through the hole, and the eye selected for this purpose is the one used habitually for sighting and fixing. b. Adjustment of trial frame.—In using equipment from the trial lens case, adjust the spectacle trial frame on the examinee so that it is comfortably worn and so that the cells are properly placed as to interpupillary distance. 124 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 101-102 c. Imbalance in horizontal meridian.—For determining an imbalance in the horizontal meridian, that is, exophoria or esophoria, place the Maddox rod in the cell before the nonsighting eye so that the axis of the rod is horizontal. Adjust the muscle lamp, or spot of light at 20 feet distance so that its diameter is about 1 centimeter, and switch off all lights in the vicinity of this area. Direct the examinee to fix upon the spot of light, and for a few seconds at a time alternately cover and uncover the nonfixing eye with an opaque card, allowing the fixing eye to maintain fixation constantly. The momentary covering of the nonfixing eye aids further in weakening of fusion control. d. Orthophoria.—If orthophoria exists the visual line of the non- fixing eye will not deviate and the vertical line of light will be seen as passing through or bisecting the spotlight. e. Heterophoria.—If heterophoria (in this case exophoria or eso- phoria) is present, the vertical line of light will be seen to the right or left of the spotlight. f. Homonymous diplopia.—If the line of light is seen on the same side (homonymous diplopia) of the examinee as the Maddox rod is placed (for example, the Maddox rod before the left eye and the line is seen to the left of the spotlight), the qualitative diagnosis of esopho- ria is made, and in order to make a quantitative determination a prism must be used base out. Homonymous diplopia indicates esophoria, the amount of which is estimated by the use of a prism before the non- fixing eye, with base out or toward the temporal side. Place a weak prism, that is, of 1 diopter, before the nonfixing eye, and repeat the procedure of covering and uncovering the nonfixing eye. It will be found that perhaps the line of light has moved nearer the spot of light but still does not bisect it. Replace the prism with a stronger one until the line of light passes through the spot of light and the strength of the prism used indicates the amount of esophoria in prism diopters. g. Crossed diplopia.—If the line of light is seen on the opposite side, as for example when the Maddox rod is before the left eye and the line is seen to the right (crossed diplopia), then a qualitative diagnosis of exophoria is made. A crossed diplopia indicates exophoria, the amount of which is estimated by the use of a prism before the nonfixing eye with base in or toward the nasal side. The procedure is the same as with esophoria except that the prism is used base in. The strength of the prism used to cause the line of light to bisect or pass through the spot of light indicates the amount of exophoria in prism diopters. 102. Optical principles involved in horizontal deviations.— a. Orthophoria.—The sighting eye fixes the spotlight; the nonsighting eye, having before it the Maddox rod, sees the spotlight as a vertical 125 TM 8-300 102-103 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT line of light. If no deviation is uncovered (orthophoria), the rays of light from the spotlight will fall upon the foveae of both eyes and be projected into space to the point actually occupied by the light. When this occurs, the line of light will be seen passing directly through the spotlight. h. Deviating eye.—As the Maddox rod possesses the power of weak- ening the action of the fusion centers, the eye behind the Maddox rod will deviate if it has a tendency to do so, provided the action of the fusion centers is reduced below the urge to deviate. c. Esophoria.—Assuming the eyes have a tendency to deviate in- ward (esophoria), and the right eye is the nonsighting eye, the prin- ciples involved remain unchanged insofar as the sighting eye is con- cerned, but with the nonsighting (right eye in this case) they are different. The rays of light enter the eye as in orthophoria, but in- stead of falling upon the fovea they fall upon the retina on its nasal side. This occurs because the cornea is rotated inward, and the fovea is rotated outward. Therefore, following the law of projection, the image of the line of light being formed upon the nasal retina will be projected to the temporal field and will be seen as a vertical line upon the right side of the spotlight (homonymous diplopia). d. Measurement of deviation.—In order to measure the amount of deviation that has been uncovered, prisms are utilized to refract the rays of light entering the deviating eye, outward or toward the temple until they fall upon the fovea. Rays of light passing through a prism are bent or refracted toward its base and the object seen through the prism is projected toward its apex. Therefore, increasing prisms placed base out before the deviating eye will refract the rays toward the temporal side, until they eventually reach the fovea, and at the same time the line of light will be seen to move over toward the nasal field until it passes directly through the spotlight. When this is accom- plished the deviation is corrected by the refraction of the rays of light, the eye remaining stationary. The strength of prism required to accomplish this represents, in prism diopters, the amount of deviation uncovered at that time. 103. Vertical deviations.—a. Hyperphoria.—Ordinarily hyper- phoria only is used as a diagnostic term where there exists a deviation of the visual lines in the vertical meridian, and the designation is made as to right or left, depending upon which of the visual lines is the higher, or which tends to deviate upward in comparison with its fellow. For the measurement of hyperphoria the Maddox rod is adjusted before the nonfixing eye with its axis vertical, hence the line of light is seen as horizontal. The nonfixing eye is alternately cov- TM 8-300 XOTES OX EYE, EAR. ETC., IX AVIATIOX MEDICIXE 103-104 ered and uncovered, and if the line of light is seen passing through the spot of light there is no hyperphoria. If the line of light is seen below the spot of light there is a hyperphoria of the eye behind the Maddox rod, and the strength of prism used base down before this eye which causes the line to pass through, or bisect, the spot of light represents the deviation in prism diopters. If the line of light is seen above the spotlight a hyperphoria of the fixing eye is indicated, and the strength of prism base up before the eye behind the Maddox rod represents the amount of deviation in prism diopters. Suppose the right eye to be the fixing eye; the Maddox rod is adjusted with axis vertical in the spectacle trial frame. The left eye is covered and momentarily un- covered repeatedly. Further, suppose that, in this instance, when fusion is weakened the left visual line turns upward (left hyper- phoria). Then with the left eye elevated, its fovea would be depressed and at a lower level than the fovea of the right eye. In this instance the image of the horizontal line of light would be formed above the fovea of the right eye, and when an image is formed above the fovea it is projected into the visual field below the point of fixation. Therefore, the examinee would see the spot of light with the right eye and the line of light with the left, and the line would be projected below the spot of light, indicating, in this case, a left hyperphoria. By using a prism of sufficient strength the position of the image of the line of light may be shifted from above the fovea to the level of the fovea, and the line of light will be seen as passing through the spot. The strength of the prism required indicates the amount of deviation in prism diopters. b. Heterophoria.—Where the phorometer trial frame with rotary prism is used, in testing for heterophoria, the same procedure is used except that the examinee, by turning the milled thumbscrew control- ling the rotary prism, adjusts the prisms himself so that the line of light passes through the spotlight. The nonfixing eye is alternately cov- ered and uncovered. In horizontal deviations where the prism is adjusted base in (nasally) an exophoria is indicated, where the prism is adjusted base out (temporally) an esophoria is indicated. c. Amount of deviation.—In vertical deviations the prism adjusted base down indicates a hyperphoria of the eye behind the Maddox rod. Where it is adjusted base up a hyperphoria of the opposite eye is indicated. In all instances the amount of deviation is read in prism diopters as the indicator shows on the markings on the rotary prism. 104. Heterophoria at 33 centimeters.—a. This test is not done routinely. The test is carried out in exactly the same manner as the test at 6 meters except that a small electric lamp (ophthalmoscope 127 TM 8-300 104 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT without head) is held before the examinee at the level of his eyes. This is a test for imbalance at the ordinary reading distance, and may give the examiner some information as to the existence of refractive error, insufficiency of convergence, and may be indicative of a reduction of fusion control at usual reading distance. It will be frequently found that an examinee who shows a marked exophoria at 33 centimeters will also show a low angle of convergence, and under stress of fatigue may exhibit a diplopia (crossed) at reading distance. Right eye fixing F'—L in left visual axis, F’ the left fovea. Image of A falls on A' to nasal side of fovea and is projected at B. Homonymous diplopia. Figubb 16.—Esophoria. h. Cyclophoria.—The existence of a cyclophoria is not investigated as routine, but only when considered as necessary by the examiner. In testing for cyclophoria, two Maddox rods with the axes exactly vertical, one before each eye, may be used. With the spotlight at 20 feet a horizontal line of light is seen with each eye, and in order to prevent fusion, these may be separated by a strong prism (8 diopters) base up or down before one of the eyes. If there is a latent tendency 128 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 104-105 toward intorsion or extorsion in either eye, one of the lines will appear as at an angle with the other or slanted, and the eye intorted or extorted may be determined by the line that does not appear hori- zontal. For example, if the prism, base down, is before the left eye the upper line is the one seen with the left eye, etc. But with an in- torsion the line of light will appear as slanting outward and down- ward, and with an extorsion the line will appear as slanting inward and downward. Right eye fixing L-F’, left visual axis. Image A falls on A', which is on temporal side of left fovea and therefore is projected at D. Crossed diplopia. Figueh 17.—Exophoria. 105. Etiology of heterophoria.—a. Deviation.—As previously stated the eyes are held in their proper positions in the orbits and in the proper relation to one another by the combined action of all of the extrinsic ocular muscles. Why one eye will deviate, when the action of the fusion centers is partially abolished, is not clear. There seems to be, for some reason or other, an urge of one or more of the 271818°—40 9 129 TM: 8-300 105 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT extrinsic muscles to overact or to underact. This action will cause the eyes to deviate from the parallel whenever released from control of the fusion centers. b. Cause.—If the urge for binocular single vision is greater than the urge to deviate, the condition of heterophoria will exist; if the urge for binocular single vision is less than the urge to deviate, heterotropia will exist. If fusion control is habitually weak, or is easily weakened by artificial measures, the more significant will be deviation. If fusion control is active and not easily weakened, the existing latent devia- tion will be unimportant, at least under normal conditions. There must be taken into consideration, too, the possibility of anatomical defects. It may be that one muscle is actually longer or shorter than the normal. c. Factors.—Whatever the primary cause of a latent deviation may be, certain factors tend to increase its tendency, and produce the con- dition of muscular asthenopia. Probably the most common factor encountered is an error in refraction, whether it be myopic, hyperopic, or astigmatic. This is particularly true when accumulative fatigue is superimposed. The fatigue may be induced by the refractive errors alone, or it may be secondary to them, originating in nervous or physical stress, chronic infection, etc. d. Latent deviations.—Latent deviations of low degree, at 6 meters, such as 2 prism diopters of esophoria, 1 of exophoria, and y2 of hyper- phoria, may be considered ordinarily as orthophoria. Latent devia- tions higher than these are in themselves of little significance, and can be considered worthy of attention only after all the associated factors which apply to the type of deviation have been investigated. e. Factors to be considered.—(1) In all forms of latent deviations there must be considered: (a) Ease with which fusion control is weakened. (b) Efficiency with which visual lines are maintained parallel while moving in the six cardinal positions. (c) Errors in refraction of one-half diopter or higher. (2) If esophoria is exhibited there must be considered in addition to the above factors: (a) Power of abduction (prism divergence;. (b) Amount of accommodation that can be exerted. (3) If exophoria is exhibited, the power of adduction (angle of convergence or meter angle) must be taken into consideration in addition. /. Increased tendency to deviate.—When the tendency to deviate increases, because of errors in refraction and fatigue, this increase 130 TM 8-300 105 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE will be accompanied, generally, by defects in one or more of the associated factors, which apply to the type of deviation. The asso- ciated parallel movements are usually the first to exhibit evidences of ocular strain, which is manifested by diplopia in the primary, or in one or all of the cardinal positions on the tangential plane. For example, an individual exhibiting an exephoria of 2 prism diopters, a refractive error of 1 diopter, divergence power of 5 prism diopters, and an angle of convergence of 50°, may, after the onset of fatigue, exhibit a tendency to deviate 6 or 8 diopters, exhibit diplopia in the lateral positions on the tangential plane, and if fatigue continues, divergence power may increase to 10 diopters while the convergence power drops to 30°, g. Return to original level.—After correcting the refractive error, thereby inducing ocular rest, all these factors tend to return to their original level, the defective parallel movements being the first to recover. Horizontal deviations respond much more readily after correction of existing refractive errors than do the vertical deviations. h. Refractive errors.—It is a generally accepted theory that many cases of heterophoria are due entirely to refractive errors. It is believed, however, that all cases of heterophoria exist independent of refractive errors and that the errors, when fatigue is superimposed, only exaggerate the deviation without being the primary cause. i. Accommodative theory.—The accommodative theory is as follows: Esophoria and exophoria are manifestations of muscular asthenopia due to hyperopia and myopia, respectively. This occurs because there is believfed to be a definite relation or nervous balance between accom- modation and convergence, with accommodation the dominating or controlling influence. In emmetropia, when the eyes accommodate 1 diopter, convergence is sufficient to cross the visual lines at 1 meter distance, that is, 1 meter angle of convergence is exerted, at which point the obj ect focused upon is located. When this occurs the nervous balance between accommodation and convergence is equal. j. Hyperopia.—In the hyperope accommodation exceeds conver- gence. In order to focus an object at 1 meter distance the eyes may have to accommodate sufficiently to focus a normal eye at 50 centi- meters. When such accommodation occurs the eyes are still required to converge 1 meter angle, that is, the eyes are accommodating 2 diopters while converging only 1 meter angle. The excessive or unused stimulation to converge to 50 centimeters (2 meter angles) is present, and this may lead to convergence excess. In order to maintain binoc- ular single vision the individual must overcome this convergence excess by increased action of his external recti muscles, which is initiated 131 TM 8-300 105-106 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT by the fusion centers. When the action of the fusion centers is low- ered, one eye will deviate inward as stimulation of the external recti is no longer great enough to maintain parallelism of the visual lines. Continuing further under accumulative fatigue, the external recti can no longer maintain their increased action; therefore, the con- vergence excess pulls one eye inward. This deviation will occur, in the early stages of fatigue, only when the eyes are turned in one or all of the cardinal positions, but in the more advanced stages deviation may occur while the eyes are in the primary position and independent of the action of the fusion centers. k. Myopia.—In myopia the reverse occurs. Accommodation is less than convergence. When a myope fixes an object at 50 centimeters distance, he may be accommodating only enough to focus an em- metropic eye at 1 meter, that is, he is accommodating 1 diopter while converging 2 meter angles. When this occurs the urge to converge is weak, as accommodation is the dominating impulse. The external recti soon take advantage of the weak convergence stimulation and divergence excess results. When the action of the fusion centers is lowered one eye will deviate outward because of underaction of the internal recti, due probably to lowered muscular tonicity. Under accumulative fatigue the internal recti cannot overcome the pull of the external recti and deviation will occur; first, when the eyes are turned in one or all of the cardinal positions, and later, when in the primary position, independent of the action of the fusion centers (squint). I. A dual findings.—Although the foregoing is the theory generally accepted in reference to heterophoria., and squint resulting from errors in refraction, in actual practice it does not hold true at all times. Twenty-three cases with hyperopic errors exhibited esophoria in six- teen instances and exophoria in seven. All of these cases developed muscular asthenopic symptoms, under accumulative fatigue, with con- siderable increase in deviation. When the errors in refraction were corrected, symptoms disappeared and the deviations returned to their former levels. It will be noted that in this small series of cases above 30 percent of hyperopes exhibited exophoria which was influenced by the refractive errors. 106. Incidence of heterophoria.—In a series of 500 unselected cases, 244, or 48.8 percent, exhibited a horizontal imbalance above the amount considered as orthophoria; 132, or 25.6 percent, of this series exhibited a vertical imbalance; 25, or 10.24 percent, of the cases showing a horizontal imbalance exhibited, after the onset of fatigue, an increase in the deviation (or a greater amount could be uncovered), and devel- oped defects in one or all of the associated factors. 132 TM 8-300 107 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 107. Other methods of detecting heterophoria.—a. Cover test.—The examinee fixes the spotlight at 6 meters’ distance. A card is held before one eye for a few seconds, then quickly removed. If exophoria exists the eye behind the card will deviate outward, and when the card is removed the eye will be seen to swing inward and take up fixation with the fixing eye. If esophoria exists the covered eye will deviate inward, and will swing outward when card is removed to take up fixation as in exophoria. The same applies to hyperphoria. This test is repeated by fixing an object or light at 33 centimeters’ distance. This test cannot always be depended upon, as considerable heterophoria may exist before it can be exhibited by this method. l>. Diplopia or displacement tests*.— (1) Horizontal or lateral im- balance (esophoria or exophoria).—The trial frame is adjusted to the examinee and a strong prism, 8 or 10 diopters, is placed base down before the right eye. The spot of light (muscle lamp) is seen at a distance of 20 feet and a diplopia is induced, the upper light being the image projected by the right eye and the lower light that projected by the left. If there is a condition of orthophoria the two images will be seen as separated in the vertical meridian only. In heterophoria there will be a lateral displacement of the two images as well as vertical. If the upper image is displaced to the right (homonymous diplopia), an esophoria is indicated, and if to the left, an exophoria. A quantitative determination is made with prisms, base in or out, in a manner similar to that where the Maddox rod is employed. (2) Vertical deviations.—A strong prism is placed, base in, in front of the right eye (the prism must be of such strength that diplopia is induced, and as the eyes have a stronger converging than diverging power, base in is suggested). The image seen by the right eye will be displaced to the right (toward the apex of the prism), and if no hyperphoria exists the two images will be seen in the same horizontal plane. If the right image is higher than the left, a left hyperphoria is indicated; if the right image is lower than the left, condition is right hyperphoria. Where left hyperphoria is found the quantitative estimate may be made by prisms, placed base down before the left eye until both images are seen at the same level. In right hyperphoria the determination is made by prisms base up before the left eye until both images are at the same level (Peter). c. Maddox double prism.—The Maddox double prism (two 4- diopter prisms, base together, in a trial lens frame) may be used in determining heterophoria. The Maddox double prism causes a monocular diplopia, an image being displaced toward the apex of each prism. When the spot of light is used at 20 feet with the Maddox double prism before one eye, three images are seen, the 133 TM 8-300 107-108 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT upper and lower being images of the eye behind the double prism and the middle image that of the fellow eye. In orthophoria the three spots of light are seen in line; where heterophoria exists the middle image will appear displaced, that is, to right or left, above or below (depending upon the position of the double prism). Lat- eral and vertical deviations may be determined and prisms used to arrive at a quantitative conclusion. d. Red lens test.—This test furnishes valuable corroborative evi- dence in examining for heterophoria and in some cases of hetero- tropia. (1) The examinee is seated facing a blackboard, wall, or other flat surface at a distance of 75 centimeters. A red glass is placed in front of the right eye. Care should be taken to see that the glass is large enough and held in such position as to be in front of the pupil in all the movements of the eye. (2) A small spotlight is now carried over the surface of the blackboard from the point of fixation along the eight cardinal merid- ians for a distance of 50 centimeters. In the normal individual the red and white lights remain fused as a pink light; in cases of heterophoria with low fusion; faulty; or in cases of actual hetero- tropia which do not suppress, a red and a white light will be seen. If the red glass is in front of the right eye and the red light is seen on the right side of the white light, then we have homonymous diplopia. If the red light appears on the left side, we have crossed diplopia. (3) The position at which diplopia occurs can be reported in dis- tances from the point of fixation or, by referring to the table in the chapter on the tangent rule, the distances in centimeters may be converted into degrees. Diplopia occurring within 50 centimeters of the point of fixation is considered to be disqualifying for flying applicants. 108. Associated factors.—As previously stated, the associated factors are vitally important in determining the significance of heterophoria. The factors to be considered are— a. Power of abduction. b. Power of adduction. c. Associated parallel movements. d. Accommodation. e. Errors in refraction. /. The power of fusion. Only those factors pertaining to the recti muscles will be consid- ered here. Accommodation and errors in refraction will be con- sidered in their respective sections. 134 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 109-111 109. Power of abduction (prism divergence).—The normal power of abduction ranges between 3 and 6 prism diopters with an average of 4. When a low prism divergence is exhibited (below 4 diopters) associated with an esophoria, it indicates an overaction of the internal recti muscles, an underaction of the external recti, or both. At the same time, the power or urge of the fusion centers may be weak. The examinee is seated facing a spotlight 6 meters away. The rotary prisms of the phorometer trial frame are adjusted before the eye so that wdien the milled screw operating the prism is turned toward the nose, the prisms will be acting base in, thereby placing the apex over the external rectus muscle. With the prism set at zero on the scale, the examinee should see but one spotlight. As the prisms are slowly rotated, base in, by the examiner, diplopia will be produced. The number of prism diopters which cause the onset of diplopia is read from the scale and entered on the record as abduction or prism divergence. The prisms should be turned steadily and at slow speed. 110. Optical principles involved.—The test is based upon the fact that the muscle lying beneath the apex of a prism is stimulated to action while the other remains passive, insofar as the action of the prism is concerned, and the urge of the fusion centers to main- tain binocular single vision. Therefore, in this test care must be taken that the fusion sense is not impaired and that the prisms are accurately placed. The rays of light enter the eyes, fall upon the fovea of each, and the retinal images of the light are projected in the point actually occupied by the light, and app°ar as one. The prism being operated, base in, before one eye will refract the rays of light entering that eye toward the nasal side of the fovea. The fusion centers strive to maintain binocular single vision and in order to accomplish this the external rectus is stimulated to contract. This contraction rotates the cornea outward, and the posterior pole and fovea inward. In this way the refracted rays continue to fall upon the fovea and binocular vision is maintained. However, a point is reached, eventually, when the external rectus can no longer rotate the eye outward; the rays continue to be progressively refracted and soon travel beyond fovea, thus inducing diplopia. When diplopia occurs, the fusion centers instantly lose their urge for binocular single vision and the eye rotates back to its normal position. This test should not be repeated more than two or three times as the strain on the external rectus causes considerable discomfort. 111. Power of adduction.—There are three principal methods of measuring the power of adduction, namely— 135 TM 8-300 111-115 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT a. Prism convergence. b. Angle of convergence. c. Meter angle. The power of convergence should be, normally, three times as great as divergence, that is, 1 to 3, or 8 to 24. 112. Prism convergence.—This test is conducted in the same way as that for prism divergence except that the prism is placed base out, thereby placing the apex over the internal rectus. The results of this test are not as satisfactory as those of divergence, for there occurs a marked inconsistency in the findings. It is probable that the factor of accommodation plays an important part in convergence. The amount of accomodation with the muscle lamp at 20 feet is negligible. 113. Angle of convergence.—This method is much more satis- factory than prism convergence. The angle is computed from the near point of convergence (PcB) and interpupillary distance {Pd). The near point of convergence is represented by the symbol PcB, meaning the near point of convergence on the base line. The meas- urement is made from an imaginary line connecting the centers of rotation of the two eyes, situated 13.5 millimeters behind the anterior surface of the cornea. The point to be obtained is to determine the greatest amount of convergence that can be exerted and still main- tain binocular single vision. 114. Near point of convergence.—The end of the Prince rule, or a modification of the same, is placed edge up at the side of the nose ll!/2 millimeters in front of the anterior surface of the cornea. A white-headed pin is held 33 centimeters in the median line above the edge of the rule, and the examinee is instructed to look at it intently. If both eyes are seen to converge upon the pin, it is then carried in the median line, along the edge of the rule, towards the root of the nose. The examinee’s eyes are carefully watched, and the instant one is observed „o swing outward the limit of convergence has been reached. The point on the rule opposite the pin is then read in millimeters. This test is repeated until a fairly constant reading is obtained. To the reading thus obtained 25 millimeters are added (the center of rotation is 13.5 mm. behind the cornea, and the end of the rule is placed 11.6 mm. in front of cornea, making in all 26 mm.), which gives the distance from the near point of con- vergence to the base line. The normal eyes should be able to con- verge to 80 millimeters or less. A near point more remote than 80 millimeters indicates an underaction of the internal recti. 115. Interpupillary distance.—a. The examiner stands with his back to the light, face to face with the examinee. The rule is laid 136 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 115-116 across the eisjminee’s nose in line with his pupils, as close to the two eyes as possible. The distance is measured from the temporal side of one pupil to the nasal side of the other. The examiner closes his right eye and instructs the examinee to fix his eyes upon his open left. With the eyes in this position a predetermined mark on the rule is placed in line with the nasal border of the examinee’s right pupil. The rule must be held steadily in this position while the examiner opens his right eye and closes his left. The examinee is then instructed to look at the open right eye. The point on the rule in line with the temporal border of the examinee’s left pupil is read in millimeters, and the difference in millimeters between the two points on the rule is the interpupillary distance. h. Angle of convergence.—The following formula is used to com- pute the angle of convergence: Angle of convergence equals one-half the interpupillary distance multiplied by 100 divided by the near point of convergence plus three, thus: y2Pdx 100 10 4 , ~PcB rd = Angle or convergence The above formula for determining the angle of convergence is purely empirical, and it is not accurate from a standpoint of pure mathematics. It is fairly accurate when PcB and Pd are approxi- mately equal. It is suggested that the table showing the angles of convergence with different findings as to Pd and PcB be used instead (see page 152), as this table is computed accurately from tables of tangents. 116, Meter angle.—a. General.—Another convenient method of determining the power of convergence is the employment of the meter angle. This method has all the advantages of the angle of convergence except that the interpupillary distance is not taken into consideration. h. Unit of measurement.—The unit of measurement is obtained by the eyes fixing an object midway between the two eyes at a distance of 1 meter (1,000 mm.). The angle formed by a line joining the object with the center of rotation of either eye makes, with the median line, 1 meter angle. With an interpupillary distance of 64 millimeters, the above meter angle equals about 1.5°. c. Near 'point of convergence.—The near point of convergence is determined as described under angle of convergence. When the number of millimeters obtained is divided into 1,000 the result will equal the number of meter angles of convergence exerted, thus: if the ... , inr. 1,000 millimeters near point of convergence equals 100 millimeters, millimeters ' 137 TM 8-300 116-118 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT equals 10 meter angles. Again, if the near point of convergence , , 1,000 millimeters , „„ , , equals 50 millimeters, then Wf-prs~equals 20 meter anSles- d. Convenience.—This method is especially convenient because, in the emmetrope, the amount of convergence reckoned in meter angles is exactly the same as the amount of accommodation reckoned in diopters. 117. Associated parallel movements.—a. Applicability.—This test is applicable almost exclusively to paresis and paralysis of the ocular muscles, and offers little information where latent errors are concerned. b. Test.—The examinee stands near a window where good illumina- tion falls on both eyes. The examiner holds a white-headed pin about 33 centimeters directly in front of the examinee’s eyes and di- rects him to look at it steadily. % Nystagmus in the primary position is to be noted at this stage of the test. The examinee is then in- structed to hold his head still and watch the pin as it is moved slowly in the eight cardinal positions. Care is taken not to carry the pin beyond the field of binocular fixation. The eyes are inspected to discover any failure in fixing the pin. A lagging or overaction of either eye is noted. c. Interpretation.—A lagging of either eye in any of the eight cardinal positions is due to an underaction of at least one of the extrinsic muscles. It may indicate a paresis or complete paralysis. d. Recording.—The underaction is recorded by stating which eye lags and in which direction the lagging is observed. In the same way any overshooting of either eye is recorded by stating which eye is involved and in which direction. e. Confirmation.—If any underaction or overaction is observed with this test the findings are confirmed on the tangent plane. 118. Squint.—a. Definition.—Squint or strabismus is a term which may be applied to those conditions where there is an obvious or manifest deviation of the visual axes from the normal. There are two general varieties of squint encountered : (1) Concomitant.—Concomitant squint, or strabismus (hetero- tropia) which is characterized by the two visual axes, although ab- normally directed, maintaining their relative position to one another with all ocular movements. (2) Paralytic.—Paralytic squint or strabismus definitely due to a paresis or paralysis of one or more of the extrinsic ocular muscles, in which the two visual axes do not maintain their relative positions in all positions in the field of binocular fixation. There is another 138 TM 8-300 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 118 type of squint due to a defective innervation in which there is an unequal stimulation, rather than paralysis, of conjugate muscles. This latter type may be encountered where there is an unequal stimulation of certain nerve centers as in meningitis, brain tumor, cerebral irritation as in epilepsy, etc. h. Concomitant squint {lieterotropia').—As stated, in concomitant squint the visual axes retain their relation in all positions within the binocular field of fixation, differing in that respect from paralytic squint. In concomitant squint (this term is being more frequently employed clinically than heterotropia) there may be then a con- vergent type (esotropia), divergent type (exotropia), and deviation of the visual axes in the vertical meridian (hypertropia). A Center of rotation, left eye. B Center of rotation, right eye. D Point of maximum convergence. DC Distance from point of convergence to base line (PcB). Interpupillary distance (Pd) is same as AB. Angle of convergence, angle ABB. Figure 18.—Angle of convergence. U) Convergent squint or esotropia.—Space hardly permits a dis- cussion of the etiological factors concerned in esotropia. Associated with such a condition are usually refractive errors, and a defective or absent fusion faculty. The deviating eye may be constant, that is, the deviation is always in the same eye (monocular esotropia), or it may be alternating. In either case the angle of deviation remains practically the same whatever position the fixing eye assumes. With convergent squint there is usually an amblyopia ex anopsia, either 139 TM 8-300 118 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT End of rule is placed 11.5 millimeters In front of cornea, which is 13.5 millimeters in front of center of rotation. Therefore 25 millimeters Is added to findings on rule in determination of PcB. Figobh It).—Point of convergence on base line. 140 Examinee fixes upon left pupil of examiner, who places end of rule before nasal margin of right pupil of examinee. Examinee then fixes upon right pupil of examiner, who notes distance to temporal margin of left pupil of examinee. This distance is same as distance between centers of rotation of two globes. Figure 20.—Measuring interpupillary distance. 141 TM 8-300 118 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT congenital or acquired, of the deviating eye, especially in the monoc- ular variety. With any variety of concomitant squint binocular single vision is impossible, so there must be either a diplopia or a suppression of the image of the deviating eye. The latter almost invariably occurs in concomitant squint. Therefore, the absence of diplopia is usually characteristic of any type of concomitant squint differing from paralytic type. Esophoria and esotropia are closely allied, and in conditions of extreme stress as fatigue or toxemia, an esophoria may become manifest. (2) Divergent squint or exotropia.—This is less frequently en- countered than convergent. As with esotropia it may be monocular or alternating; the fusion faculty is usually weak and the angle of squint will be likely to vary at times. Exotropia differs from eso- tropia in that the latter more frequently responds to nonoperative treatment (correction of refractive errors). Diplopia rarely occurs. (3) Hypertropia.—According to Peter, “A vertical deviation which exceeds the limits of hyperphoria is comparatively rare. In most instances such a deviation is not strictly concomitant, but paralytic in character.” Such a deviation is usually due to a muscular anomaly, and may frequently be found coexistent with an esophoria or exophoria. c. Kappa angle.—Even a marked appearance of squint does not necessarily indicate that a true squint actually exists, and the squint may be apparent only when the visual and optical axes do not coin- cide. The existence of a positive angle kappa will give the examinee the appearance of a divergent squint and a negative angle kappa will simulate a convergent squint. Usually a positive kappa angle is found with hyperopia and a negative angle with myopia. By the angle kappa is meant the angle formed by the intersection of the visual line with the geometric or optic axis, a line connecting the anatomical anterior and posterior poles of the eye. The angle kappa is of clinical value and has an important bearing on the accurate measurement of the angle of squint. The angle kappa can be meas- ured on the perimeter; the eye not being examined is occluded; the other is carefully centered on the fixation point of the perimeter. The examiner rotates the arc of the perimeter until it is in the hori- zontal position; a small electric light bulb (ophthalmoscope without head) is held at the point of fixation and the examiner determines the position of the reflex of the light on the cornea. If the reflex is exactly within the center of the pupil (with the examiner sighting directly behind the light), the visual and optic axes coincide and the kappa angle does not exist. If the reflex is seen on the inner or 142 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 118 nasal side of the center of the pupil, kappa angle is positive; the light is slowly moved along the arc of the perimeter in the. temporal field until its reflex is seen exactly at the center of the pupil, the eye continuing to fix on the fixation point of the perimeter; the number of degrees read off on the arc of the perimeter indicates the value of the positive kappa angle. If the reflex is seen on the tem- poral side of the center of the pupil, kappa angle is negative and may be measured by moving the light in the nasal field until it reaches the center of the pupil. F, FOVEA M, CENTER OF ROTATION. K, NODAL POINT A3, OPTIC OR GEOMETRIC AXIS. O, POINT OF FIXATION. OF VISUAL AXIS, OR LINE. ANGLE OCA IS ANGLE KAPPA. ANGLE OKA IS ANGLE ALPHA. ANGLE OMA IS ANGLE GAMMA. Pxgueh 21.—Measuring angle of squint In the emmetropic eye angle kappa is usually slightly positive, and in hyperopia it may be markedly positive, as much as 5° or more, which would cause an appearance of a divergent squint of 10° or more, as the case may be, when both eyes are casually in- spected. Thus an individual may actually have a squint which is not apparent. On the other hand, he may present the appearance of a marked squint and yet have a condition of orthophoria. 143 TM 8-300 118 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT d. Measuring the angle of squint.— (1) IHrschberg method.— There are several methods of determining the deviation of the squinting eye, the simplest of which is, perhaps, the Hirschberg method. A small lamp is held before the examinee and the posi- tion of its reflex on the cornea is noted, if the reflex is noted on the pupillary edge (outer edge in convergence, inner in divergence), the deviation may be estimated as about 15°. When the reflex is half- way between the pupillary border and limbus the deviation is ap- proximately 35°, and when the reflex is seen at the limbus the deviation may be estimated at 45°. (2) Perimeter method.—The perimeter may be used to measure the amount of deviation in squint. This method is quite accurate but the value of angle kappa must be taken into consideration. The chin rest of the perimeter is adjusted so that the squinting eye is opposite the point of fixation. The examinee is directed to fix upon a point at least 20 feet away, immediately above the point of fixation. The small light, as used in measuring angle kappa, is moved along the arc of the perimeter until its reflex is exactly in the center of the pupil of the squinting eye. The position of the light on the arc of the perimeter indicates the number of degrees of deviation. e. Paralytic squint.— (1) General.—A paralysis of any one of the extrinsic ocular muscles will result in a limitation in the rotation of the globe in the direction of the action of that muscle. Instead of a complete paralysis there may be a slight paresis and the defect of motility may be so slight that it is not recognized upon ordinary inspection. (2) Detection.—Limitation of movements of the globe may be detected in the manner described in paragraph 117, provided they are gross enough to be easily noticed. When the limitation is slight they may escape detection without the employment of special tests. (3) Ghoract eristic symptom.—There is one characteristic symptom, or subjective manifestation of paralytic squint, that the patient prac- tically invariably complains of—diplopia; it is a valuable point in differentiating concomitant and paralytic squint. Unlike concomi- tant squint the affected eye does not have its image suppressed, and the character of the diplopia in the different parts of the normal field of binocular fixation aids the examiner tremendously in arriving at a diagnosis as to the muscle, or muscles, involved. (4) Differentiation between physiologic and pathologic diplopia.— Physiologic diplopia has been referred to repeatedly in connection with stereoscopic vision, and is considered as being a regular accom- paniment of normal binocular vision. Alexander Duane differen- tiated between physiologic and pathologic diplopia, emphasizing the 144 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 118 characteristics of each, in a manner that will be found to be extremely helpful to the student of ophthalmology. The following table is taken from his article “Diplopia and other Disorders of Binocular Projection,” appearing in the Archives of Ophthalmology, volume T, February 1932. Physiologic diplopia Pathologic diplopia The object of fixation appears single The object of fixation appears double, and is distinct. one image being distinct, the other indistinct. The only objects seen double are those Most of the objects in the field of view that are obviously farther or nearer appear double, but particularly those than the object of fixation. The that are close to the object of fixation nearer the objects are to the latter, and are alongside it. the less double they appear, and those that are alongside it appear single. The diplopia is hardly ever recognized Diplopia often obtrudes itself on the spontaneously and rarely causes notice and often causes confusion confusion. and discomfort. If an object is seen double, both images When an object is seen double, one are indistinct, and are also shadowy image has the natural appearance of or ghost-like, so that if one is in the object itself; the other is more or direct line with the object of fixa- less indistinct and shadowy. tion, the latter can be seen through it. The diplopia is not affected by shifting The diplopia is often increased or dimin- the gaze laterally or vertically, pro- ished by shifting the gaze sideways or vided the convergence is unaltered. up and down. The diplopia can be made to disappear Frequently the diplopia remains when at once by changing the convergence, the convergence is altered. so as to fix an object nearer or more remote, as the case may be. /. Symptomatology of paralytic squint.— (1) Deviation.—When one of the lateral recti is paralyzed, and its direct antagonist intact, the eye will deviate toward the side of the intact antagonist, that is, away from the side of the paralyzed muscle. If the degree of in- volvement of the affected muscle be slight the deviation may not be appreciable upon casual inspection. However, it can usually be de- termined by the use of the perimeter and small light, the corneal reflex being noted. A paralysis of one of the vertically acting mus- cles is even less noticeable upon inspection, due to the compensatory action of its synergist. The position of corneal reflex usually will show a deviation that is not obvious. The cover test will be found 271818°—10 10 145 TM 8-300 H18 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT to be of value in deviations of paralytic origin, but it does not dif- ferentiate heterophoria. (2) Primary and secondary deviation.—When directed to fix upon an object in the distance, the examinee -with paralytic squint will fix with the unaffected eye almost invariably, and the eye having a paralyzed muscle will deviate. The deviation in this instance is designated as primary. If the unaffected eye is covered, and he is directed to fix upon the distant object, the unaffected eye will assume the position the affected eye assumed in primary deviation, or assume secondary deviation. Secondary deviation is greater, or more accentuated, than primary deviation. Quoting Peter directly, The reason for the difference in deviation is as follows: The innervation required of the non-paralyzed eye to fix when the paretic eye is covered, is a normal innervation, the paralytic eye simply failing to follow in this conjugate movement because the yoke muscle is paralyzed. In secondary deviation when the paralytic eye fixes in primary position or “eyes front,” a greater effort is required to hold this eye in position because of the paralytic mus- cle, and this same excess of energy goes into the yoke muscle of the non- paralytic eye causing it to deviate, excessively. For example, if the right internal rectus is paralyzed, in the cover test or without the cover test, the right eye will deviate to the right while the left eye fixes. In secondary deviation, if the cover is applied to the left or normal eye, and the right eye is forced into fixa- tion, it will be noted that the left eye will be turned to the left, and to a greater extent than was the primary deviation of the right. It is to be remembered that the amount of rotation of the globe in one direction is not altogether abolished by the paralysis of a muscle, but is lessened only, providing the action of that muscle’s synergist is still affected. (3) Determination of limitation of globe movement.—In paralytic squint there is a limitation in the movement of the globe in the di- rection of action of the paralyzed muscle. This limitation may be determined objectively by actual perimetric measurements, that is, fixation on a small electric light which is moved on the arc of the perimeter and the position of the corneal reflex noted; or subjectively, by moving a small card with a few test letters along the arc of the perimeter, keeping the head fixed and noting the position where blurring occurs (beyond the limit of fixation the retinal image will not be on the fovea, consequently will not be clearly defined). The most peripherally located letter on the card should be used, and the amount of rotation in eight positions of the arc of the perimeter noted, or more as considered necessary, in the direction of the field of action of the paralyzed muscle. The results may be plotted on any ordinary perimetric chart, and a comparison made with the normal field of fixation or with the field of fixation of the unaffected eye. 146 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 118 The limitation in the field of fixation is a valuable point in differen- tiating concomitant and paralytic squint. The meridian in which the limitation is found indicates the muscle that is paretic. g. False 'projection and diplopia.—(1) General.—As has been stated, diplopia is a symptom almost invariably complained of by the patient with paralytic squint, and is rarely encountered in the concomitant type. Frequently, where there exists a diplopia, the patient has con- siderable difficulty in determining which is the true and which is the false image. The tangent screen, in conjunction with a red glass before one eye, greatly facilitates the determination of the muscle at fault. By the use of the red glass there may be determined the relative position of the two images in position of “eyes front,” the direction of movement in which there occurs the greatest separation, whether or not the diplopia is in the horizontal or vertical plane (or both), and the type of diplopia in the horizontal plane, that is, whether crossed or homonymous. The method of use of the red lens test has already been described. The red glass before one eye causes a red image upon the retina of that eye, and the projection of its image upon the screen is noted by the examiner as to the right or left, or above or below the image seen by the fellow eye. In the diplopia of paralytic squint the false image is always the image more distally located, or peripherally projected upon the tangent screen, and by the use of the red glass it can be determined to which eye the false image belongs. For example, consider a paralysis of the right external rectus; the examinee has before his right eye the red glass; the small light is moved along the screen horizontally toward the examinee’s right. A diplopia is noted, and the separation increases as the light moves toward the right side of the screen. The red image will be seen to the right of the white image (homonymous diplopia), and as the light is moved to the right the two will become more widely separated. When the right eye has reached its maxi- mum degree of abduction (by action of the synergists of the right lateral rectus) the retinal image of the light (red as it is seen through the glass) moves away from the right fovea on the nasal side of the retina, and is projected falsely in the right temporal field. As the light is moved in the cardinal directions of the action of each of the six extrinsic muscles, the relative positions of the red and white images indicate the muscle at fault. The false image is the image more distally located, and it is displaced in the direction of action of the affected muscle. (2) Red glass equipment.—It is suggested that the frame of an ordinary “antiglare” goggle, or automobile goggle, be used with 147 TM 8-300 118 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT the tangent screen. The left lens may be removed altogether and the right replaced with a red lens. A lens of this size is more satis- factory than an ordinary red lens from the trial lens case, as it affords a greater field. (3) Torsion.—In paralysis of the extrinsic muscles, an oblique position of the false image may be noted, which indicates a torsion. (4) Disturbances.—Vertigo, dizziness, and even nausea with vom- iting, may be complained of by the patient with paralytic squint, particularly where there is a vertical diplopia or a torsion. Lateral displacement is less likely to give rise to such reflex disturbances. (5) Symptom:—A prominent symptom of paralytic squint, and one that is easily accounted for, is an abnormal rotation of the head. This may be evidenced by face rotation and tilting of the head in order to avoid a diplopia and its annoying accompaniments. In paralyses of the medial and lateral recti the face is turned toward the field of action of the paralyzed muscle. For example, in paralysis of the right external rectus the patient in fixing on an object will turn his head to the right; by so doing he may adduct his right eye and avoid a diplopia which may occur in fixing on an object directly ahead, or certainly would occur if looking at an object to the right. The medial and lateral recti are mentioned in this instance as each of these two muscles has no subsidiary action. In the case of each of the other extrinsic ocular muscles, the face turns toward the normal maximum vertical pull of the paralyzed muscle. Where there exists a torsion of the false image there will likely be an associated tilting of the head to the right or left. (6) Manifestations.—Below is a summary of the objective and sub- jective manifestations of paralyses of the individual ocular muscles: (a) Right external rectus. Primary deviation.—Bight eye deviates to left. Secondary deviation.—Left eye deviates to right. Limitation in movement toward right. False projections.—Toward right, homonymous diplopia, ac- centuated in right temporal field. Lace turned to right. (b) Right internal rectus. Primary deviation.—Bight eye deviates to right. Secondary deviation.—Left eye deviates to left. Limitation in movement to left, nasalward. Crossed diplopia, accentuated toward left. Face turned to left. 148 TjVC 8-300 118 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE (Y2+1G2+1GZ and a pigment blue the formula 3i?2+26?3+lF1. Now if these two are mixed the yellow absorbs the blue rays and the blue absorbs the yellow rays and only the 'ireen rays are reflected. (3Y2 +1#2 +1£3) + (3£2+2£3 + IF1) = 1G2+3G3 + IF1. In other words, when the spectral colors are mixed the result is obtained by addition; when pigment colors are mixed the result is obtained by subtraction. DARK LIGHT DARK YELLOW BLUE RED OR YELLOW GREEN BLUE VIOLET Figure 30.—Simple dichromatic spectrum. The point of maximum luminosity of the spectrum and the most typical yellow are represented by X\ the most characteristic blue by Z; the shades of the several colors by the appropriate initial fol- lowed by a numeral. The dichromic equivalent of any color in the normal spectrum can be approximated by direct comparison of the two spectra. Likewise, if the exact composition of a mixed color is known the dichromic equivalents can be substituted in the formula and the dichromic result can be calculated, for example, R2+B2=rose. Dichromic equivalent : F2+.Z?2=gray, or more commonly (due to the fact that the blue is less luminous than the red) a shade of blue or bluish gray. Consider the match, blue-green, gray, and crimson, made by this examinee; 190 TlVt 8-300 142 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Hexachromic perception Dichromic equivalent Blue-green=-B2 + G2 _ _ B2+ F8=a shade of gray. Gray. 3 yi + .B®=dark brownish gray. Gray Crimson=3/2' + V2 Brown is a shade of dark yellow or orange. In this case a part of the yellow is neutralized by the blue and makes gray, so the end result is a blend of gray and brown with the brown predominat- ing. If the proportions of red and violet in the crimson be varied the result for the dichromat may be dark bluish gray or a blue. In the match yellow, yellow-green, and orange, the three shades are on the same side of the spectrum and would be perceived as three values of yellow, Y5, Y7, and Y8. This case of simple dichromatism is not an extreme example of color-blindness; on the contrary, his color perception is much better than that of any of the common types of dichromatism. These types arise when the dichromic spectrum is still further impaired by the presence of a neutral zone in which no color is perceived. The neutral area may occur as a shortening at the red end, as a shortening at the violet end, as a band of variable width centering in the green, or as combinations of these three. In figure 31 types B and C are ordinarily referred to as deuter- anopes or green blind, the most common form of dichromatism. Types E and F are called protanopes or red blind. This classifi- cation is based on the location of the neutral band. It is apparent that if the neutral band is located in the green, the defect in per- ception of green will be more obvious than the defect in red percep- tion, since there is no sensory basis for a judgment respecting green, whereas an opinion as to red can be reached on the basis of the shades of yellow that are perceived in the red area. In this classifi- cation a spectrum shortened at the violet end would be called violet blind, but since inability to distinguish between dark violet and black is of little practical importance, there are relatively few references to this type. This nomenclature, therefore, is based on the location of the neutral band and takes no account of the reduction in the number of psychophysical units. All types of dichromatism will make mistakes similar to those made by the simple dichromic case that we have considered. In addition to these the dichromat with a shortened spectrum or a neutral band will match colors corresponding to the neutral area 191 TM 8-300 142 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT with black if the color be dark, with gray if it be more luminous. In mixed colors the color from the neutral area does not enter into the mixture except as a black or gray. The only effect of its pres- ence, therefore, is to produce a darkening of the mixture. Since the green is a very luminous area of the spectrum, the neutral band in the green appears gray, and these individuals frequently match RED OR YELLOW GREEN BLUE VIOLET YELLOW BLUE YELLOW BLUE GREEN BLU E YELLOW BLUE A Normal hexachromic spectrum. B Spectrum of normal length. Neutral band in green. C Spectrum shortened at violet end. Neutral band in green. D Very wide neutral band. Practically totally color-blind. E Spectrum shortened at red end. F Spectrum shortened at both ends. Neutral band in green. X Indicates point of maximum luminosity. Figure 31.—Various types of dichromatic spectra. dirty white or white shaded with some color with the green. The ends of the spectrum being darker appear black if there is a terminal shortening; hence deep, pure reds or violets are matched with black; pale shades of pink (red and wdiite) may be put with light grays, etc. 192 TM 8-300 143 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 143. Effect of a shortened red.—A shortening of the red end of the spectrum produces defects that are of considerable practical importance. The red rays from the extreme left of the spectrum are the most penetrating and visible under conditions of obscurity. This explains the red appearance of the sun through a smoked glass. The other rays are cut off, but the extreme red penetrates the smoked glass. We have seen that signal lights, rockets, flares, etc., are com- posed of a mixture of rays; hence, if the observer is unable to per- ceive the red rays because of a shortened spectrum, the red signal light will be invisible at a distance o: under conditions of obscurity; on approaching closer to the liglu the perception of green and blue rays will cause the light to appear green. All colors reflecting rays from the shortened portion appear darker than they do to the normal-sighted and are matched with darker colors that reflect rays from a point more internal. Hence a dichromat with a shortened red will match a red with a darker green. Another com- mon mistake is the confusion of pink and blue. If the pink is com- posed of red and violet and the red rays come from the shortened por- tion of the spectrum, only the violet is perceived and it is seen as a blue. The effect, then, is blue mixed with black; that is, a dark blue. Hence the common confusion of pink with a darker blue or violet. For convenience in reference two tables are now given. The first shows the common color confusions of the color blind and indicates the diagnostic significance of each. In the second table the result of a mixture of spectral colors is found at the intersection of the horizontal and vertical columns. It should be remembered that these results do not apply in mixtures of pigment colors, since these are compound colors and the result is obtained by subtraction. Table I.—Diagnostic import of common color confusions Table I.—. Confusion of Significance Red and black Shortening of red end of spectrum. Shortening of violet end of spectrum. Neutral band extending into violet. A dichromat with neutral band in blue-green. A dichromat for whom two colors are comple- mentary; that is, when mixed, they form gray. Practically diagnostic of dichromatism (2 unit). Practically diagnostic of trichromatism (3 unit). Practically diagnostic of tetrachromatism (4 unit). Violet and black Blue and black Green and black Any mixed color with black or gray. Red and green Green and brown Blue and green 271818°—40 13 193 TM 8-300 143-144 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Table II.—Results of mixture of primary (spectral) colors Violet Blue Green Yellow Red Red Purple Rose Dull yellow Orange Red. Yellow Rose White Yellow-green Yellow. Green Pale blue Blue-green Green. Blue_ Indigo Blue. Violet Violet. 144. Military significance of color-blindness.—a. General.— The various types and degrees of defective color perception give rise to a great diversity of color confusion, many of which are of a bizarre nature and are apparently inexplicable on superficial examination. The color-blind learn to adjust to the defective color perception and some cases compensate so well that the defect is not only not apparent to their associates but may not even be known to themselves. The dark yellow, the bright yellow, and the pale yellow traffic lights of the dichromat are, for all practical purposes, as effective as the red, orange, and green lights of the normal-sighted. In fact, it seems probable that a totally color-blind individual would be able to distinguish the traffic lights by the difference in luminosity; if not, a satisfactory ad- justment could easily be made on the basis of the relative position of the lights; as a last resort he could always follow the traffic. The military significance of color-blindness is discussed below. First it is considered in relation to the arm that has a very high standard, the Air Corps. 5. Air Corps.—In the physical examination of applicants for flying training considerable importance is attached to the detection of color- blindness. The reason for the stress placed on the detection of this condition in prospective military aviators may be briefly outlined as follows: (1) Requirements.— (a) Recognition of various luminous signals such as field boundary lights, obstruction lights, navigating lights, and rocket signals. Distinctive colors are employed to signify various vital conditions and prompt comprehension of their portent is essential for efficient military flying. (h) Recognition of colored flags and other daytime signaling devices. 194 TM 8-300 144 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE (f the gray skeins that was selected as a rose is put up as a test skein, and the instructions are repeated. Number of gray skeins selected—1. Number of gray skeins omitted—I. Other colors selected—i (1 light blue, 1 bluish white, 1 light pink, 1 pale lavender). Manner of performance—confident. Selections by direct comparison—none. Examiner: [Spreads yams on table.'] I want you to cast your eyes over these yarns and pick out for me the three skeins that are the most brightly colored. It makes no differ- ence what the color is or whether they are all the same color or not. Just get the three that seem to stand out most distinctly. Candidate: Selects (1) bright yellow; (2) bright orange; (3) dark red. (8) Comment.—(1) and (2) can fairly be considered as among the brightest colored skeins on the table, but there are at least 12 skeins that are more conspicuous than (3), the well saturated red. It has been observed that in this simple test the color-blind tend to select yellows and blues rather than reds or greens, and that when reds or greens are selected they are the darker, well-saturated shades; for example, this candidate passed over four vivid reds that were conspicuously placed to select a dark, inconspicuous shade. (9) Impression.—A very mild degree of deuteranopia or red-green color-blindness of the green type. This is based on the following consideration: (a) Difficulty in reading and in tracing plates involving red-green discrimination shows that to him there is but little difference in the shades of red and green used in the plates. He probably sees them 271818°—40 14 209 TM 8-300 148 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT as two shades of brownish yellow. This is confirmed by the match- ing of a brown skein with the green. (&) A mixture of red and blue that makes rose for the normal individual is gray to him or nearly so. In other words, red and blue are complementary colors for him. This is evidenced by the indis- tinctness of the rose-colored numerals against a gray background in Ishihara plates 12 and 13 and by matching gray skeins with rose in the Holmgren test. (10) In view of the fact that color blindness disqualifies under the regulations and the disqualification includes the mild as well as the pronounced cases, the examiner properly reported that he did not meet the prescribed standards. It was thought, however, that the defect in this candidate was of such mild degree that it was devoid of any practical significance and could not possibly interfere with the performance of any military duty. It became necessary to revise this appraisal after further investigation. (11) An opportunity was found to give this candidate a practical test; that is, to test his ability to distinguish standard signals under service conditions. The test was carried out as follows: A night was selected when the conditions for visibility were good. The can- didate was posted at a suitable point with three individuals of normal color perception as controls. Nine shots of various colors were fired from a Very pistol from a point y2 mile distant. The candidate and each of the controls made a record of the color of the shots. The distance was then increased to 1 mile and the procedure was repeated. Distance % mile Shots fired Candidate’s reading Shots fired Candidate’s reading Red. Red Red. White. White White. White. White. _ White. Green. Green Green. Red Red. Distance 1 mile Shots fired Candidate’s reading Shots fired Candidate’s reading Green Green, Red Red. Red Red. Red Red. Green Green. Green Green. White White. White. White. White Green. 210 TM 8-300 148 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE (12) This candidate had been passed as having normal color per- ception by more than one examiner. Even after the defect was de- tected by the use of the plates, another examiner who had been informed of the nature and the degree of the defect failed to demon- strate it with the Ishihara and the Holmgren tests, certified him as normal with respect to color perception, and stated that he met the physical requirements. When the Holmgren test was properly con- ducted, however, the defect was at once evident and subsequent test- ing under service conditions showed that this individual cannot al- ways distinguish a green light from a white light or a white light from a green one when he is deprived of all secondary criteria for a judgment and is forced to make his decision solely on the basis of the chromatic quality of the lights in question. One is forced to conclude that he is incompetent to make any decision of conse- quence that depends on a recognition of white or green Very flares. The three controls recorded the shots correctly in each instance. (13) A second case of deuteranopia in which the gross discrepancy of findings on successive examinations was the cause of some em- barrassment is quoted in order to illustrate another error of tech- nique in the conduct of the Holmgren test that may vitiate the results. This candidate on preliminary examination read the Ishi- hara plates in the manner characteristic of the green blind. He was informed that he was disqualified because of color-blindness. Sev- eral months later he asked for another preliminary examination and then read the Ishihara and the Stillings plates fairly well. His per- formance was quite similar to that of the first case referred to. The hesitancy in reading plates 6, 7, 8, and 9- and the difficulty expe- rienced in tracing plates 14 and 15 led to careful checking with the yarns, with the following results: Red test skein. Selected—18. Omitted—2. Included—1 bright orange yellow and 1 paler orange yellow. Performance—confident. Direct comparisons—none. Rose test skein. Selected—23. Omitted—2, Included—3 grays, 2 reds, 2 dark blues, 1 dark purple. Performance—hesitant. Direct comparisons—with gray and with dark blue and purple. 211 TM 8-300 119 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT Green test skein. Selected—5. Omitted—21. Included—none. Performance—hesitant. Direct comparisons—none. Gray test skein. Selected—7. Omitted—3. Included—5 rose. Performance—very hesitant. Direct comparisons—many with gray and rose. (14) The candidate then secured a third examination elsewhere. He read the plates in a manner that was apparently satisfactory to the examiner, but since he had been warned that the candidate was definitely color-blind he put him through the Holmgren test as well. This was conducted in the following manner: The yarns were spread on a small table in bright light; direct sunlight fell on the corner of the table, but not on the yarns. The three test skeins—red, rose, and green—were laid side by side at the edge of the heap of yarns and instructions were given to sort the yarns, one by one, into four piles, one for each test skein and one for the remaining yarns. The candi- date stood beside the table. When he picked up a skein and held it in his hand, it and the three test skeins were in the central visual field in very strong light. He then had to decide by direct com- parison whether a given skein looked more like the red, the green, or the rose test skein, or whether it was some other color (yellow or blue) or whether it had no color at all. Since his ability to see yellow and blue is not impaired, it is not surprising that he made but few errors and that they were minor ones. The examiner was unable to concur in the diagnosis of deuteranopia and expressed the opinion that at most there was only a slight weakness of green perception that was of no practical importance. (15) Other cases could be cited to show that there are many possi- bilities for error in the conduct of the examination of the color perception. Unfortunately, an error in technique that may seem to be of no consequence may render the examination completely value- less. The surest way to avoid such errors is to acquire at least an elementary knowledge of the two main types of defective color per- ception which concern the military service. These are dichromic color perception or red-green color-blindness, subdivided into prota- nopia or red-blindness and deuteranopia or green-blindness; and trichromic color perception, also called tritanopia and anomalous 212 NOTES OX EYE, EAR, ETC., IX AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 148-150 trichromatism. Both of these disqualify for the military service. The latter is much more difficult to detect than the former, especially when the examinee has familiarized himself with the test plates. It is believed, however, that the vast majority of both types can be detected by the use of the common tests, if they are carefully con- ducted by an observant examiner who has given some thought to the matter. 149. Ishihara test.—a. This test is very effective, provided the examinee has not had an opportunity to study the plates. Unfortu- nately, many of them have done so; hence it is necessary for the examiner to have a very low threshold of suspicion, especially in “reexamination” cases, when using this test. Familiarity with the test is to be suspected when: (1) The plates with alternative readings (plates 2, 3, 4, 5) are read with greater facility than are the plates that have only one reading (plates 6, 7, 8, 9). (2) Greater difficulty is experienced in tracing the line in plates 14 and 15 than in reading any of the plates. (3) There is any indication that the numerals do not stand out in bold relief as they do to the normal. b. The Ishihara test can be conducted quite rapidly, IV2 to 2 minutes for an alert, intelligent normal, and for this reason is very useful when a large number of candidates are to be examined. It is be- lieved that this test will disclose practically all cases of significant abnormality of the color perception and that it will never lead to a diagnosis of color-blindness in a normal individual or in one who is not disqualified by regulations. A new seventh edition of this test is now available in two forms, the reduced and the complete edition. No new principle is involved. The plates are similar to those of the sixth edition. One, the abbreviated edition, contains 11 plates and is not as good as the sixth edition; the other contains 32 plates and has the advantage that it has more plates to be traced end that the lines are more tortuous—a very difficult problem for tne examinee who can see but little difference between the colors employed. 150. Stillings test.—Several editions of this test are in use. When detailed report is made of the results of this test the edition used should be stated. The seventeenth edition contains 32 plates, most of them double ones. The plates are arranged in groups that are designed to demonstrate the various types of defective color per- ception. Hence, if the examinee’s readings are recorded, it is possible to analyze the results and make a reasonably accurate differential diagnosis. Due to the number of the plates and the fact that there 213 TM 8-300 150—153 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT are no alternative readings for the color-blind, this test cannot be given as quickly as the Ishihara test. On the other hand, the indi- vidual with defective color perception has much more difficulty in familiarizing himself with these plates, and even if he has done so, the results of his study of them can be largely nullified by the simple expedient of turning the book upside down. Many normals have some difficulty in reading the plates that involve discrimination be- tween red and brown, red and orange, and green and blue, (Groups VI, VII, IX, XI of seventeenth edition). The test is considered highly effective in determining the true state of color perception, but there seems to be no doubt that the milder types of dichromatism and the anomalous trichromats can improve their reading materially by familiarizing themselves with the test before they come up for the final examination. 151. Jennings test.—Some normals make errors in this test and many of the color-blind go through it without making a mistake. For this reason, the results are not considered conclusive unless confirmed by some other test, except in those cases where the examinee makes errors that are characteristic of the color-blind, such as punching red, brown, or gray for green; green, gray, blue, or purple for rose; or failure to punch green and rose. Any of these errors are conclusive evidence of a disqualifying defect of the color perception. It is necessary to reverse the opinion expressed in Army Medical Bulletin Xo. 34 that the Jennings test is suitable for the processing of large numbers of candidates. Further experience shows that it is not reliable enough for this purpose and that it is more time-consuming than either the Ishihara or the Stillings. It is believed, however, that the test has a certain field of usefulness in that it can be conducted in a mechanical manner, and in some apparently borderline cases it may afford confirmatory evidence of color-blindness that is surpris- ing in its completeness and its finality. It should be reiterated that a negative Jennings test is of little value in determining the quali- fications of a candidate and that significance can be attached to the results only when it is positive; that is, when errors characteristic of the color-blind are made. 152. Holmgren test.—a. This is regarded as the basic simple test, for it is possible to obtain more accurate information about the state of the color perception with the yarns than by any other simple method. It is believed that this test if conducted carefully and under- standingly will reveal all defects of the color perception that are dis- qualifying for the military service, and that no individual who is 214 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-3UU 152 not actually disqualified will fail to pass the test with ease. From a practical point of view, however, the test has certain limitations that largely destroy its usefulness. (1) It is tedious and time-consuming. An adequate examination of an alert individual of normal color perception can be done in about 10 minutes. The color-blind case, especially if mild in degree, may require one-half hour or even more. This fact alone makes the test unsuitable for use in the examination of a considerable number of can- didates. (2) In addition to the matter of time, the test does not lend itself to a routine technique of administration as do the plates, and minor variations in technique often produce great discrepancy in the results. Such errors, apparently inconsequential, but actually decisive in their effects, are quite apt to be made unless the examiner has some special knowledge of the common types of defective color perception. (3) Many of the sets of yarns supplied contain so few skeins and so few of the paler shades apt to be confused by the color-blind that they are practically worthless in the detection of the trichromats and the milder cases of dichromatism. Even a careful examination with one of these abbreviated sets may reveal only the well marked dichro- mats (red-green blind). The haphazard use of these inadequate sets of yarns is sufficient to account for the fact that in the past we have detected only one in each six color-blind individuals examined. h. One of these inadequate sets of skeins was recently examined. There are no directions with it. It is made up of 51 skeins, distrib- uted as follows; Green 13 Red- 4 Rose 6 Blue 5 Orange and yellow 11 Lavender 2 Confusion skeins (gray, brown, etc.) 10 Total 51 This set is not only faulty because of the small number of skeins, but also because most of the skeins are the well saturated shades with which the color-blind have the least difficulty. In contrast to this set is a set of 125 skeins that has been used at every opportunity during the past year to examine individuals who have failed to pass the Ishihara or the Stillings test. In every case the results of the plate tests have been confirmed. Some of them made few errors, but 215 TM 8-!i00 15&-153 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT all of them made some errors that were characteristic of defective color perception. This set is composed of the following skeins: Red 15 Green 27 Rose _ 10 Blue-purple 13 Orange-yellow 13 Gray 10 Brown, tan, etc 37 Total 125 This set contains too few gray skeins and a disproportionate number of the brown confusion skeins, but even with this obvious defect it has proved to be reliable enough for all ordinary purposes. c. It is believed advisable before using a set of skeins to examine it critically to determine whether it is suitable for the purpose, for there is little use in using a set that in all probability will not give accurate results. It appears that a good set should contain about 100 skeins. There should be a good assortment of green, of rose, and of red, which must include the lighter shades. The gray skeins should range from very pale gray to very dark gray. It is much better if the gray skeins are not tinted with color; that is, they should be pure neutrals. It is essential that there be such a variety of shades of the different colors that the color-blind individual will find at least some skeins that he is unable to classify. If the set fulfills this require- ment, it is necessary only to require the examinee to examine the skeins one at a time and reach a conclusion as to its chromatic quality purely on the basis of his own sensations and without the aid of any secondary criteria. 153. Proposed method of examination.—Put the candidates through either the Ishihara or the Stillings test. Display plates 6, 7, 8, and 9 of the Ishihara in irregular order, and note particularly any difference in the facility with which the two types of plates are read and any discrepancy between the reading of the numeral plates and the tracing of the line plates. Display the Stillings plates in irregular order and show some of them upside down. If the read- ings in either of these tests are characteristic of the color-blind, a diagnosis of color-blindness is warranted without further examina- tion and the candidate should be disqualified. If the plates are read in the same way with the same facility that the normal read them, appropriate entry (“Normal Ishihara; Normal Stillings”) is made on the form and the candidate is reported as physically qualified. If there is any evidence that the plates do not afford 216 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 153-154 the normal contrast to the candidate, or if because of the discrepan- cies previously referred to it seems probable that the candidate has studied the plates, a detailed record is made of the readings and the examination is continued with the Holmgren test; a detailed record of the results is made and the report is forwarded, with appropri- ate recommendations, for the action of the War Department. An attempt has been made to draw up detailed instructions for the conduct of the Ishihara and the Holmgren tests and to devise forms for the reporting of the results. The ideas expressed have been derived from a number of sources and revisions have been made in the light of knowledge gained from further experience. No claim of finality is made for these instructions; on the contrary, it is anticipated that further revisions will be necessary. They are reproduced here in the hope that they may prove helpful to some examiners and that they may serve as a basis for criticism and for suggestions for improvement. It would seem highly desirable to devise an effective and uniform system for the examination of the color perception and for the reporting of the results, and thereby bring some order and clarity into a situation that at present is con- fused and obscure. 154. Directions for use of Ishihara test plates.—a. The book of colored plates consists of a series of 16 plates (32 in last edition), most of which contain numbers of uniform size and shading, placed in the center or to one side of the center of the circles. Plate 1, containing the figure 12, which is visible to the color-blind as well as to those of normal color sense, shows the size and shading of the other numbers. h. The candidate is considered to have failed on this test if he does not read the plates in approximately the same way as a person of normal color sense. The examination for color sense is to be conducted only by medical officers found to have normal color sense. c. To a person of defective color sense, some of the plates seem to show numbers different from those seen by a person of normal color sense, or to show no number at all. Others of the plates appear to contain no number to a person with normal color sense, while the color-blind will read numbers in these plates. d. The test is made by the examiner holding the book on a table, or in his hands, in good, diffused daylight, between 20 and 30 inches from the eyes of the candidate being tested. The test is made on both eyes together, since for practical purposes defective color sense may be considered as always binocular. The candidate is first asked 217 TM 8-300 154 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT to read the figure on plate 1 and is told that the other figures which he is expected to read (plates 2 to 13) are of the same size and type of shading. The other plates, 2 to 13, are then shown to him in turn, allowing him from 2 to 5 seconds to read aloud the figure on each plate—2 seconds for the more alert and intelligent and for those who may have familiarized themselves with the plates previously, and 5 seconds for those who are slower to comprehend. The same time is allowed for plates on which the normal eye can see no fig- ures as for the others, and the examiner is not to signify by tone or motion whether the candidate has read a plate correctly or not. Plates 14, 15, and 16 (see e below) may take longer than 5 seconds apiece to explain and trace, but for all the plates the ease with which the plates are read is to be taken into consideration as well as the final answer; thus, some individuals of normal color sense may make out a well shaped 5 and a similar 2 on plates 10 and 11, respectively, but less easily than the figures on the other plates. e. No numbers are to be read on the three plates, 14, 15, and 16, but they are to be used by having the candidates try to trace a curv- ing line across each plate from the X at the left-hand margin to the X at the right-hand margin, each line being more or less of the same color. These lines should be traced without pressing on the plate, with the rounded wooden end of a penholder, a rounded glass rod, a fine dry camel’s hair brush, or other instrument which will not mark the plate. The color-blind may trace a line different from the line traced by the normal, trace no line at all where a line is visible to the normal eye, or trace a line where no continuous line from one X to the other X is seen at all by the normal eye. The examiner is not to name the color of the line to the candidate, but may ask the candidate the color of the line. Before each test care should be taken to see that no telltale line has been left on any of these three plates from previous use. /. In every case where color perception is found deficient, the results of the test will be recorded on the attached or a similar form, the figures as read by the candidate being placed after the number of each plate, a dash indicating that no figure was read. In the case of plates 14, 15, and 16, the word “red” indicates that a reddish or pinkish line was traced by the candidate, while “blue” indicates that a blue line was traced, and a dash indicates that no line was traced. If the applicant reads both numerals on plates 12 and 13, he will be asked which numeral stands out more distinctly and his answer recorded in the space provided on the blank. 218 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 154 Result in typical case Plate Reads Plate Reads Plate Reads 1 12 7 12 2 2 3 8 13 4 3 5 9 14 Blue 4 2 10 5 16- 5 21 11 2 16 Blue 6 — Tho person whose record is given above is called typically “green- blind” and exhibits the commonest form of color-blindness. If the figures 6 and 2 had been read on plates 12 and 13 respectively, the person would have been called “red blind.” All of these responses, except that for plate 1, are different from those given by a person of normal color sense, and yet such a “green-blind” or “red-blind” person can distinguish some red and some green at ordinary distances and in ordinary weather conditions. g. If there is a possibility of the candidate’s having memorized the correct answers, the order of the plates may be changed by slipping them out of the slots at the four corners and reinserting them at dif- ferent places. The original order may be determined by reading the small numbers in the margins of the plates with a magnifying glass. h. The plates must be kept out of direct sunlight and the book should be closed except when actually being read. The book and all directions accompanying it are to be safeguarded from scrutiny by persons not officially responsible for conducting the test. Provided the applicant is found to have normal color vision on this test, no further tests need be used. If color perception is found to be deficient by this test, then the Holmgren yarn test will be given as outlined in the instruction sheet and chart. 219 TM 8-300 154-155 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Plate Examinee Report of Ishihara test of tbe color perception of number reads Applicant for 1 Plate No. 12, number . is more distinct. 2 Plate No. 13, number . 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Place._ / Date.. Examiner _ Medical Corps. 155. Directions for Holmgren (yarn) test.—The test is to be conducted by a medical officer who has normal color perception. Seat the examinee at a table with his back to the window. Place skeins on the table in good diffused daylight—not in direct sunlight. Place the test skein 3 or 4 feet away, so that matches will not be made by direct comparison unless the examinee makes a special effort to do so. Instruct the examinee to take the skeins from the heap one by one and sort them into two piles, the first pile to contain all shades of the color of the test skein, the second pile to contain all other skeins. Go through this procedure separately with the green, the rose, and the red test skeins, mixing the skeins after each sorting. Make no com- ment on the examinee’s selections, but note carefully any erroneous selections, any skeins of the test color that are overlooked, and any skeins that seem to be difficult to classify. (See 2, 3, 46, 4c of report, sheet.) Then use these skeins as test skeins and go through the pro- cedure as before, entering the color of such additional test skein in one of the blank columns of the report sheet. Spread the skeins out on 220 TM 8-300 155-156 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE the table and instruct the examinee to select the three most brightly colored skeins in the lot. Report of yarn test of the color perception of Applicant for _ Test skein used Green Rose Red 1. Number of skeins of test color selected. 2. Number of skeins of test color omitted. * 3. Were any skeins other than test color selected? If so, report color and shade of each. 4. Manner of performance: а. Hesitant or confident? б. Were any selections made only by direct comparison with test skein? If so, report color and shade of each. c. Was classification of any skein changed after direct comparison? Report color and shade of each. Selected as brighest colors: (1) (2) (3) Does the examiner agree with this selection? (1)_ _ (2) _ (3) Medical Corps. Section XIX FIELD OF VISION FOE FOEM AND COLOES Peripheral vision 15$ Confrontation test 157 Perimeter 158 Form and color fields 159 Tangent screen and rule 169 Classification of defects in visual fields 161 Physiologic blindspot 162 Paragraph 156. Peripheral vision.—a. In the testing of visual acuity the ability to perceive form is determined in only the foveal region of the retina. This portion of the retina represents a very minute frac- tion of the entire percipient retina which extends as far forward as the choroid, that is, to the ora serrata, which is about 6 millimeters 221 T3Vt 8-300 156 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT:1 from the limbus. Normally, this entire surface of the retina, with the exception of that portion occupied by the structures of the optic nerve, functions, but in a somewhat different manner than at the fovea. h. Structurally and functionally the fovea and peripheral portions of the retina differ. At the fovea only cones are found, but at a distance of only 3,4 millimeter from center or 1° away from the fovea the rods are more numerous than the cones; 4° away the rods are ten times as numerous and at 10° there are about twenty rods to one cone. Visual acuity, as such, diminishes rapidly away from the fovea. According1 to Burckardt's formula, the acuity of a point of the retina may be estimated as y3 n. n oeing the number of degrees away from the center of the fovea. Other estimates of peripheral acuity have oeen made wbicb may be at fault, as it is practically impossible to compare by the same test two portions of the retina that are so little alike. “As soon as any test object approaches the proximity (5°-10°) of the fixing area, it exerts so powerful an attraction upon the attention and powers of the central few degrees, that it is almost impossible to resist the impulse to use direct vision55 (Lloyd). Binoc- ular fixation, as is obtained with the Stereo-Campimeter, permits more accuracy in determining acuity away from the fovea, fairly accurate findings within the central 20° being obtained. It is evident that we use a surprisingly small portion of the macula for ordinary reading. c. In the periphery of the retina detection of moving objects is remarkably keen, although fixed objects are not seen with a sharp definition of detail. This function of the retina is one that we do not consider ordinarily, but it is by this function that we are able to orient ourselves in relation to objects within the field of vision. We constantly use the periphery of our retinae in any form of loco- motion. walking, driving an automobile, and flying. Further, such function may be considered as a very valuable protective device or warning mechanism. d. The macula area, where central vision is concerned, is less sen- sitive to low degrees of illumination, or, in comparison with peripheral points of the retina, is “night blind51 normally. One authority has found the most sensitive portion of the retina in recognizing barely luminous points to be between 11° and 18° away from the fovea. This can be determined by observing a barely perceptible star at night; it will disappear on direct fixation, but reappears when the point of fixation is 10° to 15° away from it. e. We may appreciate the value of peripheral vision by occluding all but the central portion, as looking through a tube of small diameter and attempting to walk across the room. Thus, in a way, the function 222 TM 8-300 156-157 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE of the peripheral portion of the retina is as important as that of the highly developed fovea. 157. Confrontation test.—In determining the extent of the field of vision there are two methods that may be used, but at the same time there are several factors that must be taken into consideration. First, a “rough and ready” method is the confrontation test, or fixation and finger test which, while appearing to be very crude and inaccurate, is very useful when it is done carefully. The examiner stands about 2 feet in front of the examinee, facing him, with a window at one side. The examiner covers his left eye with his left hand and directs the examinee to cover his right eye in a similar manner. Next the ex- aminee is told to fix upon the exposed pupil of the examiner, who in turn fixes upon the examinee’s left pupil. The examiner moves his right hand in from the periphery toward the line of fixation, with one or more fingers exposed, keeping his hand at all times as nearly as possible in a vertical plane halfway between him and the examinee. The hand movement is carried out in the vertical, horizontal, and two oblique meridians, and the examiner makes a comparison of the left visual field of the examinee with his own right visual field. The examinee, if his visual field is normal, will identify fingers and finger movements at the same instant as does the examiner, provided the field of vision of the latter is normal. In this manner, an estimate is made of the examinee’s left visual field. The examiner and ex- aminee may then change places, and in a like manner the latter’s right field of vision may be approximated. This method, quite simple and rapid in its application, will detect any gross defect in the extent of field for form. It has the following advantages: it is quick, re- quires no special equipment, and enables the examiner to be assured that throughout the test the examinee is maintaining fixation con- stantly. Its disadvantages are the inability to make accurate quan- titative determinations, the impossibility of carrying the test objects (fingers in this case) to the full extent of the normal temporal field (90° or more), and the possibility of overlooking such defects as homonymous and bitemporal hemianopsia, “They may be roughly tested for by telling the patient to look straight at the surgeon, sit- uated as before, both eyes being open. The surgeon holds up both hands, one in each temporal field, and the patient is told to touch the surgeon’s hand. If he asks, ‘Which one?’ he has not bitemporal hemianopsia, since he sees both hands. If he promptly points to one, he should be asked if he sees the other; if he does not, he probably has homonymous hemianopsia” (Parsons). 223 TM 8-300 157-158 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Where a defect in the visual field is found, or suspected by the fixation test, a more accurate examination using the perimeter should be made. Figure 32.—Perimetric field studies. 158. Perimeter.—The perimeter consists essentially of an arc of 90° or more (more frequently of 180° or more) which may be ro- tated about a pivot upon which the examinee fixes with the eye being examined. The eye being examined must be situated exactly 224 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 158-159 before the point of fixation and at a distance equal to the radius of the arc of the perimeter. The examinee must be seated with his back to the light, which should be uniform on the arc of the per- imeter regardless of its position. The eye not being examined must be completely screened, as with an eye patch, or the lids may be closed with a strip of adhesive. With the arc of the perimeter adjusted horizontally the extent of the temporal field for form should be determined by bringing the test object (white in this instance) inward along the arc toward the point of fixation, noting where it is first observed (not where thei color is identified) by the examinee (the back of the perimeter are is marked in degrees away from the point of fixation). At least eight meridians should be investigated, that is, temporal, upward and temporal, directly upward, upward and nasalward, nasalward, down- ward and nasalward, directly downward, and downward and out- ward; or, in other words, at intervals of 45°. In each of these meridians the test object should be carried to the fixation point in order to determine the presence of scotomata. The examinee should be cautioned repeatedly to maintain fixation, which is quit© difficult, particularly as the test object approaches the point of fixation. Fur- ther, the examiner should observe the eye under examination to be sure that fixation is maintained. In outlining the form field, the test object should be gently agitated as the normal limit for each meridian is approached, as the function of the peripheral portion of the retina is primarily to detect moving objects. 159. Form and color fields.—a. Plotting field.—The form field is plotted upon the perimetric chart which has concentric circles corresponding with degrees on the arc of the perimeter. Where a defect is noted, as many additional meridians as may be necessary must be investigated. b. Determination of blue, red, and green fields.—After mapping the field for form using the white test object, the extent of the fields for blue, red, and green should be determined, using appropriately colored test objects. “The limit of a field for a color is the point at which, passing from the periphery to the center, the color first becomes evident” (Parsons). This is a point that is difficult to determine even with the help of a very cooperative examinee. c. Color perception at extreme periphery.—It has been thought, and taught, that the extreme periphery of the retina is color-blind, but by many authorities today this is not considered as being strictly true. “Although it is relatively color-blind, compared to the acuity of the color sense at the fovea, all the colors can be perceived in the periphery in a normal individual if the test object is large enough” 271818°—40——15 225 TM 8-300 159 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (Adler). “There is overwhelming proof—that the peripheral vision behaves in exactly the same manner as central vision but with diminished sensitivity. Greater stimuli are required to produce simi- lar responses, but if the stimuli are sufficiently great, the differences disappear, including even qualitative differences so that the fields of vision for colors extend to the extreme periphery” (Parsons). Apparently the size of the test object used for outlining color fields is of the greatest importance, in addition to the intensity of the colors used in test objects. Otherwise, the determination of the so- Figueh 33.—Hand perimeter with spherical test objects. called color fields, as so frequently conducted, may be of little if any value. d. Conditions to he observed in color tests and apparatus.—The fol- lowing is quoted from Visual Field Studies y by Ralph I. Lloyd: Conditions to t>e observed in Color Tests and Apparatus.—If color tests are to be used, it is absolutely essential that certain conditions be complied with. The background must be colorless grey which is of the same brightness as the colors used. The tests must be conducted in full “daylight” or the equivalent, which means “standard daylight” obtained by filtering electric light through blue grass to remove the yellow. The test must be done in full illumination and a dark room or low illumination is not permissible because this introduces 226 TM 8-300 159 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE the question of dark adaptation whereby “twilight worth” is substituted for “periphery worth.” A red and a blue which are of the same brightness by “twilight vision,” when observed by the light adapted eye, are peripherally colorless and not of equal brightness. In one case, the dark adapted rods are tested and in full light the peripheral cones. The size of the test discs will be decidedly larger than those we have been accustomed to use, because to obtain peripherally similar colors, the saturation must be lowered. The colors should be invariable, that is, they must pass from the peripheral colorless zone wherein they appear without color, into the zone where they are recognized as true colors, without passing through intermediate phases. They should also be of the same brightness as the background against which they must appear, when observed by the peripheral retina. The test object must be protected from the light and should be replaced at regular intervals. The use of colored test objects will be referred to again in this section in connection with certain abnormal conditions. The tests of the fields of color are not made in the regular examination of appli- cants and flyers. e. Test object.—Going back to the use of the white test object in delineating the field of form; in using the white object, two pre- cautions should be observed; the examinee must be told to indicate when the object is first seen, not to identify it as white; and the test object should be moved back and forth as it is brought in from the periphery. Further, the white test object should be of a certain size. The size of the test object should be noted in degrees rather than its diameter in millimeters. The size of a test object, either spherical or of disc shape, may be estimated fairly accurately by first determining the radius of the perimeter used: 2 X 3.1416 X radius . „ 0 — ggQ = diameter or a 1 test object. 2 X 3.1416 X radius . . = diameter ot a 2° test object. 2X3.1416Xradius = diameter of a go tes( ob]-ect- = d;am(ster rf & 1QO object_ Or, in other words, the circumference of the circle (of which the perimeter is an arc) divided by 360, the number of degrees in a circle, will be the approximate diameter of a 1° test object. The word ap- proximate is used as in this instance the arc is being used rather than chord to determine the diameter. In outlining the field for form a 1° white test object is ordinarily employed. In plotting the outlines of a scotoma, particularly if it is near the point of fixation, smaller test objects may be used for greater accuracy. The test objects should be illuminated by not less than T-foot candles. 227 TM 8-300 159-160 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT /. Field for form.—The average normal field for form is approxi- mately as follows: Out, 90°. Out and up (45° from horizontal), 62°. Vertically, 52°. Vertically and inward (45° from horizontal), 55°. Inward, 60°. Inward and downward (45° from horizontal), 55°. Downward, 70°. Downward and out (45° from horizontal) ,85°. As a rule, emmetropes and hyperopes have wider fields than myopes. Any contraction of the form field of 15° or more in any meridian dis- qualifies for flying. g. Field for color.—The field for blue should be about 10° to 15° smaller than the field for form, and roughly concentric with it; the field for red about 15° less than that for blue; and the green field is usually the smallest, being 10° to 20° smaller than that of the red. As has been shown, the value of color fields as commonly determined is at best questionable. It is not the intention of the writer to advocate abolishing the use of colored test objects altogether, but the student should bear in mind the criticisms of certain authorities before making a definite interpretation of the extent of the various fields for color. Particular attention should be paid to the investigation of the central portion of the field with colored test objects, as central relative scoto- mata, an acquired color-blindness, may be indicative of toxic amblyopia or retrobulbar neuritis. In such conditions there may be a red-green blindness. In optic atrophy there may be found early a marked con- traction of the color fields altogether out of proportion with the con- traction of the form field, which shows some degree of contraction. h. Divisions of visual field.—The visual field, as a whole, may be divided for descriptive purposes as follows (Fuchs) : (1) The central area, from the point of fixation to about 2° around it. (2) The paracentral zone, from the peripheral limits of the central zone to about 8° on the nasal side and 12° on the temporal side. (3) The coecal zone, from the limits of the paracentral zone to approximately 25°. (4) Intermediate zone, from 25° to 60°. (5) The peripheral zone, beyond 60° in all meridians. 160. Tangent screen and rule.—a. Use.—The use of a tangent plane is a recognized and established procedure in the determination of the central, paracentral, and coecal zones of the visual field, for both form and color. For this purpose various campimeters are employed, and in particular the Bjerrum tangent screen or curtain. 228 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 160 Such a device has its limitations in visual field studies, for an attempt to project the entire visual field of a normal eye upon a plane surface is quite impossible. The difficulty lies in the fact that toward the periphery of the field the tangent increases enormously, the limiting value of the tangent of a 90° angle being infinity. For this reason the use of a plane surface in delineating visual fields is limited to the vicinity of approximately 45° which includes the central, para- central, coecal, and a good portion of the intermediate zones. h. Advantages.—The method of employing the tangent screen as an adjunct to perimetry is simple and need not be discussed at length. The disadvantages are, as already stated, the limitations of its appli- cation to certain zones, and the difficulty experienced with the vary- Figurb 34.-—Projection of visual field upon plane surface. ing size of the retinal image produced by a given test object at different distances from the point of fixation. The advantage of a plane surface is the magnification or enlargement of a scotoma in its projection which assures greater accuracy. Furthermore, the exam- iner obtains a true and realistic projection of that portion of the visual field which is covered by the screen or plane surface. The ordinary perimetric record does not quite accomplish this result. To interpret the flat perimetric record properly the perimetrist must imagine that he is examining the interior of a hemisphere rather than a plane surface. c. Equipment.—(1) Screen.—A blackboard about 6 feet square, or even smaller, will answer the purpose admirably. Blackboard cloth on an ordinary window shade will serve the purpose if wall space 229 TJVt 8-300 160 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT is not available. For “rough and ready” use the blackboard is not necessary, for a blank portion of wall space may be utilized. The blackboard or wall space may be used at the convenient working distance of 75 centimeters, or at any distance; the value of the tan- gents, of course, varying with different distances. Any given dis- tance may be halved, quartered, or doubled whiie using one rule. (2) Buie.—The tangent rule may be made from a yardstick, or a light strip of lath such as is used as a stiffener for the common roller ■window shade. A more elaborate one may be made from one-eighth inch celluloid, such as is used in making the triangles, protractors, and irregular curves employed by draftsmen. A protractor at one end may be included, which will be found to be very helpful. The values of the tangents should be etched or scratched upon one edge of the rule. d. Values of tangents.—The exact distance, or values of the various tangents, may be easily computed; but for convenience they are tabu- lated as follows (for a working distance of 75 cm.) : Degrees Distance from end of rule (in centimeters) Degrees Distance from end of rule (in centimeters) 1 1. 31 26 36. 58 2 2. 62 27 38. 21 3 3. 93 28 39. 88 4 5. 24 29 41. 57 5 6. 56 30 43. 30 6 7. 88 31 45. 07 7 9. 21 32 46. 87 8 10. 64 33 48. 71 9 11. 88 34 50. 69 10 13. 22 35 52. 52 11 14. 58 36 54. 49 12 15. 95 37 56. 52 13 17. 32 38 58. 60 14 18. 70 39 60. 74 15 20. 09 40 62. 93 16 21. 50 41 65. 20 17 22. 93 42 67. 53 18 24. 37 43 69. 94 19 25. 82 44 72. 43 20 27. 30 45 75. 00 21 28. 79 46 77. 60 22 30. 30 47 80. 43 23 31. 84 48 83. 296 24 33. 39 49 86. 28 25 34. 97 50 89. 385 TM 8-300 160 Centimeters Centimeters 75 Centimeters TANGENT RULE beveled Edge Length Of Rule . Beveled Edge Stops Here Beveled Edge TANGENT RULE. Material % Inch Clear Celluloid. Scale Equals 1 Centimeter All Dimensions Expressed In Centimeters Note: AH Lines And Letters Cut And Black Filled On Back Side Of Rule. Front Face Shown On Drawing. Figure 35.—Tangent rule. 271818°—40 (Face page. 230) NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TIVi 160 It must be borne in mind that these figures are computed for a work- ing distance of 75 centimeters, and to obtain accurate findings the scale and blackboard are to be used with the examinee’s eye at exactly this distance from the fixation point on the blackboard. e. Procedure.—The examinee should be seated comfortably in front of the blackboard at the proper distance, which may be maintained with a fair degree of accuracy by using the back of a chair as a chin rest. The blackboard should have daylight illumination, that is, should be located before a window in such position that a uniform illumination is assured. The eye not being examined is screened. The tangent rule may be placed against the blackboard as a T-square and adjusted until a point is found directly before the eye being examined, and at this point a dot is made with crayon. This dot is used as a fixation point during the examination. The test objects are brought in from the periphery of the black- board toward the fixation point in as many meridians as desired, and where any point of significance is noted, as for example the border of a scotoma, or the limitation of the field for form, a minute crayon mark is made upon the board. The significant points, or marks, in the various meridians may later be connected by lines, and these points and lines measured in degrees from the fixation point by the tangent rule. If a protractor is included with the tangent rule, the exact meridians may be drawn on the board after the examination is completed. Thus we have a fairly accurate outline of the field of v,v' n within 50° of the fixation point, and from it we may arrive at conclusions or make a definite diagnosis just as readily as by study- ing a record obtained by a perimeter. /. Record of findings.—When a permanent record of the findings is desired, the tracings on the blackboard may be transferred to an ordinary perimeter form by using the tangent scale, locating points of significance in degrees away from the point of fixation and the exact meridians in which these points are found. If the tangent rule is not provided with a protractor at one end, the various meridians may be determined accurately by again using the measurements on the rule for the tangential values of angles. The horizontal meridian may be determined by measuring the distance from the point of fix- ation to the floor, and locating a point on the margin of the black- board at exactly this distance from the floor. A line joining these two points indicates the horizontal meridian. At a point 75 centi- meters from the point of fixation on the horizontal meridian, the tangent rule may be placed at right angles to the horizontal meri- dian; then lines joining the point of fixation and the various points 231 TM 8-300 160 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT on the tangent rule indicate the meridians of corresponding degrees (see fig. 33). The same procedure may be adopted for locating the meridians beyond 45° by using the tangent rule at right angles to the vertical meridian, at a point 75 centimeters from the point of fixa- tion. The tangent rule combined with a protractor at one end greatly facilitates this procedure. g. Test objects.—As to test objects, the following suggestion is offered. Again referring to the tangent rule, we find that the diameter of a 1° test object at 75 centimeters is 1.31 centimeters (approxi- mately, as we are dealing with an arc rather than a chord), and that a 10° test object would be 13.22 centimeters in diameter. But this holds true only when the test object is at the point of fixation on the blackboard. The retinal image of it decreases in size as it is moved Horizontal M eri-dian Fixoti'on Poinf Figure 36.—Use of tangent rule to determine meridians. toward the periphery, and at 45° away from the point of fixation the size of its retinal image has decreased about 33ys percent. This fact should be borne in mind and appropriate allowances made. Test objects for form only may be procured in the form of white beads of spherical shape in any required diameter, which may be mounted upon piano wire. Colored beads are available, but here a difficulty presents itself that is hard to overcome, that is, the standardization of the colors. For accurate results in color fields, it is advisable to obtain the discs made of Heidelberg paper. h. Supplemental use.—Another valuable use of the tangent rule in connection with the blackboard is the determination of the kind and amount of diplopia. For this purpose the blackboard is employed exactly as is the Bjerrum curtain, the red glass before the right eye and the point of illumination being carried outward from the central 232 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 160-161 point in the six cardinal directions. The point where diplopia occurs is noted by making a small crayon mark upon the board, red crayon being used to indicate the location of the red image, and white crayon for that of the white image. By using the tangent rule, the locations of these positions can be determined in degrees, and the amount of separation noted. The plot formed by the location of the crayon marks may be transferred to a blank form for this purpose, or any perimetric form, with appropriate notations as to whether the diplopia is crossed, vertical, and so on. 161. Classification of defects in visual fields.—a. General.— In addition to a constriction of the visual field, visual field defects may be classified as follows (Lloyd) : (1) Scotomata, which may be— (a) Negative. (&) Positive. (c) Relative. (d) Absolute. (e) Coeco-central. (/) R^g. (2) Hemianopsia, which may be— (a) Homonymous. (h) Heteronymous. (3) Sectorial defects. (a) Quadranopsia. (5) Hemichromatopsia. (4) Blindspot scotomata. (a) Seidel’s sign. (6) Bjerrum’s sign. (c) Roenne’s nasal step. h. Scotomata.—“Scotoma is usually defined as an insular defect in the visual field” (Lloyd). A positive scotoma is one that the patient is aware of, that he sees as a shadow, as may be caused by an opacity in the vitreous near the retina. A negative scotoma is one that the patient is not conscious of, such as that that occurs where there has been degenerative changes in recipient layer of the retina (choroido- retinitis, etc.). Where a positive scotoma is found it is assumed that the perceptive portion of the retina is functioning in part at least. A negative scotoma indicates that a certain area of the retina has lost its funtion. A relative scotoma is one within which an object is seen as being distorted, or in which the color is not recognized. In an obsolute sco- toma the patient does not recognize the object either by form or color. 233 TIYL 8-3UU 161 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Coeco-central scotomata, as the name implies, interferes with central vision; the ring types are more peripherally located, the latter are often roughly concentric in outline. c. Hemianopsia.—By hemianopsia, or hemianopia, is meant a loss of half of the visual field. It may be homonymous, that is, corre- sponding halves affected, both right halves for example. Or it may Figure 37.—Tangent screen record for findings •within 35° of point of fixation. be heteronymous, which would include the binasal and bitemporal types; “the former points to a lesion crippling both uncrossed bundles which supply the temporal halves of each retina which, in turn, means a loss of each nasal field. To do this the lesion must affect the lateral angle of the chiasm. Bitemporal hemianopsia is the classic sign of pituitary pressure upon the crossed bundles of the chiasm which sup- ply each nasal retinal half which, in turn, implies a loss of each tern- 234 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 161 poral field” (Lloyd). There is a type of hemianopsia classified as altitudinal which is characterized by a loss of function of the upper or lower halves of the retina. Bilateral altitudinal hemianopsia is very rare, but the monocular type is not infrequently encountered (detached retina, optic nerve lesions, etc.). d. Sectorial defects.—Sectorial defects include losses which are less than quadrants of the visual field, and may be indicative of lesions of Figure 38.—Tangent screen record for findings within 50° of point of fixation. This form may be used for recording diplopia with red glass and tangent screen. the cortex. By hemichromatopsia is meant a loss of half the field for color only. e. Blindspot scotomata.— (1) Seidel's sign.—“Seidel’s sign” is a slender, finger-like scotoma extending from the physiologic blindspot upward or downward and is found in early glaucoma. It usually 235 TM 8-300 161-162 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT requires the employment of quite small test objects with the tangent screen for its detection. (2) Bjernvm's sign.—The Bjerrum sign is a finger-like scotoma extending from the physiologic blindspot above or below, or both, assuming the form of an arc and possibly involving the fixing area. Early it is a relative (color) scotoma, but later it is absolute in glau- coma. The tangent screen, or campimeter, with small test objects, is required for its detection. In addition, it may be found in the senile. (3) Roenne’s nasal step.\—Eoenne’s nasal step is a scotoma involving the nasal quadrant, more frequently the inferior nasal quadrant. Figure 39.—Outlining physiologic blindspot, right eye, on blackboard. The scotoma is usually roughly right-angular in outline, and its apex advances toward the physiologic blindspot. Such a finding is indica- tive of glaucoma. 162. Physiologic blindspot.—The physiologic blindspot repre- sents the entrance of the optic nerve into the globe, and therefore cor- responds with the anatomical location of the disc in the fundus. Obviously it is within the temporal field of vision of each eye, and is located about 15° away from the macula. Usually it is approximately a little less than 5° in diameter, and its center is situated a little below 236 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-30Q 162 the horizontal meridian. As a rule, its vertical diameter is somewhat longer than the horizontal. Clinically its size is important; an en- largement may be indicative of glaucoma, optic atrophy, toxic ambly- opia, posterior ethmoidal or sphenoidal sinus disease, arterio-sclerosis, and opaque nerve fibers. An enlargement of the physiologic blindspot is usually found in high degree of progressive myopia. Figukd 40.—Using tangent rule in locating position and size of physiologic blindspot. The tangent screen, with small test objects, is more valuable in out- lining the physiologic blindspot than is the perimeter. With the latter its location only can be determined. It is suggested that the student outline his own blindspots upon the tangent screen at a distance of 75 centimeters. The position of the physiologic blindspot may be useful in determin- ing the angle of squint. 237 TM 8-300 163 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT Section XX Paragraph Classification of lenses 163 Principal focus—focal length 164 Combination of lenses 165 Analysis of lenses 166 Classification of errors of refraction 167 Determination of errors of refraction 168 Correction of presbyopia 169 Transposition of formula 170 Decentering of lenses 171 REFRACTION 163. Classification of lenses.—a. Before considering methods of quantitatively determining refractive errors, it is advisable that some points in elementary optics be reviewed. The action of a prism has already been considered, but it must be remembered that a prism will form no image and it has no focus. If two prisms are placed together base to base and viewed from the side, their faces appear somewhat similar to the cross section of a convex spheric lens; the similarity becomes more marked when a comparison is made with a series of truncated prisms placed one above the other, the larger two being base to base. (See fig. 41.) It is easily seen that a lens toward its periphery has the effect of a prism. b. Lenses may be classified as follows: (1) Spherical (abbreviated as S, or sph).—In such lenses the curved surfaces represent sections of spheres, and a spherical lens refracts rays of light equally in all meridians. Spherical lenses may be— (a) Convex (synonyms plus, positive, collective, magnifying),—Of the convex variety there may be plano-convex, bi-convex, and concavo- convex types. All convex lenses are thickest at the optical center. {b) Concave (synonyms minus, negative, dispersive, minifying).—• Such a lens may be plano-concave, bi-concave, or convexo-concave. All concave lenses are thinnest at the optical center. Any spherical lens, on section, may be compared to a series of truncated prisms gradually increasing in strength from the center toward the periphery. In a convex lens the bases of the prisms are toward the center; in a concave lens they, are toward the periphery. This com- parison enables us to realize the effect a lens has upon parallel rays of light. (2) Cylindrical lenses (abbreviated as C or cyl).—A cylindrical lens is so termed because it is a segment from a cylinder. There is one meridian of a cylindrical lens in which the lens is of uniform thickness, and this meridian is parallel to the axis of the original cylinder of 238 TM 8-300 163-164 NOTES ON EYE. EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE which the lens is a segment. This meridian is designated as the axis of a cylindrical lens. A cylindrical lens has no focal point, but has a line of foci, and this line is parallel to the axis. Cylindrical lenses may be concave or convex and may be made with both surfaces curved (bi-convex, bi-concave, etc.). In addition to the designation of the strength of a cylindrical lens, the position of its axis must be noted, when it is prescribed. The commonly accepted designation of axes positions on the spectacle trial frame is 0° to 180° (both these extremes being the same, axis horizontal), increasing in an anticlockwise man- ner, for each eye, when facing the spectacle trial frame. Consequently, axis 90° is vertical for each eye, but axis 45° is up and out for the left eye, and up and in for the right. Figure 41.—Aggregation of prisms forming bi-convex lens. F, real focus. 164. Principal focus—focal length.—a. Principal focus.—The principal focus of a lens is the point where parallel rays of light, after refraction by the lens, intersect the axial ray; it is the point, as a matter of fact, where all parallel rays are brought to a focus after being refracted by the lens. In a spherical lens this is a point, in cylindrical lenses the principal focus is a line. In concave lenses, the principal focus is located at the intersection of a backward prolonga- tion of the emerging divergent rays (see fig. 42). The optical center (nodal point) of a simple spherical lens with equal curvature on both sides may be described as a point within the lens equidistant from its two surfaces in the thickest part of a plus lens, and in the thinnest part of a minus lens. It is through this point the axial ray passes. 239 TM 8-300 164 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Figure 42.—Aggregation of prisms forming bi-concave lens. F, virtual focus. A Plano-convex. li Plano-concave. C Meniscus. D Convexo-concave. E Convex cylindricaL F Concave cylindrical (concavo-convex) Figure 43.—Lenses. FIGURE 44.—Convex cylindrical lens considered as segment of cylinder. 240 TM 8-300 164-165 NOTES ON EYE, EAE, ETO., IN AVIATION MEDICINE The optical center and the geometrical center of a lens do not neces- sarily coincide. The optical center of a cylindrical lens lies within the plane of the axis of the cylinder and is not a point but a line. J>. Focal length.—The focal length of a lens is the distance from the optical center to the principal focus, and this distance determines the strength of the lens. The focal length is measured in the metric system almost universally, and from it the dioptric value of a lens is easily determined. A lens of 1 diopter strength has a focal length of 1 meter, one of 2 diopters has a focal length of 50 centimeters, one of 10 diopters has a focal length of 10 centimeters. Lenses used in the cor- rection of errors of refraction are graduated in one-eighth diopter, expressed decimally; for example, plus 0.12 JS, plus 1.75 JS, plus 0.50 O, minus 0.87 JS, minus 2.25 G, etc. The focal length of a lens is easily computed, as the dioptric power of a lens is the reciprocal of the focal distance in meters; for example— Focal length of 1 diopter lens=j meter. Focal length of 2 diopter lens=i meter. Jj Focal length of y2 (0.50) diopter lens=—_— meter. 0.50 c. Conjugate points.—A lens has two principal foci, depending upon the direction from which the parallel rays of light striking a surface come. These are, of course, the same distance from its optical center. So far the effect of a lens on parallel rays of light has been considered. We may now consider rays of light emanating from a point exactly the focal length away from the optical center of a convex lens; such divergent rays, after passing through the lens would emerge parallel to one another and would never intersect. Rays of light from a point at a distance from the optical center of a greater distance than its focal length would eventually converge at a point after passing through the lens. The point from which rays of light diverge and the point at which they converge after passing through such a lens are the conjugate points or'foci. 165. Combination of lenses.—a. Rules.—Lenses may be com- bined, as a bi-convex spherical lens is actually in effect two spherical lenses together. There are seven rules regarding the combination of lenses that will enable the student to understand more thoroughly problems in the quantitative estimation of errors of refraction, as well as their correction. (1) Two spheres of the same sign are equal to a sphere having the strength of the sum of the two; for example, plus 2.00 JS combined with plus 3.00 /S'=plus 5.00 iS. 271818°—40—16 241 TM 8-300 165 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (2) Two spheres of opposite sign are equal to a sphere having the strength of the difference of the two; for example, plus 4.00 S com- bined with minus 3.00 1.00 S. (3) These above rules apply to two cylindrical lenses when both have the same axis; for example, plus 2.00 Cx 90° combined with minus 1.00 Cx 90° = plus 1.00 Cx 90°; and minus 4.00 Gx 180° combined with plus 3.00 Cx 180° = 1.00 Cx 180°. (4) Two cylinders having the same sign and strength when placed with their axes exactly at right angles make a sphere of the same sign and strength as one of the cylinders; for example, plus 3.00 Cx 90° combined with plus 3.00 Cx 180° = plus 3.00 S. (5) Two cylinders having same sign but different strengths wdien placed with their axes crossed at right angles form a sphere of a strength of the weaker cylinder combined with a cylinder equal in strength to the difference between the two and having the axis of the stronger cylinder; for example, plus 3.00 Cx 90° combined with plus 4.00 Gx 180°=plus 3.00 JS combined with plus 1.00 Cx 180°. (6) A cylinder combined with a sphere of equal strength but op- posite sign is converted into a cylinder of the same strength and sign «s the sphere but with an axis at right angles; for example, plus 2.00 Cx 90° combined with minus 2.00 JS=minus 2.00 Cx 180°; and minus 1.00 Gx 45° combined with plus 1.00 >S'=minus 1.00 Cx 135°. (7) This concerns the result of a combination of two cylinders of opposite sign when crossed with their axes at right angles. Two cylinders, one being plus (a) and the other minus (&), when crossed with their axes forming a right angle, form a plus sphere of the same strength as (a) combined with a minus cylinder of a strength of (a) and (b) with the axis of (&); or they form a minus sphere having the strength of (b) combined with a plus cylinder of a strength of (a) and (5) and having the axis of (a). For example, -we may con- sider a plus 2.00 Cx 90° combined with a minus 2.00,Cx 180°. This would result in a plus 2.0Q S, combined with a minus 4.00 Cx 180°, or a minus 2.00 jS combined with plus 4.00 Cx 90°. The two are optically the same. b. Application.—As will be seen later these rules are particularly applicable in arriving at a conclusion regarding what lenses to place in the trial frame for subjective check on the objective findings of re- tinoscopy. c. Validity.—The above rules do not hold strictly true except when the lenses are in absolute contact and when ground into a single lens, the two surfaces of which are very close together; that is, a very thin lens. A lens with a plus 10 curve on one surface and a minus 10 on 242 TM 8-300 165-166 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE the opposite will not make a piano lens in effect unless the lens is very thin. d. Combination of sphere and cylinder.—A sphere and cylinder can be combined on one surface of a lens. Such a combination can be visualized by considering a section taken from an automobile tire or a doughnut. In such an instance, there will be a curvature of a cer- tain radius in one meridian, and in the meridian at a right angle a curvature of a much shorter radius. A section from a rather wide pul- ley with a shallow groove would be an example of a lens that is minus in one meridian but plus in the meridian at the right angle. 166. Analysis of lenses.—It is important to be able to analyze a lens in order that its optical value can be known. This can be done in a number of ways but the simplest is by “neutralization.” First, we may consider a simple spherical lens: If we look at an object in the distance through the lens and move the lens we note the direction ®f the apparent movement of the object; if it moves in a direction op- posite to the movement of the lens it is a plus lens; if the object moves in the same direction it is a minus lens. This is easily accounted for when we remember that a spherical lens on section appears as an ag- gregate of prisms gradually increasing in strength as the periphery is approached. For example, if we view a point in the distance through a plus lens and the visual axis passes through the lens near the peri- phery the object will be displaced toward the periphery as we are get- ing a marked prismatic effect; apex toward the periphery; we then move the lens so that the visual axis passes through a point nearer the optical center, here the prismatic effect is not so great, so the object appears less displaced toward the periphery than before. Then the lens is moved so that the visual axis passes through the optical center of the lens and here there is no displacement of the object. Next the lens is moved laterally in the same direction as before and as the pris- matic effect increases toward the periphery the movement of the ob- ject toward the periphery becomes accentuated. Consequently, as a plus lens is moved back and forth before the eye, objects seen through the lens appear to move in the opposite direction or “against” the movement of the lens. The reverse is true regarding a minus lens which is made up of an aggregate of prisms apex toward the optical center and base toward the periphery. Therefore when a minus lens is moved back and forth before the eye objects appear to move in the same direction as the movement of the lens, or, as it is usually ex- pressed, “with” the movement of the lens. A lens in this manner may be easily identified as being plus or minus. A quantitative determina- tion may be made by “neutralizing” the lens, by using lenses of opposite sign from the trial lens case. The strength of an unknown lens is 243 TM 8-300 166-167 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT determined by the strength of the lens of opposite sign that, combined with the lens of undetermined strength, causes no movement of an ob- ject seen through the two combined. For example, a plus 2.00 JS is neu- tralized by a minus 2.00 S. “A cylinder when moved in a direction at right angles to its axis causes a parallactic movement like a convex or concave spherical glass. When moved in the direction of its axis it acts like a piano glass” (Fuchs). In addition, a cylinder causes a certain amount of distortion of objects seen through it. To test for the presence of a cylinder in a lens, two lines crossed at right angles are viewed through it and the lens slowly rotated. In certain positions the lines will appear as being at right angles, but whenever either arm of the cross coincides with the axis of the cylinder the lines will appear at right angles. For example, take a strong plus cylinder (plus 3 or 5) and with its axis vertical (90°) observe a vertical line through it; the vertical line will appear continuous above and through the lens when the line is seen through the optical center of the lens; it will appear displaced toward the periphery if seen through a portion of the cylinder lateral to the optical center, but the displaced line through the lens will be parallel to the line seen above and below the lens. Now rotate the cylinder so that the axis is about 45°, that is, clockwise; now the line, as seen through the cylinder, will appear as leaning in the opposite direction or toward axis 135°. The portion of the line nearest the periphery is farthest displaced. Therefore the line appears to rotate in a direction opposite to the rotation of the cylinder. The reverse is true with a minus cylinder. To analyze a lens two lines crossing at right angles are observed through it and the lens is moved and rotated until they appear as being continuous with the lines seen beyond the periphery of the lens. Spheres of opposite sign are added until there is no movement of the vertical line; thus the strength of this meridian is obtained. Then the meridian at right angles is neutralized and from the com- bination of the two the value of the lens is obtained. First determine the presence of a cylinder, and if such exists, its axis; then neutralize the meridian of the axis, and then the meridian at right angles to its axis; the spherical value and the cylindrical strength with its axis may thus be obtained. 167. Classification of errors of refraction.—a. (1) Emme- tropia.—The normal eye is emmetropic. Quoting Thorington, the following definitions of the emmetropic eye as listed: An emmetropic, eye is one which, in a state of rest (without any effort of accommodation "whatever), receives parallel rays of light exactly at a focus 244 TM 8-300 167 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE upon its fovea. An emmetropic eye, therefore, is one which, in a state of rest, emits parallel rays of light. An emmetropic eye is one whose fovea is situated exactly at the principal focus of its refractive system. An emmetropic eye is one the vision of which, in a state of rest, is adapted for infinity. An emmetropic eye is one which has its near point consistent with its age. An emmetropic eye is one which does not develop presbyopic symptoms until 45 or 50 years of age. (2) Ametropia,.—An ametropic eye is one which in a state of com- plete rest does not focus parallel rays of light on the retina. This condition may result from the retina’s being too near or too far away from the nodal point (axial ametropia), or due to a defect in the curvature of one or more of the refracting surfaces (curva- ture ametropia). (a) Hyperopia (hypermetropia, H, farsightedness).—In such a condition parallel rays of light are not brought to a focus on the retina, with accommodation relaxed, and the focal point is behind the retina; therefore a plus lens is required for its correction. The hyperopic eye must accommodate for infinity, consequently must accommodate more than the emmetropic eye for distances nearer than 6 meters. Hyperopia may be subdivided into the following varieties: 1. Facultative hyperopia {Hf).—Can be overcome by using the power of accommodation. 2. Absolute hyperopia {Ha).—Cannot be overcome by accom- modative effort. 3. Manifest hyperopia {Em).—Is represented by the strong- est plus lens the patient will accept, 4. Latent hyperopia {HI).—Is the additional amount revealed mnder a cyclopleglic. 5. Total hyperopia {Ht).—Is the manifest plus the latent hyperopia, and is represented by strongest plus lens required for distinct vision with accommodation com- pletely paralyzed. Facultative hyperopia becomes absolute after middle life; manifest hyperopia increases with age, while latent hyperopia decreases. With accommodation completely gone Hm=Ht. (b) Myopia {M. nearsightedness).—In such a condition parallel rays of light are brought to a focus before they reach the retina. This may be due to an antero-posterior axis too long for the dioptric system, or to an excess in power of the dioptric system. Accommo- dative power cannot in any manner come to the aid of the myope; 245 TM 8-300 167 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT therefore at distances farther away than his far point all objects are seen indistinctly. (c) Astigmatism (As).—In such a condition the dioptric power of the eye is not the same in all meridians. It frequently is the result of a difference in the corneal curvature in different meridians. The anterior surface of the cornea, in such instances, may be com- pared to a combination of plus sphere with plus cylinder. It is almost invariably a curvature ametropia. Cylindrical lenses are used for the correction of astigmatism. Astigmatism may be— 1. Irregular.—As •when the cornea is irregularly distorted, that is, the curvature may not be uniform in a single meridian. The irregular type of astigmatism may be impossible to correct by ordinary lenses. 2. Regular. (a) Simple hyperopic (as corrected by plus Gx 90°). (b) Simple myopic (as corrected by minus 1.00 Gx 180°). (c) Compound hyperopic (as corrected by plus 2 S plus 1.00 Gx 90°) . (d) Compound myopic (as corrected by minus 2 S minus 1.00 Gx 180°). (e) Mixed astigmatism.—Hyperopic in one meridian and myopic in another (as corrected by plus 2 S minus 3.00 Gx 180°). h. Regular astigmatism may be further classified as to the strength and position of the axis, as— (1) “With the rule”—As plus cylinder axis 90° or within 45° either side of 90°, or minus cylinder axis 180° or within 45° either side. (2) “Against the rule.”—As plus cylinder axis 180° or within 45° either side of 180°, or a minus cylinder axis 90° or within 45° either side. (These descriptive terms are based on statistical tables on astigmatism. As a rule plus cylinders are required axis vertical or nearly so, and minus cylinders at about axis 180°.) (3) Symmetric astigmatism.—“When the combined value, in degrees, of the meridians of shortest or longest radii of curvature in both eyes equal 180° (no more and no less), then the astigmatism in the two eyes is spoken of as symmetric” (Thorington). For example, OD plus 1.00 Cx 45°, OS plus 1.00 Gx 135°; or OD plus 0.50 Gx 75°, OS plus 0.50 Gx 105°, etc. (4) Asymmetric astigmatism.—When the combined values, in de- grees, of the two axes are not 180°. For example, OD plus 0.75 Gx 246 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 1IT1 O—OW 167—168 35°, OS plus 0.75 Cx 90°. Asymmetric astigmatism frequently accom- panies facial asymmetry. (5) Homonymous astigmatism.—When the axis of the cylinder is the same in each eye. (6) Heteronymous astigmatism.—One eye with astigmatism “with the rule” and the fellow eye “against the rule.” (7) Homologous astigmatism.—Symmetric astigmatism “with tho rule.” (8) Heterologous astigmatism.—Symmetric astigmatism “against the rule.” (9) Oblique astigmatism.—When the axis is neither vertical nor horizontal. 168. Determination of errors of refraction.—a. Methods.— There are a number of methods employed to determine quantitatively the existence of ametropia, and these may be divided into two gen- eral methods, that is, objective and subjective. Whenever possible, both methods should be employed, one being used as a check on the other. b. Scheiner method.—One method, more interesting than practical however, is that of Scheiner. It is very rarely used but may be of value to emphasize to the student optical principles involved. Two minute openings are made, a few millimeters apart, through an opaque disc, which is placed in the trial frame before the eye being examined. Accommodation is paralyzed and the pupil dilated to the extent that its diameter is greater than the distance between the two pinholes. Over one of the holes is pasted a bit of red translucent paper. The patient is directed to observe, through these pinholes, a point of light 6 meters away. If the eye is emmetropic there will be but one image of the point of light seen. If ametropia exists there will be two images of the point of light seen (a monocular diplopia), one of which will be red and the other white. Let us suppose the disc is placed before the eye with one pinhole directly above the other, and the upper pinhole is covered with the red paper (or red glass); if the eye is hyperopic, two images will be seen and the red image will be seen below the white (crossed diplopia). The strength of the plus lens required to bring the two images together represents the amount of hyperopia in the vertical meridian. Next the disc may be rotated so that the two pin- holes are in the horizontal plane and the amount of hyperopia in this meridian determined. The correction required would be the value of the two cylinders crossed at right angles. In myopia the red image will be seen above the white and the strength of a minus lens required to bring the two images together indicates the amount of myopia, etc. 247 TM 8-300 168 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT c. Cycloplegics.—The use of a cycloplegic in estimating errors of refraction enables the examiner to arrive at a definite conclusion, and further, the dilatation of the pupil renders the objective method of refraction much easier. Cycloplegics commonly used in refraction are atropine, in 1 percent solution (in children) and homatropine, 2 percent solution. Cycloplegia may be induced more quickly by scopolamine hydrobromide, 0.2 percent solution, but the possibility of the drug’s toxicity must be remembered. The use of paredrine with atropine or homatropine is now widely advocated. Atropine 2 percent, 1 drop, followed in 5 minutes by paredrine 1 percent, and 1 more drop of atropin after 15 minutes will produce satisfactory cycloplegia in 45 minutes for the examination of children. For adults we use 1 drop of 5 percent homatropin followed in 5 minutes by 1 drop 1 percent paredrine; then in 5 more minutes 1 drop 5 percent homatropine and refracting 35 minutes later. It is seldom neces- sary to use a cycloplegic after the age of 45 or 50 years. Where it is necessary to employ a cycloplegic in patients who are older, one of the weaker drugs, as homatropine, should be used, as the effect may be counteracted by the use of a miotic (eserin, y2 percent solution). Cycloplegics should never be used when glaucoma is suspected. Homatropine is used more universally for young adults, as its effect is transient, and complete recovery of power of accommodation occurs within 2 days. d. Retinoscopy.—(1) General.—Retinoscopy is the most popular objective method employed in estimation of errors of refraction. It has many advantages; it is quickly learned, is altogether objective, requires little time and only inexpensive apparatus, and is remark- ably accurate. The procedure of retinoscopy is simple. Rays of light from a mirror are reflected into the eye being examined, and as the mirror is tilted back and forth the direction of movement of the light as seen in the pupillary area is noted. For practical pur- poses it is not considered necessary to go into detail regarding the optical principles involved. Either a plane or concave mirror may be used, but with a concave mirror all movements of the spot of light will be in a direction opposite to those when the plane mirror is used. The plane mirror is more commonly used and henceforth any refer- ence to the retinoscope will be to the retinoscope with the plane mirror. (2) Simple retinoscope.—The simple retinoscope is a small cir- cular mirror with a hole through the center, or instead of a hole a minute portion of the “quicksilver” may be removed at the center, through which the examiner observes the reflex in the eye of the pa- tient. A handle is attached to one margin of the mirror. This retinoscope is used in conjunction with a source of light placed 248 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEM CINE tm 168 alongside the examinee. The light should be screened, asi with an opaque chimney, which has an opening approximately 1 centi- meter in diameter on the side facing the examiner. (3) Electric retinoscope.—The electric retinoscope has its own source of illumination placed below the mirror which is set at an angle'that reflects the rays of light at a right angle, that is, when the retinoscope is held vertically the rays of light pass forward from the mirror in a horizontal plane. (4) Purpose.—The purpose of retinoscopy is to neutralize as nearly as possible the movement of the illuminated area within the examinee’s pupil by the interposition of suitable lenses. The illumi- nated area is called the “shadow,” and when it has no movement with movements of the retinoscope, that is, neither “with” nor “against,” it is an indication that the emergent rays of light from the eye of the examinee are brought to a focus at the eye of the Figure 45.—Electric retinoscope, simple retinoscope, and electric ophthalmoscope (May model). examiner. The lens which accomplishes this neutralization of move- ment is the lens which will correct the examinee’s vision for the distance at which the retinoscope is used. (5) Distance.—It is impractical to attempt retinoscopy at a dis- tance of 6 meters. One meter is a convenient working distance, and is the distance at which the retinoscope is used ordinarily. In addi- tion it is simpler, particularly for the beginner, to make the deduction 249 xm u-juu 168 MEDICAL, DEPARTMENT for a working distance of 1 meter, which is the focal distance of a lens of 1 diopter. The student must remember that with the retino- scope he is obtaining the correction required by the patient at 1 meter’s distance; this correction is 1 diopter more than that required for infinity. Therefore, a correction of 1 diopter deduction must be made for the correction for infinity. (6) Method.—The examinee is seated at a meter’s distance in front of the examiner in a darkened room, and the examinee is directed to look fixedly at the forehead of the examiner but not directly at the mirror. A soft, clear light, not too brilliant, is reflected into the eye of the examinee by the retinoscope held firmly before the examiner’s right eye with the sighthole immediately before the pupil. The beginner probably will have some difficulty at first in finding the examinee’s eye with the beam of light; it is suggested that first it be directed toward the examinee’s chest, where it is easily located, and then the spot of illumination slowly raised by tilting the retinoscope until it reaches the eye being examined. With the retinoscope held in this manner the examiner will obtain a reflection from the pupillary area of the examinee’s eye, which will vary in its appearance depending on the amount of illumination used and the refractive error. The periphery of the so-called “shadow” is to be ignored and only the central point noted. If it is circular it is an indication that there is no great difference in the two princi- pal meridians, and very little, if any, cylinder will be required in the correction. A band-shaped reflex or “shadow” is indicative of cylinder being required, and the position of the band indicates the axis. Next the retinoscope is gently tilted back and forth not more than 2 or 3 millimeters or the reflex will be lost. The most common two principal meridians are at 90° and 180°, but they may be at any axis; they are always at right angles to one another. “The axes are indicated by the direction in which the shadow moves when the mirror is tilted. If the mirror is tilted in the vertical meridian and the shadow slides off toward 45°, we know that the two principal meridians are at 45° and 135°. We must shadow these two meridians” (Knighton). The examiner notes the form of illuminated area, its direction of movement in the different meridians, and the rate of movement. (?) Axis of cylinder required.—If the mirror is tilted vertically it is rotated about its horizontal axis, and if it is tilted laterally it is rotated about its vertical axis. The axis about which the mirror is tilted indicates the axis of the cylinder required. 250 NOTES ON EYE. EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE 168 Figure 46.—Spectacle trial frame and diagram showing positions of axes of cylindrical lenses. 251 TM 8-300 168 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (8) Type of lens.—A movement of the illuminated area with the movement of the retinoscope indicates hyperopia, emmetropia, or myopia of less than 1 diopter, and a “with” movement is neutralized by a plus lens. A movement “against” the movement of the retino- scope indicates a myopia greater than 1 diopter. An “against” movement is neutralized by a minus lens. Spheres are used to neutralize the movement in the two principal meridians. .(9) Point of neutralization.—The exact point of neutralization is difficult to obtain, consequently the point of reversal is recorded. The weaker sphere that causes a reversal of the movement is recorded as the end point. Where a movement “against” is found, it may be overcorrected by a minus lens that is obviously too high, thus giving a movement “with,” and then plus lenses added until the first move- ment against is noted. The value of the combination of the two lenses is noted and recorded. (10) Recording findings.— (a) Method.—It is suggested that the beginner use an inverted letter T in recording his findings. He may imagine that the cross bar indicates the horizontal meridian and the upward stroke the vertical meridian. If the inverted T is considered as being placed before the examinee’s face, the right angle to the left side of the examiner will indicate the findings of the right eye and the right angle on the right will indicate the findings of the left eye. (&) Examples. 1. First consider one eye alone. With the retinoscope at 1 meter there is a “with” movement in all meridians. Place a plus 1.00 S, in the trial frame, before the eye and movement is still “with”. Replace the plus 1.00 S with a plus 1.25 /S; the movement is still “with,” Gradually increase the strength of the sphere until a reversal is obtained. Suppose in this case a reversal in all me- ridians is obtained first with a plus 2.00 S. The findings are then recorded thus: +2.00 + 2.00 and, since 1 diopter must be deducted for correction for infinity— + 2.00 .. +1.00 o -e • n •, +2 00 ~-‘-==+100 = +1.00 JS for infinity In this instance two cylinders of the same sign and strength crossed at right angles are dealt with (rule (4) in combination of lenses). 252 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8—300 168 2. Next consider several other types of cases, as for example— a. + 3.00 + 4.00 _ .+2.00 I +3.00 OD +2.00 £ +3.00 + 4.00 1~+2.00 | +3.00 ~OS +3.00£ b. OD+2.00 £ + 3.00 +2.00 .+2.00 1+1.00 +1.00 Ox 90° +4.00 + 2.50 “1~+3.00 | +1.50 =(9£+1.00 £ ~~ “ — - +0.50 Ox 90° Here is an application of rule (5) in combination of lenses. For the right eye there is a reversal with vertical tilting of the retinoscope (axis 180°) with a +3.00 £, and a reversal with horizontal tilting (axis 90°) with a + 4.00 £. In this case, a +3.00 Ox 180° combined with + 4.00 Ox 90° is actually dealt with. This is equivalent to +3.00 £ (which is +3.00 in all meridians) combined with a +1.00 Ox 90° (the meridian that requires a total of +4.00 £). c. —1.00 —1.25 ._-2.00-2.25 OD-1.00£-1.00Ox 180° 0 -0.25"1 ~-1.00-1.25=OS -1.25 S-1.00 Ox 180° a. +0.50+0.75 . _—0.50 —0.25 6>Z>-0.25 £-0.25 (7a? 180° + 0.75 + 0.75~1 ~- 0.25 - 0.25~OS- 0.25 S e. + 1.75 +2.00 ,_+0.75 +1.00 _ + 0.25 -0.25 1~-0.75 -1.25 ~ OD + 0.75 £-1.50 Ox 90°, or -0.75 £+1.50 Ox 180° OS+1.00 £-2.25 Ox 90°, or -1.25 £+2.25 Ox 180° In this instance there is a mixed astigmatism; rule (7) in combination of lenses applies in this case, as in the retinoscopic findings a plus cylinder in one meridian and a minus cylinder in the other are dealt with. f. In oblique astigmatism the inverted T has to be modi- fied ; it is suggested that in such instances a right angle for each eye be used, the arms of which indicate the principal meridians, as— +2d)050 . +F00'\^'+T50 4-3^00/^^,+ 3.25 ~"1— +2^00^^\+2^25 _ OD+1.00 £+1.00 Ox 135° ~ OS +1.50 £+0.75 Ox 75° 253 TM 8-300 168 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT (11) Subjective check.—From the findings that have been de- termined by retinoscopy, the examiner has the data necessary for the subjective check. If the cycloplegic is complete and the retinoscopy done carefully the correction for infinity as found by retinoscopy should obtain an acuity of 20/20 or better, that is, with accommoda- tion paralyzed. There are several factors that necessitate a subjective test immediately after retinoscopy, particularly the exact determina- tion of axis of a cylinder as well as strengths of both sphere and cylinder. In the subjective examination the formula will be found to be somewhat less, as to spherical value (less plus or more minus). The addition of a minus 1.00 S to the findings at 1 meter corrects for infinity; with the Snellen test types at 6 meters there must be brought into play either one-sixth diopter of accommodation, or one-sixth diopter added to the formula. Consequently, when the patient is fully corrected for a distance of 6 meters he may be slightly overcorrected for infinity. One quarter diopter sphere is usually subtracted from the trial lens for 6 meters distance for actual correction for infinity. (12) Subjective examination.—A subjective examination should follow retinoscopy immediately. This is of particular value where there is a cylindrical correction, and both the strength and axis of the cylinder should be checked carefully. Each eye should be checked separately. (13) Checking axis of cylinder.—In checking the axis of the cylinder, it is shifted quickly from one side to the other (about 15° to 20°) from that found by retinoscopy, and the degree of rotation gradually decreased until maximum vision is obtained. The position of the axis of a cylinder may be confirmed by employing a stronger cylinder than that found by retinoscopy. This naturally will result in some blurring of vision, but is of value as the axis may be shifted back and forth until all vertical lines appear as vertical to the patient, then the stronger cylinder may be replaced by one of the strength as found by retinoscopy. (14) Astigmatic died.—The “clock face” or “sunburst” (astigmatic dial), consisting of radiating lines is of value in determining both the axis and the strength of a cylinder. This is based on the fact that in astigmatism lines in different meridians are not seen with equal dis- tinctness. The meridian of the eye which corresponds to the most sharply defined lines, or the blacker, is the meridian of the greatest ametropia, and the median at right angles to this is the most nearly emmetropic. The axis of the cylinder required will be at right angles to the meridian of the darkest lines. The greater number of test cabinets and charts for visual acuity have an included astigmatic dial or chart. 254 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AYIATTON MEDICINE TM 8-300 168 e. Stenopaeic slit.—The stenopaeic slit may be used for the subjective determination of astigmatic errors of refraction, with test types at 6 meters, or in connection with the astigmatic dial. The stenopaeic slit consists of an opaque disc with a very narrow {y2 to 1 mm.) slit in one diameter. The slit is of the same size as the trial lens and may be placed in the rotating cell of the trial frame; the slit, before the eye being tested, is rotated until it reaches the meridian where clearest vision is obtained; the sphere is then changed until 20/20 is seen, and a notation is made of the strength required for this meridian. Next the disc is rotated exactly through 90°, and spheres added until 20/20 Figure 47.—Retinoscopic findings. vision is secured. The total correction may be estimated fairly ac- curately by these two findings. For example, with slit placed ver- tically the patient reads 20/20, and with- the slit horizontal he only reads 20/40. But with a plus 1.25 S before the slit (placed hori- zontally) he reads 20/20. We may conclude that his correction is plus 1.00 Ox 90°. The slit is used at right angles to the axis of the cylinder required. The slit may be of value in determining ac- curately the position of the principal meridians. /. Cross cylinder.—The, cross cylinder is used to a distinct advantage in finding the strength and axis of a cylinder where such is indicated. 255 TM 8-300 168 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT The method of its use is altogether subjective. The cross cylinder, as its name implies, is a compound lens made up of two cylinders of opposite sign with axes crossed at right angles; these two cylinders are of the same strength and, of course, are equivalent to sphero- cylinder combination. For example, a minus 0.12 Cx 90° combined with a plus 0.12 Cx 180° would be equivalent to a minus 0.12 S com- bined with a plus 0.25 Cx 180°. Cross cylinders are used in strengths of 0.12 to 1.00 diopters; more frequently the middle strengths are used. The lens is mounted in a circular frame to which a handle is at- tached radially at a midpoint between the tw© principal axes, and is not placed in a cell of the trial frame but held by the handle before it. By rotating the handle 180° about its own axis, the examinee can make a decision as to> the better of the combination. For example, the handle may be held obliquely up and out so that the cross cylinder amounts to a plus Cx 90° and minus Cx 180°. By flipping it over (rotating the handle), it becomes minus Cx 90° and plus Cx 180°. In using the cross cylinder for a determination nf cylindrical strength the lens is held with first one and then the other of its principal axes coinciding with the axis of the cylinder already placed in the trial frame, or coinciding with any axis in regard to which the presence or absence of astigmatism is to be determined. A notation is made as to which position gives the best acuity, although either of the two may blur the test types to some extent. The position of the cross cylinder selected by the examinee is an indication of the change to be made in the cylinder in the trial frame, that is, an in- crease or decrease in strength. “Thus if the trial frame contains a minus cylinder with its axis at 60°, and the preferred position of the cross cylinder is that in which its minus axis is at 60°, the strength of the cylinder in the trial frame should be increased. The reverse would be true if the preferred position of the cross cylinder were that in which its plus axis was held to correspond with the axis of the minus cylinder in the trial frame. And vice versa, if the cylinder in the trail frame is a plus cylinder” (Crisp). In instances where the astigmatism is practically vertical or horizontal this determination may be somewhat at fault, as regards an exact determination, as the examinee may select the position of the cylinder which causes a ver- tical rather than horizontal distortion of the test letters. In the description of the use of the cross cylinder for the location of the exact axis required, the following is quoted directly from Dr. Crisp: The cross cylinder test for axis is at first sight rather more complicated. It Is based on the principle that two cylinders of like denomination, superimposed with their axes at an acute angle with one another, form a new cylinder of 256 TM 8-300 168 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE different strength, whose axis is somewhere intermediate between the two axes of the separate lenses. If we lay a plus 1.00 D. cylinder upon another plus 1.00 D. cylinder, so that the two plus axes are 45° apart, the combination will be equivalent to a plus 0.25 D. sphere combined with a plus 1.50 cylinder, whose axis is exactly half way between the two separate axes. If the two cylinders are of unequal strength, the new axis will be nearer the axis of the stronger individual cylinder. Suppose now that we have in the trial frame a minus 1.00 D. cylinder with its axis at 90°, and we wish to determine whether the axis should be changed in either direction from the exactly vertical position. A cross cylinder—say minus 0.50 sphere combined with plus 1.00 cylinder—is held with its two axes at 45° with the axis of the cylinder in the trial frame. If the minus axis of the cross cylinder is at 45° there is produced before the patient’s eye a new cylindric effect at an axis midway between 90° and 45°, that is at If, on the other hand, the minus axis of the cross cylinder is at 135°, the resulting minus cylinder has its axis midway between 90° and 135°, or at 112 If the axis of the patient’s astigmatic error lies nearer 45° than 135°, he may find the test type blurred by either position of the cross cylinder, but the blur will be less pronounced when the cross cylinder is held with its minus axis at 45° than when it is held with its minus axis at 135°. The change in position of the cross cylinder is again made by a simple rotation of the handle between the examiner’s thumb and index finger. The patient having expressed a preference as between the two positions of the cross cylinder, we must move the minus axis of the lens in the trial frame toward the preferred position of the minus axis of the cross cylinder. We have no exact indication as to how far this change of axis should be carried. But the cylinder in the trial frame is moved arbitrarily any distance in the indicated direction, and the test with two positions of the cross cylinder is again made, only this time the axis of the cross cylinder must be at 45° with the new position and not with the original position of 90°. At every new test, it is important that the cross cylinder shall follow the changed position of the cylinder which is in the trial frame. We may have shifted the position of the cylinder in the trial frame either too far or not far enough. In either, case the next test with the cross cylinder will tell us whether to go farther or to come part way back. But as we gradually diminish the range of alteration in the position of the cylinder in the trial frame, we shall at last reach a position in which the patient is unable to read any more or fewer of the letters with one position of the cross cylinder than with the other. We have then reached the desired point and have determined the correct axis required by the patient, subject to any change which may be obtained in check- ing up the strength of sphere and cylinder. Like all other astigmatic tests, this one is more likely to he successful if the accommodation is relaxed and, therefore, if whatever spherical lens is in the trial frame is so strong a plus or so weak a minus as barely to allow the patient to obtain his full visual acuity. Further, the patient must base his comparison of the two positions of the cross cylinder upon a study of the lowest line of letters which he can even partially or imperfectly read. It must also be remembered that the vision with the cross cylinder before the eye is very commonly less distinct than without it, and especially that at the final axis obtained the cross cylinder blurs the vision equally in both positions of the test. 271818°—40 17 257 TM 8-300 168 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT For eyes with good visual acuity the test for axis may usually be mad© satisfactorily by means of the cross cylinder of minus 0.25 sphere and plus 0.50 cylinder. For the earlier stages of testing a high error, or sometimes as a check in unusually variable cases, the minus 0.50 sphere combined with plus 1.00 cylinder is useful. The minus 1.00 sphere combined with plus 2.00 cylinder is of value in relatively amblyopic cases. In making the cross cylinder tests, the patient should usually not be asked whether he sees better with the cross cylinder or without it. What is needed is the choice between its two positions. Furthermore, there is almost never any advantage in checking the cross cylinder test for axis by means of the old- fashioned method of turning the cylinder in the trial frame in either direction until the patient decides that the vision is blurred. g. Fogging method of refraction.—In instances where the ampli- tude of accommodation is obviously within a narrow range, as with presbyopes, the “fogging” method of refraction may be resorted to. This is purely subjective and does not require the use of a cycloplegic, therefore is particularly adaptable to those who are past midlife, or those patients in whom the use of a cycloplegic is contraindicated. It is not so satisfactory in young adults, and its use is usually limited to patients who are over 45 years of age. This method simulates a cycloplegia by the induction of ciliary relaxation by the interposition of a plus sphere which is obviously of sufficient strength to over- come any ciliary muscle power the eye might otherwise use when looking at a distance of 6 meters. The eye being examined is rend- ered artificially myopic temporarily by the strong plus sphere causing the ciliary muscle to relax. This method is especially applicable in cases of hyperopia, hyperopic astigmatism, and mixed astigmatism. As an example, the patient is 47 years of age and-has a visual acuity of barely 20/20, obviously with effort. A plus 4.00 sphere is placed before the eye examined and the test types at 6 meters are illumi- nated. His vision is blurred or fogged, and in an effort to see more clearly his accommodation will relax. Minus spheres, gradually in- creasing in strength, are placed before the plus 4.00 S, until he can read 20/20. Suppose in this instance the strength of minus sphere required is 2.50. He is hyperopic to the extent of 1.50 diopters (plus 4.00 S combined with minus 2.50 >S'=plus 1.50 S). h. Strength and axis of cylinder.—Where there exists an astigma- tism, the astigmatic dial, the stenopaeic slit, and the cross cylinder may be used in testing strength and axis of cylinder required, after maximum acuity has been obtained by spherical correction alone. The stenopaeic slit may be utilized, as the two principal meridians may be located and each fogged separately. i. Manifest method.—In the manifest method, lenses are added un- til normal vision is obtained without the induction of ciliary relaxa- 258 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 168 tion. Therefore, it is not applicable to hyperopes under 45 years of age. Where myopia is suspected in markedly defective visual acuity, the use of the pinhole disc is of value, as with it vision will be greatly improved, if the visual defect is due to the myopia alone. The man- ifest method should be restricted to patients who are over 45 years of age, and in some instances, cases of compound myopia in young adults where a cycloplegic is contraindicated. “The rule is to employ the strongest plus lenses or the weakest minus lenses which will give normal vision” (Thorington). Figueb 48.—Trial lens case. j. Post-cycloplegic test.—The post-cycloplegic test (indicated where glasses are to be prescribed) should be done after complete recovery of the power of accommodation. This will be 3 to 4 days after retinoscopy where homatropine has been used, and by the find- ings at this time we are guided as to the tolerance of the patient for his full correction. It will be found as a rule young adult hyperopes will not accept the full spherical correction as determined by retin- oscopy and confirmed subjectively with cycloplegia. In the young hyperope there has been a habit acquired of correcting, either wholly 259 TM 8-300 168-169 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT or in part, his refractive error, and this habit will not be broken easily. (1) Hyperopes.—In hyperopes it is suggested that the full correc- tion (as determined previously) be placed on each eye and the two eyes checked together. If this results in blurring, add weak minus spheres (minus .12) to each eye until normal vision is obtained (20/20), and the strongest sphere giving 20/20 is that indicated to be worn. However, never add minus sphere until the total sphere becomes minus. The same strength of minus sphere must be added to each eye (otherwise an anisometropia will be induced) provided the retinoscopy and previous subjective findings have been accurate. For example, from tlie examination under cycloplegia tlie follow- ing is found: OD + 2.75o . 05+2.25/5 Four days later, after restoration of the power of accommodation, it is found that the above formula causes a marked blurring and an acuity of only 20/40, binocularly. Add a minus 0.12/5, then minus 0.25S, and eventually a minus 0.505, until 20/20 is obtained. The following acceptable formula is derived: OD + 2.75£ combined with — 0.505= +2.25$ 05+2.255 combined with — 0.50/5= + 1.755 The hyperope should wear as much plus sphere as he will accept. (2) Myopes.—The myope, at the post-cycloplegic test, will usually accept his full correction. In no instance should he be given more minus than required to give his normal vision, consequently weak minus spheres are not to be added to the full minus correction at the post-cycloplegic test, although he will probably accept more minus readily. The strength and axis of the cylinder should be checked on each eye separately. As a rule the full cylindrical correction will be accepted, even in hyperopes, and will require no reduction in strength although the sphere is reduced. However, it may be found that cylinders of high strength may not be accepted or tolerated unless the patient has worn such a correction previously. A very strong cylinder may have to be reduced in strength before being worn comfortably. 169. Correction of presbyopia.—In cases of presbyopes the cor- rection for near vision must be obtained, either for reading glasses 260 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 169-170 or for bifocals. The usual distance considered for a reading correc- tion is 33 centimeters. However, the patient’s vocation and habits must be taken into consideration. If he has formed a habit, as work- ing at a desk, his near correction will be for about 50 centimeters. The amount of plus sphere addition required will vary with several factors, principally age. The Jaeger, or some similar near vision card should be used at the distance the patient is accustomed to read, and plus spheres added to the distance correction in gradually in- creasing strengths, until Jaeger No. 1 can be read in comfort. It is better to add too little plus sphere for reading than too much. The total addition should never exceed plus 3.00N; if the patient is cor- rected for distance, he is, to all intents and purposes, emmetropic, and only 3 diopters of accommodation are required by the emmetrope for reading at 33 centimeters. Consequently, no more than 3 diopters are required even if the patient has no accommodative power as an addition for reading. As a general rule in regard to the addition required for near vision the following plus spherical lenses will be required for reading in addition to the distance correction: Age Plus sphere addition + 0.50 S to +1.00 S. + 1.00 S to +1.50 S. + 2.00 S to +2.50 S. + 2.50 S to +3.00 S. Practically invariably the same addition in plus sphere will be required for each eye. Care should be taken in prescribing a different “add” for reading for each eye, such may indicate an error in the correction for distance, or in some instances, rarely encountered, may mean a difference in accommodative power of the two eyes due to disease or injury. 170. Transposition of formula.—Transposition of a formula may be useful in order to utilize a thinner lens, to compare two for- mulae, and to make similar prescriptions in the two eyes. In the trans- position of any formula the sign of the cylinder is changed, and 90° 261 TM 8-300 170-171 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT added to the axis to obtain the new cylinder. The new sphere is the algebraical sum of the sphere and cylinder. For example— + 3.00 £ with -4.00 Gx 90°= -1 £+4.00 Gx 180° and -2.00 £-1.00 Cx 90° = - 3.00 £+1.00 Cx 180° and + 1.00 £+0.50 Gx 180° =+1.50 £-0.50 Gx 90° and + 1.00 £-1.00 Gx 90° = O £+1.00 Gx 180° The transposition of a sphere does not change its optical value. The optical principles involved are those of rule (7) in the combination of lenses. For example— ' +2.75 Cx 45°-3.25 Gx 135°= +2.75 £-6.00 Cx 135° or (transposing) -3.25 £+6.00 Cx 45° As an example, in the comparison of prescriptions we may deter- mine by retinoscopy that a patient, in our opinion, should wear a minus 0.75 £ plus 1.00 Cx 90°. He has been wearing habitually minus 1.00 Cx 180°. These two formulae appear at a glance to be quite dis- similar. Nevertheless, they are almost the same; transposing our find- ings we have plus 0.25 £ minus 1.00 Cx 180°, and the difference between the two is only minus 0.25 £. Minus 0.25 Cx 45° and plus 0.25 Cx 135° appear at first to be two entirely different formulae, but actually there is a difference of 0.25 £ between the two. The optician who grinds the lenses may transpose a formula to obtain a lens that is thinner or thicker, as the case may be. A formula may be transposed for the purpose of making prescrip- tions for the two eyes similar, that is, plus or minus cylinder for each eye, instead of a plus cylinder for one and a minus cylinder for the other. 171, Decentering of lenses.—Occasionally a spectacle lens may be decentered to obtain a prismatic effect. Decentering a plus sphere gives the effect of a prism base toward the direction of decentering (base will be toward the optical center). A decentered minus sphere gives the effect of base away from the optical center. The same is true of a cylinder decentered at right angles to the axis. The prismatic effect of a decentered lens will vary with the refractive index of the glass, but as a rule, for every centimeter of decentering there will be produced as many prism diopters as there are diopters in the meridian which is decentered. For example, consider a plus 4.00 £; if the lens is decentered out (the optical center 1 cm. lateral 262 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 171-173 to the geometric center) the effect is a 4 diopter prism base out. Or another method— number of millimeters decentering where n equals prism diopters required and d equals dioptric strength of the lens. For example, 2 prism diopters are required in a plus 10 X 2 4.00 S\—-—=10 millimeters. The optical center would have to be placed 10 millimeters to the nasal side of the geometric center in this instance. Section XXI OPHTHALMOSCOPIC EXAMINATION Paragraph Ophthalmoscope 172 Examination of media 173 Indirect method 174 Direct method 175 Color of normal fundus 176 Optic disc 177 Retinal blood vessels 178 Central area of retina 179 Periphery of retina 180 172. Ophthalmoscope.—a. Use.—The invention of the ophthal- moscope by Helmholtz in 1851 opened a new field of possibility in the diagnosis of systemic disease as well as affections involving the eye alone. By the use of this instrument the interior of the living eye may be examined, that is, those structures behind the lens, the vitreous, retina, choroid, intraocular nortion of the optic nerve, and the retinal blood vessels. b. Description.—Essentially the simple ophthalmoscope consists of a mirror, plane or concave, with a perforation or “peephole” at the center. Kays of light are reflected by the mirror through the pupil illuminating the interior of the eye and the details of the fundus. The dioptric system of the eye itself is utilized in obtaining an image of the structures under observation through the aperture or perforation in the mirror. The instrument is augmented by the use of lenses, plus and minus spheres, so placed in a disc that any one desired may be rotated before the opening in the mirror. c. Modifications.—There are many designs and modifications of the ophthalmoscope, of which the electric, which has its own source of illumination, is particularly useful, especially in the examination of patients who are bedridden, although the older type, as the Loring, 263 TM 8-300 172 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT still has some distinct advantages which are not replaced entirely by the more modern electric types. The more popular models employed are those designed by May, Morton, Jackson, and Marple. Some of the electric ophthalmoscopes use prisms to deflect the emergent rays of light rather than mirrors. Further, some are designed to use ordinary 110-volt current through a transformer or from dry cells carried in the handle of the instrument. Regardless of the type or model, all ophthal- moscopes are designed for the purpose of inspecting the media and fundus of the eye. d. Pupil in examination.—The ophthalmoscopic examination is ac- complished more thoroughly through a widely dilated pupil and with the accommodation of the examinee relaxed. The reasons are quite obvious. In the examination of applicants for Air Corps training the ophthalmoscopic examination should follow refraction routinely, while the pupil is still widely dilated and accommodation paralyzed. The examiner should wear his own correction for error of refraction when using the ophthalmoscope. e. Practice.—The student is advised that upon every opportunity he take advantage of the occasion to use the ophthalmoscope in a routine manner and that he adopt a definitely outlined procedure in order that no detail be overlooked, even where no pathology of the media or fundus may be suspected. Every fundus, normal or other- wise, is within itself a most interesting and instructive study, and every fundus is a new story, as no two are alike. The use of the ophthalmoscope is not limited to the ophthalmologist but is a diag- nostic instrument that is utilized to an advantage by the internist, the surgeon, the pathologist, obstetrician, and the general practi- tioner. The latter should familiarize himself with its use as he does his stethoscope. But to be adept in the use of the ophthalmo- scope, and particularly in the interpretation of ophthalmoscopic findings, will require an infinite amount of practice, perseverence, patience, and study; otherwise, the fundus of the eye may be a meaningless, although interesting and beautiful picture. /. Abnormal conditions of fundus.—It is not possible nor practi- cable in this section to describe the various appearances of abnormal conditions of the fundus; the student is encouraged to avail him- self of the opportunity to study the colored plates of Wilmer, Adam, and Clarke, and the stereoscopic plates of Oatman when possible, as so much more may be expressed by a picture, in colors, than by pages of description. g. Light.—The ophthalmoscope should be used in a darkened room, or at least the amount of light falling upon the eye being 264 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 172-174 examined should be reduced to a minimum, that is, light from sources other than the ophthalmoscope. 173. Examination of media.—In the examination of the media the examiner, standing or sitting on the right side of the examinee when examining the right eye, holding the ophthalmoscope in his right hand before his right eye (in a manner similar to the retino- scope) approaches the examinee until the details of the iris pattern are seen through the aperture. The pupillary area will appear as a dull red reflex. Where there exists an opacity (in cornea, anterior chamber, lens, or vitreous) it will appear dark against the red back- ground—a bit darker than it actually is, as it is seen partially by reflected light. The approximate location of the opacity may be determined by having the examinee shift his visual axis slightly from the right to the left, or up and down; if the opacity is anterior to the pupillary plane it will move “with” the eye movement, if it is posterior to the pupillary plane it will move “against” the move- ment of the eye. The strength of the lens before the aperture may be increased or decreased as desired, but with any change in the lens strength the position of the ophthalmoscope before the eye must be altered, that is, brought closer to the eye with a stronger and farther away with a weaker lens. The self-illuminating types of ophthalmoscopes are equipped usually with a rheostat which controls the amount of illumination, and faint and diffuse opacities may be seen more easily with illumination reduced (Wilmer). The exam- iner should always use his right eye in examining the right eye, and left when examining the left eye of the examinee. 174. Indirect method.—After the media has been inspected the fundus may be examined by the indirect method. And for this pur- pose the older type of reflecting ophthalmoscope, as the Loring, may be used to an advantage. The indirect method of ophthalmoscopy is not employed as frequently as it should be in many instances. It should be remembered that it has several distinct advantages; a greater portion of the fundus is seen at one time than by the direct method, and it is particularly applicable in high degrees of ame- tropia and where there exist slight opacities in the media. A plus 4 to 8 diopter lens is rotated before the aperture of the ophthalmo- scope, and a plus 18. diopter lens (from the trial lens case) is held before the examinee’s eye with the other hand. The examinee is directed to look directly forward. The plus 18. lens, held between the thumb and forefinger of the examiner, is held before the eye being observed and steadied by the other fingers resting against the forehead. The examiner then approaches the examinee until a view of the fundus is obtained. By shifting the position of the plus 18. 265 TM 8-300 174-176 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT lens the details may be brought more clearly into focus. Various portions of the fundus may be brought into view by having the examinee shift his visual axis. The indirect method gives a magni- fication of 3 to 5 diameters and the image of the fundus seen is always inverted. It is to be remembered that the room must be darkened, and after the student has once seen the fundus clearly he may change the lens from the trial lens case, increasing or decreas- ing its strength, and change the strength of the lens in the aperture of the ophthalmoscope until he finds the combination best adapted for his purpose. 175. Direct method.—In the direct method of using the ophthal- moscope the examiner holds the instrument as near the examinee’s eye as possible, using “right eye for right eye and left for left.” The strength of the lens to be used in the aperture will depend upon the dioptric power of the eye being examined and the ability of the examiner to relax his own accommodation (taking for granted he is wearing his correction for ametropia). If the examinee is em- metropic and the examiner can voluntarily relax his accommodation no lens will be required in the aperture. As a rule the beginner will have some difficulty in learning to relax Ms accommodation and at first will prefer to use a weak concave lens in the aperture. He should learn to use the ophthalmoscope with both eyes open and in examining the fundus should imagine that he is looking at a picture some distance away in order to relax his accommodation. In ex- ploring the fundus the position of the ophthalmoscope may be shifted and the position of the examinee’s visual axis changed to bring into view different portions. By the direct method of ophthalmoscopy a magnification of about 14 diameters is obtained and the image is upright. In the examination of the fundus by the direct method a change in the dioptric strength of the lens in the aperture of the ophthal- moscope of 3 diopters represents an approximate difference in depth of 1 millimeter. For example, with no lens in the aperture the level of the outer portion of the disc is clearly seen but the bottom of the physiologic cup is blurred; if a minus 3 diopter sphere in the aperture gives a sharp definition of the bottom of the cup we may conclude that it is 1 millimeter below the level of the disc itself. 176. Color of normal fundus.—The color of the normal fundus varies with individuals and in general is in keeping with the amount of pigment of the skin and hair, and depends upon the amount of pigment in the epithelial layer of the retina and in the choroid. Consequently the fungus of the pure-blooded negro is chocolate colored due to the fact that the amount of pigment in the epithelical 266 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE TM 8-300 176-177 layer of the retina is so dense that very little of the underlying choroid may be seen through it. In the brown mulatto it may be described as being of a chocolate red color. In the Indian and Chinese it begins to assume a more stippled appearance, and in the latter has a distinct yellowish tinge. With the Caucasians it varies from an orange red in the Mediterranean types to a light orange red in the pronounced blondes. With the latter the retinal pigment is so scanty that the underlying choroidal vessels are seen distinctly and even certain portions of the sclera may show either faintly or distinctly, due to scantiness of pigment of the choroid. In the albino pigment is lacking altogether in the retina and choroid, so portions of the sclera that are not obscured by retinal and choroidal vessels are seen distinctly, especially toward the periphery. 177. Optic disc.—The optic disc (papilla) or the intraocular por- tion of the optic nerve is perhaps the most noticeable feature of the fundus and by its diameter as a standard of comparison the lo- cation of other points of interest are noted. The disc is located somewhat to the nasal side of the posterior pole of the eye (about 15°). It is roughly circular in form and from it the retinal blood vessels radiate. The disc is made up of innumerable nerve fibers of the optic nerve spreading out from the main trunk over the entire surface of the retina, in a manner somewhat similar to a strand of rope being frayed at one end and evenly distributed over a concave surface after passing through a perforation in the surface. How- ever, these fibers are transparent. Consequently, the disc presents normally a depression or physiologic cup more or less centrally located. At the bottom of this depression there may be found the reticular or mesh-like strands of the lamina cribosa, the net-like fibers of the inner third of the sclera bridging across the opening. At the lamina cribosa the nerve fibers normally lose their medullary sheaths for the sake of transparency. The depth and diameter of the physiologic cup vary a great deal in normal eyes, and it may be so noticeable that it resembles a glaucomatous excavation. This portion of the disc is the paler in color, that is, whiter than the peripheral portion which may be described as being a pearl pink, which is derived from the capillaries invading its substance. The disc is usually paler on its temporal side, inasmuch as there are fewer nerve fibers emerging from this side. Immediately surround- ing the disc and more noticeable on the temporal side there may be seen a white ring concentric with the disc, which represents, where it does exist, a portion of the surrounding sclera which is visible because the retina and choroid do not quite meet the disc margin. 267 TM 8-300 177-179 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Frequently there may be found an accumulation of granules of pig- ment from the choroid, usually on the temporal side of the disc which may fill in partially the scleral ring. In myopia the scleral ring may be so marked that it assumes a crescentic form. 178. Retinal blood vessels.—The retinal blood vessels emerge from the disc for their distribution over the retinal surface. The retinal circulation is terminal and normally does not anastomose within itself nor does it anastomose with the choroidal circulation. It can best be visualized after a review of its ontogenetic develop- ment. Occasionally there may be found an anastomosis (cilio- retinal) with a branch from one of the short posterior ciliary vessels, which enter the globe immediately around the optic nerve. Such an anastomosis is the exception rather than the rule. 179. Central area of retina.—a. Blood supply.—The central artery of the retina is the first branch of the ophthalmic (a branch of the internal carotid) after it enters the orbit. It enters into the substance of the optic nerve 7 millimeters to 12 millimeters back of the lamina cribrosa and passes forward to emerge on the optic disc where it divides into a superior and inferior branch, each of which subdivides into temporal and nasal branches. The bifurcation of the central artery may occur behind the lamina cribrosa, or on the other hand some distance after it has passed beyond the margin of the disc on the retinal surface. The two temporal branches (su- perior and inferior) appear to encircle almost completely the macular region and send numerous branches toward it. Veins accompany the arteries in their distribution and are distinguished from them by the fact that the arteries are of a brighter red appearance, are narrower, and in their course are more direct or less tortuous than the veins. The central vein emerges from the globe by way of the optic disc and optic nerve. “The vein has a longer course in the subarachnoid space of the optic nerve than the artery. Therefore, in cases of in- creased intracranial pressure, it is more subject to engorgement from compression” (Wilmer). The vein usually empties directly into the cavernous sinus, ©r it may join the superior ophthalmic vein. The walls of the blood vessels or of the retina are transparent nor- mally, and what is seen as a vessel is the column of blood within its lumen. The vessels, both arteries and veins, show a definite light streak along their axes. This is more pronounced, that is, wider and brighter, on the arteries. This difference is probably due to the reflex from the thicker media of the arterial walls, while the reflex on veins is due to the reflection from the anterior surface of the column of blood alone (Wilmer). 268 TM 8-300 179-180 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE Where choroidal vessels are seen, as in the fundus of a blonde in- dividual, they may be distinguished easily from the retinal vessels, as they appear flat, show no light streaks, anastomose freely, and appear deeply embedded. h. Nutrition.—The retina as a whole is dependent upon two sources for its nutrition. The central artery of the retina with its branches takes care of the inner layers to the depth of the outer nuclear layer. The epithelial layer of the retina is dependent upon the capillary bed of the choroid underneath. Consequently it is readily under- stood that retinal degeneration will follow invariably degenerative or atrophic changes in the choroid. c. Crossing vessels.—Veins and arteries frequently cross one an- other in the retina but never does a vein cross a vein nor an artery cross an artery. d. Pulsation.—Venous pulsation in the retina is frequently en- countered and usually is of no significance. It is diastolic and is more noticeable at the disk where the vessel bends abruptly. During the acme of systole the intraocular tension presumably rises to the ex- tent that the walls of the veins collapse. Arterial pulsation, as seen with the ordinary ophthalmoscope, is considered as definitely patho- logic and may be indicative of increased intraocular tension, orbital tumors, hyperthyroidism, and aortic insufficiency. Under good illumi- nation and magnification arterial pulsation may frequently be seen in normal eyes. e. Central area.—After locating the disc, if the area temporal to it is explored, the central area of the retina will be found. This region will be noted in contrast with other portions of the fundus, as it appears to be shunned or avoided by the larger blood vessels. It is an area about 7 millimeters in diameter and contains the macula lutea, the fovea centralis, the fundus foveae and the foveola. The macula lutea is an area centrally located in the central area of the retina about 1.5 millimeters to 2 millimeters in diameter, generally circular in out- line. It is noticeably darker in color than other portions of the retina and frequently will be found to be somewhat granular in appearance. The fovea centralis occupies the central portion of the macula lutea and represents approximately the posterior pole of the eye, being about 3.6 millimeters to the temporal side of the disc. It is actually a funnel- shaped depression at the bottom of which is the fundus foveae, which is the thinnest portion of the retina. The minute foveola is at the center of the fundus foveae. 180. Periphery of retina.—a. The extreme periphery of the retina is quite difficult to bring into view. However, with a widely 269 TM 8-300 180 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT GLASS' I 2 3 VISION 20/20 20/40 20/100 D.R 50 55 USO. MX 10 M.T.IO . MX 12 MX4 WITH I5.13 LX 4 _ L/ 0 ■ mx 5 m.t.5 mx t HYPER mtI MX I mt2 p.l) . M.T. 15 C) R L.T, 5 - P. IX MUST HQU A L OR EXCEED ANY ESC), RJ.ENS DIPLOPIA V/ITH IN 50 CM. ANGLE 40° 40° IUJ R. M.T. 1.5 TOTAL OR M.T. 50 CYL. AfM'M thread A U w ll. A G E DIOPTERs IB B.B APEX IN ESC). IB 0.7 20 0.5 APEX OUT EXO. 21 0.2 22 7 9 APEX UP HYPER 'j'i /.(> THAT EYE 24 7.4 25 7- 2 20 0 • 9 27 0 • 0 Figure 49.—Summary of minimal visual requirements for flying. 270 TM 8-300 180 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE dilated pupil and cooperative patient a view may be obtained slightly anterior to the equator. Each quadrant should be explored carefully. h. Upon the completion of the ophthalmoscopic examination it is suggested that a miotic (eserine salicylate % percent solution) be instilled. g. For a very complete review of pathological conditions of the fundus the student is advised to study carefully the colored plates from Atlas Fundus Oouli, by Dr. William H. Wilmer. 271 TM 8-300 180 MEDICAL DEPARTMENT Appendix ABBREVIATIONS AND REFERENCES 1. Abbreviations and signs used in ophthalmology. A or Acc Accommodation. Am Ametropia. As Astigmatism, astigmatic. As. H Hyperopic astigmatism. As. M Myopic astigmatism. Ax : Axis (of cylindrical lens). B Base (of prism). C or cyl - Cylindrical lens or cylinder. cm Centimeter. D Diopter. E Emmetropia, or emmetropic. F Field of vision. H Hyperopia, hyperopic, horizontal. HI -i Hyperopia latent. Hm Hyperopia manifest. Ht Hyperopia total. L or L. E Left eye. M Myopia or myopic. m Meter. M. A Meter angle. mm Millimeter. n Nasal. O. D. (R. or R. E.) Genius dexter (right eye). O. S. (L, or L. E.) Genius sinister (left eye). O. U Genius uterque (both eyes). Gph Opthalmoscope or opthalmoscopic. P. D Prism diopter. P. L Perception of light. P, p Punctum proximum (near point). P, r Punctum remotum (far point). Pr Presbyopia. R. or R. E Right eye. S or Sph Spherical lens. t Temporal. 272 TM 8-300 180 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN AVIATION MEDICINE T Tension. Y Vision, visual acuteness, vertical, w With. + Plus or convex. — Minus or concave. Equal to. Combined with. Infinity (20 feet or more distance). Foot, minute. Inch, second. Line. Degree (prism). Centrad (prism). Prism diopter. 271818°—40 18 273 TM 8-300 180 MEDICAL. DEPARTMENT 2. References.—The part of this manual devoted to otolaryn- gology is not intended to be a textbook on otolaryngology, nor is it written for the purpose of supplanting existing textbooks on the sub- ject. The purpose of the outline is to stress important points in the examination of the eye, ear, nose, throat, and adjacent structures, to enumerate and explain the disqualifying findings prescribed in regu- lations, to review certain conditions affecting these parts of particular importance to the flyer, and to bring out certain practical points in the treatment of these conditions. No attempt has been made to give a detailed description of the anatomy or the physiology of the parts under discussion, except the labyrinth, as these are fully described in the standard anatomies and textbooks of otolaryngology. The material presented in this manual was derived from lectures on the examination of the eye, ear, nose, and throat given at the School of Aviation Medicine during the past several years. It is obvious that such a manual must be, of necessity, a compilation from various texts and authors. Further, the student should have reviewed clinical ophthalmology or should study some standard text in conjunction with this manual. The following smaller texts are recommended: Diseases of the Eye, by Sir John Herbert Parsons. Diseases of the Eye, by Charles H. May. The Eye, by C. W. Rutherford. For reference reading the following are recommended: Textbook of Ophthalmology, volumes I and II, by Duke Elder. Fuchs'1 Textbook of Ophthalmology. Diseases of the Eye, by De Schweinitz. Anatomy of the Human Orbit, by Whitnall. Clinical Physiology of the Eye, by Adler. Anatomy of the Eye and Orbit, by Wolff. Anatomy and Histology of the Human Eyeball, by Salzmann. Clinical Ophthalmology for the General Practitioner, by Ramsay. The Pathology of the Eye, by Friedenwald. Squint, by Claude Worth. Atlas of External Diseases of the Eye, by Neame. Medical Ophthalmology, by Moore. Visual Field Studies, by Lloyd. Methods of Refraction, by Thorington. Practice of Refraction, by Duke Elder. 274 TM 8-300 .180 NOTES ON EYE, EAR, ETC., IN’ AVIATION MEDICINE Outline of Ocular Refraction, by Maxwell. Ophthalmic Optics, by Cowan. Errors of Accommodation and Refraction, by Clarke. Outline of Refraction, by Knighton. An Illustrated Guide to the Slit Lamp, by Butler. Slit Lamp Microscopy of the Living Eye, by Koby. Diagnostics of the Fundus Oculi, by Oatman. Atlas Fundus Oculi, by Wilmer. Interned Disease of Eye and Atlas of Ophthalmoscopy, by Troncosos. The Eye and Its Diseases, by Berens. It is suggested that where some point is not made clear in the manual the student should avail himself of the opportunity of a better and more detailed explanation in the above texts. AR 40-110 (Standards of Physical Examination for Flying) is not included in this manual for two reasons. Primarily, it would be a needless duplication as these regulations are available to the student in a bound volume, and secondarily the regulations themselves are subject to change from time to time. Upon the completion of each section of the manual the student should study carefully the para- graph of AR 40-110 pertaining to that particular phase of the examina- tion, paying special attention to the procedure, precautions, and interpretation of findings. 275 TM 8-300 INDEX Paragraphs Pages Abbreviations App. 271 Abduction, power of 109 135 Optical principles involved 11Q 135 Accessory sinuses 15 11 Accommodation of eye 120-126 154 Amplitude 121 155 Influence of age 123 156 Calculating in diopters 125 158 Changes in dioptive power 120 154 Determining power of 124 157 Far and near points 122 155 Near vision 126 159 Acuity, visual 71-79 91 Alining power of eye 75 93 Conditions that interfere with 76 93 Factors— Physiological 71 91 Psychological 71 91 Formation of retinal image 72 91 Of retina 74 92 Resolving power of the eye 73 92 Adduction, power of HI 135 Adjunctive group of factors, depth perception 86 101 Aero-otitis media 30-36 22 Complications 34 26 Definition 30 22 Diagnosis! 33 26 Etiology 31 22 Pathology 36 28 Symptomatology 32 23 Treatment 35 26 Analysis of lenses 166 243 Anatomy of— Middle ear 27 18 Inner ear 38-43 29 Angle of convergence H3 136 Meter H6 137 Anterior chamber, inspection 134 168 Apparatus, lachrimal 132 163 Associated factors, heterophoria 108 134 Auditory nerve, branches 40 35 Binocular— Factors, depth perception 82 98 Loupe 126 162 Movements 64 120 Parallax 35 101 Projections 60 111 277 TM 8-300 INDEX Paragraphs Pages Blind flying 61, 62 54, 66 Essentials 61 54 Medical contribution 62 66 Blind spot, physiologic 162 236 Blood vessels, retinal 178 268 Bones, facial, fracture of 19 13 Branches of auditory nerve: Cochlear 40 35 Vestibular 40 35 Calculating accommodation in diopters 125 158 Caloric douche test 60 54 Cavity of nose 11 9 Central area of retina 179 268 Classification of defects in visual fields 161 233 Cochlea 43 40 Cochlear branch, auditory nerve 40 35 Color blindness, military significance 144 194 Color— Field of vision 159 225 Confrontation test 157 223 Perception: Examinations 146-148 200 Methods 153 216 Tests 149-152 213 Directions for use 154,155 217,220 Trichromic 141 187 Vision 139-155 175 Definitions 139 175 Dichromic 142 189 Effect of shortened red 143 193 Physical basis 140 177 Tests of 145 197 Holmgren 152 214 Ishihara.. 149 213 Jennings 151 214 Stillings 150 213 Complete oculomotor paralysis 119 150 Complications of aero-otitis media 34 26 Conditions interfering with normal usual acuity 76 93 Confrontation test, field of vision 157 223 Convergence, vision 112-114 136 Angle of 113 136 Near point 114 136 Prism 112 136 Correction of presbyopia 169 260 Coston’s syndrome 63 81 Deafness in aviation 67 86 Decentering of lenses 171 262 Defects in visual field, classification 161 233 278 INDEX TJVl 8-300 Paragraphs Pages Depth perception 80-89 98 Defective, etiology 88 105 Definition 80 98 * Factors: Adjunctive group 81, 86 98, 101 Basic 81 98 Binocular 82 98 Diplopia, physiological 84 100 Monocular 82 98 Parallactic angle, measuring 87 104 Retinal image, size of 83 99 Determining accommodative power 124 157 Deviations, optical: Horizontal, principles involved 102 125 Vertical 103 126 Diagnosis of aero-otitis media 33 26 Dichromic color vision 142 189 Diopters, calculating accommodation 125 158 Dioptric power, changes in 120 154 Directions for use of tests 154-155 217 Holmgren (yarn) 155 220 Ishihara plates 154 217 Distance, interpupillary 115 136 Ear: Anatomy • 27 18 Conditions 63-68 81 Coston’s syndrome 63 81 Deafness in aviators 67 86 Labyrinthitis 66 84 “Leans” 68 87 Meniere’s symptom complex 65 83 Tinnitus aurium 64 83 External 21-25 14 Inner, anatomy and physiology 38-43 29 Middle, effects of flight on 26-37 18 Errors of refraction, classification 167 244 Essentials of blind flying 61 54 Etiology of— Defective depth perception 88 105 Heterophoria 105 129 Middle ear 31 22 Examinations; Color perception 146-MS 200 Eye 69, 70 88, 90 Equipment 69 88 Lens... 136 170 Pupils 135 169 Purpose 70 90 Sighting 100 123 279 TM 8-300 INDEX Examinations—Continued Paragraphs Pages Eyelids 131 162 Media 173 265 Methods: Direct 175 266 Indirect V. 174 265 Ophthalmoscopic 172-180 263 Vitreous, opacities of 137 171 External— Ear 21-25 14 Nose 10 8 Extrinsic ocular muscles, physiology 93 116 Eye: Alining power 75 93 Equipment 129-131 162 Examination 69, 70 88, 90 Illumination, focal or oblique 128 161 Inspection 127-138 161 Structure *._■ 127 161 Face 2 3 Facial— Bones, fracture 19 13 Injuries, maxillo 16-20 12 Lacerations 17 12 Field of— Fixation 91 113 Vision 156-162 221 Color 159 225 Confrontation test 157 223 Defects in, classification 161 233 Form 159 225 Perimeter 158 224 Peripheral 156 221 Tangent screen 160 228 Flight effects on middle ear 26-37 18 Focal illumination 128 161 Formation of retinal image 72 91 Fractures, facial bone 19 13 Hand slit-lamp 130 162 Hearing tests 23 15 Heterophoria 92 115 Associated factors 108 134 At 33 centimeters 104 127 Etiology.: 105 129 Incidence 106 132 Methods of detecting 107 133 Holmgren test 152 214 Horizontal deviations, optical principles involved 102 125 Illumination, focal or oblique 128 161 Influence of age upon amplitude of accommodation 123 156 280 INDEX TM 8-300 Paragraphs Pages Imbalance, technique of determining 101 124 Injuries: Maxillofacial ; 16-20 12 Nose 18 13 Orbit 20 14 Inner ear: Anatomy and physiology 38-43 29 Cochlea 43 40 Labyrinth: Membranous 39 32 Osseous 38 29 Semicircular canal f 42 37 Sense organs, special 41 36 Vestibule, physiology 42 37 Inspection of eye 127-138 161 Anterior chamber 134 168 127 161 Binocular loupe 129 162 Hand slit-lamp 130 162 Examination of eyelids 131 162 Iris 134 168 Lacrimal apparatus 132 163 Lens 136 170 Nystagmus 138 174 Opacities 137 171 Pupils 135 169 Sclera 133 166 Vitreous 137 171 Intensity of sound 25 16 Interpupillary distance 115 136 Ishihara test 149 213 Plates, use, in 154 217 Jennings test 151 214 Labyrinth: Membranous 39 32 Osseous 38 29 Labyrinthitis 66 84 Lacerations, facial 17 12 Lacrimal apparatus 132 163 Larynx 8 7 “Leans” - 68 87 Lens examination 136 170 Lenses 163-171 238 Analysis 166 243 Classification 163 238 Combination 165 241 Decentering 171 262 Focal length 164 239 Principal focus 164 239 Transposition of formula 170 261 281 TM 8-300 INDEX Paragraphs Pages Maddox rod 98 122 Maxillo facial injuries 16-20 12 Bones, fracture 19 13 Lacerations 17 12 Nose 18 13 Orbit 20 14 Media, examination of 173 265 Medical contributions, blind flying 62 66 Membrane, tympanic 22 14 Membranous labyrinth 39 32 Meniere's symptom complex 65 83 Meter angle, convergence 116 137 Metric system in visual acuity 78 95 Methods of detecting heterophoria 107 133 Middle ear 21-25 14 Aero-otitis media 30-37 22 Anatomy 27 18 Definitions 1 30 22 Effect of flight on 26-37 18 Etiology 31 22 Pathology 36 28 Physiology 28 20 Symptomatology 32 23 Terminology , 29 22 Military significance of color blindness 144 194 Monocular— Factor of depth perception 82 98 Projections 90 111 Mouth 1 2 Movements: Ocular 90-119 111 Binocular 94 120 Near point, convergence 114 136 Near vision 126 159 Nose r 9-15, 18 8, 13 Cavity 11 9 External 10 8 Injuries 18 13 Obstruction 12 9 Polyps 14 11 Septum 13 10 Sinuses 15 11 Numbering of prisms 96 121 Nystagmus: Ocular, physiological, and vestibular, differentiation between 45 43 Inspection for 138 174 Test 57 53 Oblique illumination 128 161 Obstruction of nose 12 9 282 INDEX TM 8-300 Ocular— Movements 90-119 111 Nystagmus 45 43 Oculomotor paralysis 119 150 Opacities of vitreous 137 171 Ophthalmoscope 172 263 Optic disc 177 267 Optical principles: Abduction 110 135 Adduction 111 135 Horizontal deviations 102 125 Orbit, injuries to 20 14 Organs, special sense, inner ear 41 36 Orthophoria 92 115 Osseous labyrinth 38 29 Parallactic angle 87 104 Parallax, binocular 85 101 Past-pointing 49 47 After douching 50 49 Pathology of aero-otitis media 36 28 Perception: Color, trichromic 141 187 Depth 80-89 98 Perimeter, field of vision 158 224 Peripheral vision 156 221 Periphery of retina 180 269 Phorometer trial frame 99 123 Physical basis of color vision 140 177 Physiologic blind spot 162 236 Physiological— Diplopia 84 100 Nystagmus 45 43 Physiology of— Inner ear 38-43 29 Middle ear 28 20 Extrinsic ocular muscles 93 116 Pitch -v 24 16 Point of convergence 114 136 Meter angle 116 137 Pointing test 58 53 Polyps 14 11 Power, eye: Abduction 109 135 Optical principle involved 110 135 Accommodative, determining 124 157 Adduction 111 135 Determining accommodative 124 157 Dioptric, changes in 120 154 Presbyopia, correction of 169 260 Paragraphs Pages 283 TM 8-300 INDEX Paragraphs Pages Prisms 95 120 Convergence 112 136 Numbering 96 121 Rotary 97 122 Projection; Monocular 90 111 Binocular 90 111 Pupils, examination 135 169 Ranula 5 6 Readablity of various letters 79 96 Red spectrum end. shortened, effect of 143 193 References App. 271 Refraction 163-171 238 Errors— Classification 167 244 Determination 168 247 Formula transposition of 170 261 Resolving power of eye 73 92 Retina: Acuity of 74 92 Central area 179 268 Periphery of 180 269 Retinal—■ Blood vessels 178 268 Image; Formation 72 91 Size 83 99 Rotary prism 97 122 Sclera- inspection of 133 166 Seasickness 48 47 Semicircular canals, physiology — 42 37 Septum 13 10 Sighting eye 100 124 Sinuses 15 11 Snellen test letters 77 94 Squint 118 138 Stillings test 150 213 Symptomatology of aero-otitis media 32 23 Syndrome Costen’s 63 81 Tangent- Rule - — 160 228 Screen 160 228 Technique of determining imbalance 101 124 Teeth - 3 4 Terminology middle ear 29 22 Tests: Color vision 145 197 Hearing 23 15 Heterophoria, at 33 centimeters 104 127 284 TM 8-300 INDEX Tests—Continued Holmgren 152 214 Ishihara 149 213 Jennings 151 214 Stillings 150 213 Vestibular 56-60 52 Tinnitus aurium 64 83 Tongue 4 5 Tonsils 6 6 Transposition of refraction formula 170 261 Treatment ot aero-otitis media 35 26 Trichromic color perception 141 187 Tympanic membrane 22 14 Vertical deviations 103 126 Vertigo, vestibular 46-55 45 After douching 47 47 Definitions 46 45 Falling 52 51 After douching 54 52 After turning 53 51 Past-pointing 49 47 After douching 50 49 Cerebral function 51 50 Vestibular- Branch, auditory nerve 40 35 Nystagmus 44, 45 41, 43 Definition 44 41 Tests 56—60 52 Caloric douche 60 54 Falling 59 54 Nystagmus 57 53 Pointing 58 53 Vertigo 46—55 45 Vestibule, physiology 42 37 Vision: Color 130-155 175 Dichromic 142 189 Near 126 159 Peripheral 156 221 Visual acuity 71-79 91 Alining power of eye 75 93 Factors concerned : 71 91 Metric system in 78 95 Normal, conditions interfering with 76 93 Retina ■ 74 92 Retinal image, formation of 72 91 Snellen test letters 77 94 Readability of various letters 79 96 Visual fields- classification of defects in 161 233 Vitreous, opacities of 137 171 [A. G. 062.11 (8-24-40).] Paragraphs Pages 285 TM 8-300 INDEX By order op the Secretary of War; G. C. MARSHALL, Chief of Staff. Official : E. S. ADAMS, Major General, The Adjutant General, 286 B. S. GOVERNMENT PRINTING OFFICE: 1940 For sale by the Superintendent of Documents, Washington, D. C. Price 35 cents