from Science, N. S., Vol. V., No. 119, Pages 570-573, April 9, 1897. ] SCIENCE Editorial Committee : S. Newcomb, Mathematics; R. S. Woodward, Mechanics; E. C. Pickering Astronomy; T. C. Mendenhall, Physics; R. H. Thurston, Engineering; Ira Remsen, Chemistry; J. Le Conte, Geology; W. M. Davis, Physiography; O. C. Marsh, Paleontology; W. K. Brooks, C. Hart Merriam, Zoology; S. H. Scudder, Entomology; N. L. Britton, Botany; Henry F. Osborn, General Biology; H. P. Bowditch, Physiology; J. S. Billings, Hygiene ; J. McKeen Cattell, Psychology ; Daniel G. Brinton, J. W. Powell, Anthropology. THE GRO WTH OF CHILDREN. In the years 1891 and 1892 I collected statistics on the growth of children in Wor- cester, Mass., mainly with a view to investi- gating individual growth. Although it was not possible, as was my original intention, to continue the series through a number of years, some results of interest have been obtained. The measurements were taken partly by myself, partly by fellows and students of Clark University. I am in- debted to Dr. G. M. West for many of the measurements. The stature of the same children was measured in May, 1891, and in May, 1892. The average annual increases and the vari- ability of the amount of growth (the mean of the squares of individual variations) for these intervals were as follows: ing the years of adolescence. After this period it must necessarily fall. It disap- pears as soon as all the individuals have ceased growing. This increase in varia- bility must be considered due to the effect of retardation and acceleration of growth. During the period preceding pu- berty some individuals will have reached their full growth, while others are still growing at a very rapid rate. As the rate of growth during the early years of childhood does not vary very much, retardation and acceleration will not have any effect of this sort. For the same reason the distribution of amounts of growth during the years preceding puberty is very asymmetrical and must be more so for the years from 17 to 20, for which I have no observations. It will be noticed that Average Increases in Stature of Children Between the Following Years (cm.). 5 and 6 6 and 7 7 and 8 8 and 9 9 and 10 10 and 11 11 and 12 12 and 13 13 and 14 14 and 15 15 and 16 Boys 6.55 5.70 5.37 4.89 5.10 5.02 4.99 5.91 7.88 6.23 5.64 Girls 5.75 5.90 5.70 5.50 5.97 6.17 6.98 6.71 5.44 3.34 - Variability of Annual Growth. Boys ±0.68 ±0.86 ±0.96 ±1.03 ±0.88 ±1.26 ±1.86 ±2.39 ±2.91 ±3.46 Girls ±0.88 ±0.98 ±1.10 ±0.97 ±1.23 ±1.85 ±1.89 ±2.06 ±2.89 ±2.71 This table shows that young children grow more uniformly than older children. The increasing variability is very great dur- the growth of girls is more variable than that of boys. I have furthermore divided the series for 2 SCIENCE. each year in two equal halves, the one em- bracing the tall children, the other embra- cing the short children. The average an- nual increases of these two groups are as follows: L 2 - m2 1 a = _1 - 1 \ p3 where p and the variabilities of stature at the periods t and and where m the Average Increase in Stature Between the Following Years : 6 and 7 7 and 8 8 and 9 9 and 10 10 and 11 11 and 12 12 and 13 13 and 14 14 and 15 15 and 16 Short boys 5.5 5.2 4.8 4.8 4.8 4.8 5.2 7.3 7.5 6.8 Tall boys 5.9 5.5 5.0 5.4 5.3 5.2 6.6 8.5 5.0 4.4 Difference +0.4 +0.3 +0.2 +0.6 +0.5 +0.4 +0.6 +1.2 -2.5 -2.4 Short girls 5.7 5.5 5.3 5.5 5.8 7.0 7.4 6.5 4.5 Tall girls 6.1 5.9 5.6 6.4 6.5 7.0 6.0 4.4 2.2 Difference +0.4 +0.4 +0.3 +0.9 +0.7 0.0 -1.4 -2.1 -2.3 It appears that during the early years of childhood short children grow more slowly than tall children, that is to say their gen- eral development continues to be slow. Later on, during the period of adolescence, they continue to grow while tall children have more nearly reached their full de- velopment. That is to say, small children are throughout their period of growth re- tarded in development, and smallness at any given period as compared to the aver- age must in most cases be interpreted as due to slowness of development. During early life slowness of development which has manifested itself is likely to continue, while some of the effects of retardation will be made good during the period of adolescence, which is liable to be longer than in children who develop rapidly in early life. We will call the average stature at the age t A(; the amount of growth of an indi- vidual whose stature at that period is At 4- x may be called dx. We assume that the re- lation between the actual size of an indi- vidual and the average amount of its annual growth be expressed by the simple relation dx - d + ax where a is a constant. Furthermore we will assume that the vari- ability of dx will be the same for all values of x. Then it can easily be proved that variability of the amount of growth during the period t-t. From these data the following values of a have been computed: Age. Boys. Girls. 6 0.05 0.05 7 0.05 0.05 8 0.01 0.01 9 0.03 0.03 10 0.06 0.06 11 0.06 0.07 12 0.10 - 0.11 13 0.08 - 0.17 14 - 0.03 - 0.20 15 - 0.22 The values cannot claim any great weight, since the series of observations is very small. Only about fifty individuals for each year and sex are available. They prove, however, that the values of a first decrease until about the eighth year. Then they increase and decrease again very rapidly after the thirteenth year in boys and after the eleventh year in girls. According to the assumptions made be- fore, the average individual which meas- ured A + x at the period t will measure (A + x) + (<Z + ax) = A + c? + a; -1 il---2 P P \ at the period tv If it measured A + J + p SCIENCE. 3 it would remain in the same percentile grade while according to the above formula its percentile standing will be nearer the aver- age than at the initial period t. Only when all the children of the initial measurement A -f- x grow equally, i. e., if m = 0 could they remain in the same percentile grade. This conclusion agrees with Dr. Henry G. Beyer's observations.* The above approximation is fairly satis- factory during the early years of childhood. During the period of adolescence it is not satisfactory, because the values of a are too large. More extended observations will enable us to include terms of higher order in the considerations and to obtain more accurate knowledge of the laws of growth. The results of this investigation suggest that the differences of growth observed in children of different nationalities and of parents of different occupations may also be partly due to retardation or acceleration of growth, partly to differences of develop- ment in the adult stage. In order to decide this question we may assume that in the averages obtained for all the series representing various social groups accidental deviation from the general aver- age only occurred. Then it is possible to calculate the average deviation which would result under these conditions. When the actual differences that have been found by observation are taken into consideration another average deviation results. If the latter nearly equals the former, then the constant causes that affect each social group are few and of slight importance. If it is much larger than the former, then the causes are many and powerful. The pro- portion between the theoretical value of the deviation and the one obtained by observa- tion is therefore a measure of the number and value of the causes influencing each series. I have applied these considerations to the * Proc. U. S. Naval Institute, Vol. XXI., No. 2. measurements of Boston school children ob- tained by Dr. H. P. Bowditch. I have used thirteen different classes in my calculations, namely, five nationalities : American, Irish, American and Irish mixed, German and English ; and eight classes grouped accord- ing to nationalities and occupations: Amer- ican professional, mercantile, skilled labor and unskilled labor, and the same classes among the Irish. The results are as follows : Boys. Deviation. Girls. Deviation. Age. Theory. Obs. Ratio. Theory. Obs. Ratio. 5 0.34 0.34 1.0 0.40 0.58 1.5 6 0.28 0.46 1.6 0.34 0.57 1.8 7 0.29 0.76 2.6 0.32 0.81 2.5 8 0.28 0.54 1.9 0.30 0.71 2.4 9 0.32 0.89 2.7 0.36 0.40 1.1 10 0.33 0.76 2.4 0.38 0.83 2.1 11 0.35 1.05 3.0 0.46 1.04 2.2 12 0.40 1.18 3.0 0.52 1.89 3.6 13 0.46 1.65 3.6 0.52 1.44 2.8 14 0.57 2.69 44 0.53 0.98 1.9 15 0.67 2.06 2.9 0.53 1.02 1.9 16 0.72 1.50 2.1 0.54 0.53 1.0 We see that the values obtained by actual observation are always greater than those obtained under the assumption that only accidental causes influence the averages for each class. We also see that these causes reach a maximum during the period of growth and decrease as the adult stage is reached. The maximum is found in the fourteenth year in the case of boys, in the twelfth year in the case of girls, i. e., in those years in which the effects of accelera- tion and retardation of growth are strongest. Although the values given here cannot claim any very great weight on account of the small number of classes, this phenom- enon is brought out most clearly. The figures prove, therefore, that the differences in development between various social classes are, to a great extent, results of acceleration and retardation of growth which act in such a way that the social 4 SCIENCE. groups which show higher values of meas- urements do so on account of accelerated growth, and that they cease to grow earlier than those whose growth is in the beginning less rapid, so that there is a tendency to decreasing differences between these groups during the last years of growth. Franz Boas.